메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 80, Issue 5, 2016, Pages 860-876

Proactive control of irrelevant task rules during cued task switching

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords

ADOLESCENT; ASSOCIATION; ATTENTION; EXECUTIVE FUNCTION; FEMALE; HUMAN; MALE; MOTIVATION; PHOTOSTIMULATION; PHYSIOLOGY; PROCEDURES; PSYCHOMOTOR PERFORMANCE; TASK PERFORMANCE; YOUNG ADULT;

EID: 84937844861     PISSN: 03400727     EISSN: 14302772     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1007/s00426-015-0686-5     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (26)

References (77)
  • 1
    • 0001488325 scopus 로고
    • Switching intentional set: exploring the dynamic control of tasks
    • Umilta C, Moscovitch M, (eds), Erlbaum, Hillsdale
    • Allport, D. A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Switching intentional set: exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance, XV (pp. 421–452). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
    • (1994) Attention and performance, XV , pp. 421-452
    • Allport, D.A.1    Styles, E.A.2    Hsieh, S.3
  • 2
    • 84981157288 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • You can’t have it both ways: An examination of congruency effects in task switching. Unpublished Dissertation
    • Ann Arbor: MI
    • Askren, M. K. A. (2010). You can’t have it both ways: An examination of congruency effects in task switching. Unpublished Dissertation, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI.
    • (2010) University of Michigan
    • Askren, M.K.A.1
  • 3
    • 29544432364 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Recommended effect size statistics for repeated measures designs
    • PID: 16405133
    • Bakeman, R. (2005). Recommended effect size statistics for repeated measures designs. Behavior Research Methods, 37, 379–384.
    • (2005) Behavior Research Methods , vol.37 , pp. 379-384
    • Bakeman, R.1
  • 4
    • 78649462798 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Behavioral and neural evidence for item-specific performance monitoring
    • PID: 19925177
    • Blais, C., & Bunge, S. (2010). Behavioral and neural evidence for item-specific performance monitoring. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 2758–2767.
    • (2010) Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , vol.22 , pp. 2758-2767
    • Blais, C.1    Bunge, S.2
  • 6
    • 36049005270 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Item-specific adaptation and the conflict monitoring hypothesis: a computational model
    • PID: 17907873
    • Blais, C., Robidoux, S., Risko, E. F., & Besner, D. (2007). Item-specific adaptation and the conflict monitoring hypothesis: a computational model. Psychological Review, 114, 1076–1086.
    • (2007) Psychological Review , vol.114 , pp. 1076-1086
    • Blais, C.1    Robidoux, S.2    Risko, E.F.3    Besner, D.4
  • 8
    • 34547368131 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Explaining the many varieties of working memory variation: dual mechanisms of cognitive control
    • Conway ARA, Jarrold C, Kane MJ, Miyake A, Towse JN, (eds), Oxford University Press, Oxford
    • Braver, T. S., Gray, J. R., & Burgess, G. C. (2007). Explaining the many varieties of working memory variation: dual mechanisms of cognitive control. In A. R. A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. J. Kane, A. Miyake, & J. N. Towse (Eds.), Variation in working memory (pp. 76–106). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    • (2007) Variation in working memory , pp. 76-106
    • Braver, T.S.1    Gray, J.R.2    Burgess, G.C.3
  • 9
    • 84897224302 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Conflict control in task conflict and response conflict
    • PID: 24696133
    • Braverman, A., & Meiran, N. (2015). Conflict control in task conflict and response conflict. Psychological Research, 79, 238–248.
    • (2015) Psychological Research , vol.79 , pp. 238-248
    • Braverman, A.1    Meiran, N.2
  • 10
    • 84866975991 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Dissociating levels of cognitive control: the case of Stroop interference
    • Bugg, J. M. (2012). Dissociating levels of cognitive control: the case of Stroop interference. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 302–309. doi:10.1177/0963721412453586.
    • (2012) Current Directions in Psychological Science , vol.21 , pp. 302-309
    • Bugg, J.M.1
  • 11
    • 84894454997 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Conflict-triggered top-down control: default mode, last resort, or no such thing?
    • PID: 24274385
    • Bugg, J. M. (2014). Conflict-triggered top-down control: default mode, last resort, or no such thing? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 567–587. doi:10.1037/a0035032.
    • (2014) Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition , vol.40 , pp. 567-587
    • Bugg, J.M.1
  • 12
    • 80053128574 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • List-wide control is not entirely elusive: evidence from picture-word Stroop
    • PID: 21638107
    • Bugg, J. M., & Chanani, S. (2011). List-wide control is not entirely elusive: evidence from picture-word Stroop. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18, 930–936. doi:10.3758/s13423-011-0112-y.
    • (2011) Psychonomic Bulletin and Review , vol.18 , pp. 930-936
    • Bugg, J.M.1    Chanani, S.2
  • 13
    • 84867059493 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • In support of a distinction between voluntary and stimulus-driven control: a review of the literature on proportion congruent effects
    • Bugg, J. M., & Crump, M. J. C. (2012). In support of a distinction between voluntary and stimulus-driven control: a review of the literature on proportion congruent effects. Frontiers in Psychology: Cognition, 3, 1–16. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00367.
    • (2012) Frontiers in Psychology: Cognition , vol.3 , pp. 1-16
    • Bugg, J.M.1    Crump, M.J.C.2
  • 14
    • 84941189906 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Expectations and experience: dissociable bases for cognitive control?
    • Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory, and Cognition
    • Bugg, J. M., Diede, N. T., Cohen-Shikora, E. R., & Szelmecy, D. (2015). Expectations and experience: dissociable bases for cognitive control? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. doi:10.1037/xlm0000106.
    • (2015)
    • Bugg, J.M.1    Diede, N.T.2    Cohen-Shikora, E.R.3    Szelmecy, D.4
  • 15
  • 16
    • 79958738516 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Why it is too early to lose control in accounts of item-specific proportion congruency effects
    • PID: 20718569
    • Bugg, J. M., Jacoby, L. L., & Chanani, S. (2011a). Why it is too early to lose control in accounts of item-specific proportion congruency effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 844–859. doi:10.1037/a0019957.
    • (2011) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.37 , pp. 844-859
    • Bugg, J.M.1    Jacoby, L.L.2    Chanani, S.3
  • 17
    • 65849333847 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Multiple levels of control in the Stroop task
    • PID: 19015507
    • Bugg, J. M., Jacoby, L. L., & Toth, J. (2008). Multiple levels of control in the Stroop task. Memory and Cognition, 36, 1484–1494. doi:10.3758/MC.36.8.1484.
    • (2008) Memory and Cognition , vol.36 , pp. 1484-1494
    • Bugg, J.M.1    Jacoby, L.L.2    Toth, J.3
  • 19
    • 84874710798 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Temporal dynamics of motivation-cognitive control interactions revealed by high-resolution pupillometry
    • PID: 23372557
    • Chiew, K. S., & Braver, T. S. (2013). Temporal dynamics of motivation-cognitive control interactions revealed by high-resolution pupillometry. Frontiers in Psychology,. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00015.
    • (2013) Frontiers in Psychology
    • Chiew, K.S.1    Braver, T.S.2
  • 20
    • 52849125477 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • An intention–activation account of residual switch costs
    • Monsell S, Driver J, (eds), MIT Press, Cambridge, MA
    • De Jong, R. (2000). An intention–activation account of residual switch costs. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 357–376). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    • (2000) Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII , pp. 357-376
    • De Jong, R.1
  • 21
    • 33646094192 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions
    • De Pisapia, N., & Braver, T. S. (2006). A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions. Neurocomputing, 69, 1322–1326.
    • (2006) Neurocomputing , vol.69 , pp. 1322-1326
    • De Pisapia, N.1    Braver, T.S.2
  • 22
    • 0034489305 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The Stroop effect: It is not the robust phenomenon that you have thought it to be
    • PID: 11219971
    • Dishon-Berkovits, M., & Algom, D. (2000). The Stroop effect: It is not the robust phenomenon that you have thought it to be. Memory and Cognition, 28, 1437–1449.
    • (2000) Memory and Cognition , vol.28 , pp. 1437-1449
    • Dishon-Berkovits, M.1    Algom, D.2
  • 24
    • 0003561875 scopus 로고
    • Chronometric investigations of task switching. Unpublished Dissertation
    • San Diego: CA
    • Fagot, C. (1994). Chronometric investigations of task switching. Unpublished Dissertation, University of California-San Diego, San Diego, CA.
    • (1994) University of California-San Diego
    • Fagot, C.1
  • 25
    • 79960990446 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • To be or not to be…included in an event file: integration and retrieval of distractors in stimulus-response episodes is influenced by perceptual grouping
    • PID: 21707218
    • Frings, C., & Rothermund, K. (2011). To be or not to be…included in an event file: integration and retrieval of distractors in stimulus-response episodes is influenced by perceptual grouping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1209–1227.
    • (2011) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , vol.37 , pp. 1209-1227
    • Frings, C.1    Rothermund, K.2
  • 26
    • 33845993191 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Intentional reconfiguration and involuntary persistence in task-set switching
    • Monsell S, Driver J, (eds), MIT Press, Cambridge
    • Goschke, T. (2000). Intentional reconfiguration and involuntary persistence in task-set switching. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII: control of cognitive processes (pp. 331–355). Cambridge: MIT Press.
    • (2000) Attention and performance XVIII: control of cognitive processes , pp. 331-355
    • Goschke, T.1
  • 27
    • 0027085646 scopus 로고
    • Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation and responses
    • Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation and responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 480–506.
    • (1992) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , vol.121 , pp. 480-506
    • Gratton, G.1    Coles, M.G.H.2    Donchin, E.3
  • 28
    • 84905011963 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The speed-accuracy tradeoff: history, physiology, methodology, and behavior
    • PID: 24966810
    • Heitz, R. P. (2014). The speed-accuracy tradeoff: history, physiology, methodology, and behavior. Frontiers in Neuroscience,. doi:10.3389/fnins.2014.00150.
    • (2014) Frontiers in Neuroscience
    • Heitz, R.P.1
  • 29
    • 0028250397 scopus 로고
    • Spontaneous decay of response-code activation
    • PID: 8090861
    • Hommel, B. (1994). Spontaneous decay of response-code activation. Psychological Research, 56, 261–268.
    • (1994) Psychological Research , vol.56 , pp. 261-268
    • Hommel, B.1
  • 30
    • 79960340151 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The interactive effects of listwide control, item-based control, and working memory capacity on Stroop performance
    • PID: 21517220
    • Hutchison, K. A. (2011). The interactive effects of listwide control, item-based control, and working memory capacity on Stroop performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 851–860.
    • (2011) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , vol.37 , pp. 851-860
    • Hutchison, K.A.1
  • 31
    • 0001870456 scopus 로고
    • Mental set and switch. Archives of Psychology
    • Jersild, A. T. (1927). Mental set and switch. Archives of Psychology, Whole No. 89.
    • (1927) Whole No , pp. 89
    • Jersild, A.T.1
  • 32
    • 77952874657 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Age-related shifts in brain activity dynamics during task switching
    • PID: 19805420
    • Jimura, K., & Braver, T. S. (2010). Age-related shifts in brain activity dynamics during task switching. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 1420–1431. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp206.
    • (2010) Cerebral Cortex , vol.20 , pp. 1420-1431
    • Jimura, K.1    Braver, T.S.2
  • 33
    • 77952702294 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Prefontal cortex mediation of cognitive enhancement in rewarding motivational contexts
    • Jimura, K., Locke, H. S., & Braver, T. S. (2010). Prefontal cortex mediation of cognitive enhancement in rewarding motivational contexts. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 8871–8876.
    • (2010) Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , vol.107 , pp. 8871-8876
    • Jimura, K.1    Locke, H.S.2    Braver, T.S.3
  • 34
    • 77952424019 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The reaction time task-rule congruency effect is not affected by working memory load: further support for the activated long-term memory hypothesis
    • PID: 19876645
    • Kessler, Y., & Meiran, N. (2010). The reaction time task-rule congruency effect is not affected by working memory load: further support for the activated long-term memory hypothesis. Psychological Research, 74, 388–399.
    • (2010) Psychological Research , vol.74 , pp. 388-399
    • Kessler, Y.1    Meiran, N.2
  • 36
    • 33846964556 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Task switching: on the origins of response congruency effects
    • PID: 16133574
    • Kiesel, A., Wendt, M., & Peters, A. (2007). Task switching: on the origins of response congruency effects. Psychological Research, 71, 117–125.
    • (2007) Psychological Research , vol.71 , pp. 117-125
    • Kiesel, A.1    Wendt, M.2    Peters, A.3
  • 39
    • 0019070627 scopus 로고
    • Attention and automaticity in Stroop and priming tasks: theory and data
    • PID: 7418368
    • Logan, G. D. (1980). Attention and automaticity in Stroop and priming tasks: theory and data. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 523–553.
    • (1980) Cognitive Psychology , vol.12 , pp. 523-553
    • Logan, G.D.1
  • 40
    • 0001249031 scopus 로고
    • When it helps to be misled: facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop –like task
    • Logan, G. D., & Zbrodoff, N. J. (1979). When it helps to be misled: facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop –like task. Memory and Cognition, 7, 166–174.
    • (1979) Memory and Cognition , vol.7 , pp. 166-174
    • Logan, G.D.1    Zbrodoff, N.J.2
  • 42
    • 0020394267 scopus 로고
    • Selective and divided attention in a Stroop task
    • PID: 7159848
    • Lowe, D., & Mitterer, J. O. (1982). Selective and divided attention in a Stroop task. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 36, 684–700.
    • (1982) Canadian Journal of Psychology , vol.36 , pp. 684-700
    • Lowe, D.1    Mitterer, J.O.2
  • 43
    • 0026132381 scopus 로고
    • Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review
    • PID: 2034749
    • MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163–203.
    • (1991) Psychological Bulletin , vol.109 , pp. 163-203
    • MacLeod, C.M.1
  • 46
    • 0033655957 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Modeling cognitive control in task-switching
    • PID: 11004878
    • Meiran, N. (2000). Modeling cognitive control in task-switching. Psychological Research, 63, 234–249.
    • (2000) Psychological Research , vol.63 , pp. 234-249
    • Meiran, N.1
  • 50
    • 46349097236 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Control by action representation and input selection (CARIS): a theoretical framework for task switching
    • PID: 18350316
    • Meiran, N., Kessler, Y., & Adi-Japha, E. (2008). Control by action representation and input selection (CARIS): a theoretical framework for task switching. Psychological Research, 72, 473–500.
    • (2008) Psychological Research , vol.72 , pp. 473-500
    • Meiran, N.1    Kessler, Y.2    Adi-Japha, E.3
  • 51
    • 0042264976 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Driven by information: a tectonic theory of Stroop effects
    • PID: 12885110
    • Melara, R. D., & Algom, D. (2003). Driven by information: a tectonic theory of Stroop effects. Psychological Review, 110, 422–471.
    • (2003) Psychological Review , vol.110 , pp. 422-471
    • Melara, R.D.1    Algom, D.2
  • 53
    • 0038247953 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Task-set reconfiguration with predictable and unpredictable task switches
    • PID: 12795475
    • Monsell, S., Sumner, P., & Waters, H. (2003). Task-set reconfiguration with predictable and unpredictable task switches. Memory and Cognition, 31, 327–342.
    • (2003) Memory and Cognition , vol.31 , pp. 327-342
    • Monsell, S.1    Sumner, P.2    Waters, H.3
  • 54
    • 0036513961 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Residual costs in task switching: testing the failure-to- engage hypothesis
    • PID: 12026956
    • Nieuwenhuis, S., & Monsell, S. (2002). Residual costs in task switching: testing the failure-to- engage hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 86–92.
    • (2002) Psychonomic Bulletin and Review , vol.9 , pp. 86-92
    • Nieuwenhuis, S.1    Monsell, S.2
  • 55
    • 0348090638 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Generalized eta and omega squared statistics: measures of effect size for some common research designs
    • PID: 14664681
    • Olejnik, S., & Algina, J. (2003). Generalized eta and omega squared statistics: measures of effect size for some common research designs. Psychological Methods, 8, 434–447.
    • (2003) Psychological Methods , vol.8 , pp. 434-447
    • Olejnik, S.1    Algina, J.2
  • 56
    • 21844510093 scopus 로고
    • The cost of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks
    • Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). The cost of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231.
    • (1995) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , vol.124 , pp. 207-231
    • Rogers, R.D.1    Monsell, S.2
  • 58
    • 84870868648 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The many faces of preparatory control in task switching: reviewing a decade of fMRI research
    • PID: 21998090
    • Ruge, H., Jamadar, S., Zimmerman, U., & Karayanidis, F. (2013). The many faces of preparatory control in task switching: reviewing a decade of fMRI research. Human Brain Mapping, 34, 12–35.
    • (2013) Human Brain Mapping , vol.34 , pp. 12-35
    • Ruge, H.1    Jamadar, S.2    Zimmerman, U.3    Karayanidis, F.4
  • 59
    • 84896734081 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Temporal learning and list-level proportion congruency: conflict adaptation or learning when to respond?
    • Schmidt, J. R. (2013). Temporal learning and list-level proportion congruency: conflict adaptation or learning when to respond? PLoS One,. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.008232012.
    • (2013) PLoS One
    • Schmidt, J.R.1
  • 60
    • 84899432475 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • List-level transfer effects in temporal learning: further complications for the list-level proportion congruent effect
    • Schmidt, J. R. (2014). List-level transfer effects in temporal learning: further complications for the list-level proportion congruent effect. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26, 373–385.
    • (2014) Journal of Cognitive Psychology , vol.26 , pp. 373-385
    • Schmidt, J.R.1
  • 61
    • 43549089739 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The Stroop effect: Why proportion congruence has nothing to do with congruency and everything to do with contingency
    • PID: 18444752
    • Schmidt, J. R., & Besner, D. (2008). The Stroop effect: Why proportion congruence has nothing to do with congruency and everything to do with contingency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 514–523.
    • (2008) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , vol.34 , pp. 514-523
    • Schmidt, J.R.1    Besner, D.2
  • 62
    • 84925822330 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Isolating a mediated route for response congruency effects in task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory
    • Cognition: Advance online publication
    • Schneider, D. W. (2014). Isolating a mediated route for response congruency effects in task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, Advance online publication. doi:10.1037/xlm0000049.
    • (2014)
    • Schneider, D.W.1
  • 63
    • 24944583968 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Modeling task switching without switching tasks: a short-term priming account of explicitly cued performance
    • Schneider, D. W., & Logan, G. D. (2005). Modeling task switching without switching tasks: a short-term priming account of explicitly cued performance. Psychology: General, 134, 343––367.
    • (2005) Psychology: General , vol.134 , pp. 343-367
    • Schneider, D.W.1    Logan, G.D.2
  • 66
    • 57049145197 scopus 로고
    • Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions
    • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.
    • (1935) Journal of Experimental Psychology , vol.18 , pp. 643-662
    • Stroop, J.R.1
  • 68
    • 0029461810 scopus 로고
    • Dissociation of processes underlying spatial SR compatibility: evidence for the independent influence of what and where
    • PID: 8750420
    • Toth, J. P., Levine, B., Stuss, D. T., Oh, A., Winocur, G., & Meiran, N. (1995). Dissociation of processes underlying spatial SR compatibility: evidence for the independent influence of what and where. Consciousness and Cognition, 4(4), 483–501.
    • (1995) Consciousness and Cognition , vol.4 , Issue.4 , pp. 483-501
    • Toth, J.P.1    Levine, B.2    Stuss, D.T.3    Oh, A.4    Winocur, G.5    Meiran, N.6
  • 69
    • 0031160050 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Specifying the relations between automaticity and consciousness: a theoretical note
    • Tzelgov, J. (1997). Specifying the relations between automaticity and consciousness: a theoretical note. Consciousness and Cognition, 6, 441–451.
    • (1997) Consciousness and Cognition , vol.6 , pp. 441-451
    • Tzelgov, J.1
  • 70
    • 77954171061 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Task switching: interplay of reconfiguration and interference control
    • PID: 20565170
    • Vandierendonck, A., Liefooghe, B., & Verbruggen, F. (2010). Task switching: interplay of reconfiguration and interference control. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 601–626.
    • (2010) Psychological Bulletin , vol.136 , pp. 601-626
    • Vandierendonck, A.1    Liefooghe, B.2    Verbruggen, F.3
  • 71
    • 43449117482 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Hebbian learning of cognitive control: dealing with specific and nonspecific adaptation
    • PID: 18426302
    • Verguts, T., & Notebaert, W. (2008). Hebbian learning of cognitive control: dealing with specific and nonspecific adaptation. Psychological Review, 115, 518–525.
    • (2008) Psychological Review , vol.115 , pp. 518-525
    • Verguts, T.1    Notebaert, W.2
  • 72
    • 0038457034 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Task-switching and long term priming: role of episodic stimulus-task bindings in task-shift costs
    • PID: 12809680
    • Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, A. (2003). Task-switching and long term priming: role of episodic stimulus-task bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 361–413.
    • (2003) Cognitive Psychology , vol.46 , pp. 361-413
    • Waszak, F.1    Hommel, B.2    Allport, A.3
  • 73
    • 84881546867 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Top-down versus bottom-up: when instructions overcome automatic retrieval
    • PID: 23064521
    • Waszak, F., Pfister, R., & Kiesel, A. (2013). Top-down versus bottom-up: when instructions overcome automatic retrieval. Psychological Research, 77, 611–617.
    • (2013) Psychological Research , vol.77 , pp. 611-617
    • Waszak, F.1    Pfister, R.2    Kiesel, A.3
  • 74
    • 43649093830 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The impact of stimulus-specific practice and task instructions on response congruency effects between tasks
    • PID: 17546462
    • Wendt, M., & Kiesel, A. (2008). The impact of stimulus-specific practice and task instructions on response congruency effects between tasks. Psychological Research, 72, 425–432.
    • (2008) Psychological Research , vol.72 , pp. 425-432
    • Wendt, M.1    Kiesel, A.2
  • 75
    • 64249162492 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Conflict-frequency affects flanker-interference
    • PID: 19289363
    • Wendt, M., & Luna-Rodriguez, A. (2009). Conflict-frequency affects flanker-interference. Experimental Psychology, 56, 206–217.
    • (2009) Experimental Psychology , vol.56 , pp. 206-217
    • Wendt, M.1    Luna-Rodriguez, A.2
  • 76
    • 0031862903 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Effect of increased response dominance and contextual disintegration on the Stroop interference effect in older adults
    • PID: 9640582
    • West, R., & Baylis, G. C. (1998). Effect of increased response dominance and contextual disintegration on the Stroop interference effect in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 13, 206–217.
    • (1998) Psychology and Aging , vol.13 , pp. 206-217
    • West, R.1    Baylis, G.C.2
  • 77
    • 79955778240 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Automaticity without extensive training: the role of memory retrieval in implementation of task-defined rules
    • Yamaguchi, M., & Proctor, R. W. (2011). Automaticity without extensive training: the role of memory retrieval in implementation of task-defined rules. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 347–354.
    • (2011) Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , vol.18 , pp. 347-354
    • Yamaguchi, M.1    Proctor, R.W.2


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.