메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 136, Issue 1, 2007, Pages 43-63

Modes of executive control in sequence learning: From stimulus-based to plan-based control

Author keywords

Event coding theory; Executive control; Implicit and explicit knowledge; Individual differences; Sequential learning

Indexed keywords

ARTICLE; ASSOCIATION; BEHAVIOR; COGNITION; HUMAN; LEARNING; PHOTOSTIMULATION; PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY; REACTION TIME; VERBAL BEHAVIOR;

EID: 33847668719     PISSN: 00963445     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.136.1.43     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (60)

References (98)
  • 2
    • 0030638716 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD
    • Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65-94.
    • (1997) Psychological Bulletin , vol.121 , pp. 65-94
    • Barkley, R.A.1
  • 3
    • 0011896166 scopus 로고
    • Serial choice reaction-time as a function of response versus signal-and-response repetition
    • April 10
    • Bertelson, P. (1965, April 10). Serial choice reaction-time as a function of response versus signal-and-response repetition. Nature, 206, 217-218.
    • (1965) Nature , vol.206 , pp. 217-218
    • Bertelson, P.1
  • 4
    • 27744487355 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • On the role of verbalization during task selection: Switching or serial order control?
    • Bryck, R. L., & Mayr, U. (2005). On the role of verbalization during task selection: Switching or serial order control? Memory & Cognition, 33, 611-623.
    • (2005) Memory & Cognition , vol.33 , pp. 611-623
    • Bryck, R.L.1    Mayr, U.2
  • 5
    • 0013167953 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Memory for serial order: A network model of the phonological loop and its timing
    • Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. J. (1999). Memory for serial order: A network model of the phonological loop and its timing. Psychological Review, 106, 551-581.
    • (1999) Psychological Review , vol.106 , pp. 551-581
    • Burgess, N.1    Hitch, G.J.2
  • 10
    • 0029927872 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Do executed and imagined movements share the same central structures?
    • Decety, J. (1996a). Do executed and imagined movements share the same central structures? Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 87-93.
    • (1996) Cognitive Brain Research , vol.3 , pp. 87-93
    • Decety, J.1
  • 11
    • 0030462547 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Neural representations for action
    • Decety, J. (1996b). Neural representations for action. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 7, 285-297.
    • (1996) Reviews in the Neurosciences , vol.7 , pp. 285-297
    • Decety, J.1
  • 12
    • 0035381075 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Can sequence learning be implicit? New evidence with the process dissociation procedure
    • Destrebecqz, A., & Cleermans, A. (2001). Can sequence learning be implicit? New evidence with the process dissociation procedure. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 343-350.
    • (2001) Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , vol.8 , pp. 343-350
    • Destrebecqz, A.1    Cleermans, A.2
  • 13
    • 0842346900 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Development of the acquisition and control of action-effect associations
    • Eenshuistra, R. M., Weidema, M. A., & Hommel, B. (2004). Development of the acquisition and control of action-effect associations. Acta Psychologia, 115, 185-209.
    • (2004) Acta Psychologia , vol.115 , pp. 185-209
    • Eenshuistra, R.M.1    Weidema, M.A.2    Hommel, B.3
  • 14
    • 0035161840 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Human brain activation accompanying explicitly directed movement sequence learning
    • Eliassen, J. C., Souza, T., & Sanes, J. N. (2001). Human brain activation accompanying explicitly directed movement sequence learning. Experimental Brain Research, 141, 269-280.
    • (2001) Experimental Brain Research , vol.141 , pp. 269-280
    • Eliassen, J.C.1    Souza, T.2    Sanes, J.N.3
  • 15
    • 0000076476 scopus 로고
    • Spelling and writing (and reading and speaking)
    • A. W. Ellis Ed, London: Academic Press
    • Ellis, A. W. (1982). Spelling and writing (and reading and speaking). In A. W. Ellis (Ed.), Normality and pathology in cognitive function (pp. 113-146). London: Academic Press.
    • (1982) Normality and pathology in cognitive function , pp. 113-146
    • Ellis, A.W.1
  • 17
    • 0037265289 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The role of inner speech in task switching: A dual-task investigation
    • Emerson, M. J., & Miyake, A. (2003). The role of inner speech in task switching: A dual-task investigation. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 148-168.
    • (2003) Journal of Memory and Language , vol.48 , pp. 148-168
    • Emerson, M.J.1    Miyake, A.2
  • 18
    • 0003147479 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Learning versus behavioral expression of the learned: The effects of a secondary tone-counting task on implicit learning in the serial reaction task
    • Frensch, P. A., Lin, J., & Buchner, A. (1998). Learning versus behavioral expression of the learned: The effects of a secondary tone-counting task on implicit learning in the serial reaction task. Psychological Research, 61, 83-98.
    • (1998) Psychological Research , vol.61 , pp. 83-98
    • Frensch, P.A.1    Lin, J.2    Buchner, A.3
  • 19
    • 0028012696 scopus 로고
    • Individual differences in short-term memory capacity on an indirect measure of serial learning
    • Frensch, P. A., & Miner, C. S. (1994). Individual differences in short-term memory capacity on an indirect measure of serial learning. Memory & Cognition, 22, 95-110.
    • (1994) Memory & Cognition , vol.22 , pp. 95-110
    • Frensch, P.A.1    Miner, C.S.2
  • 21
    • 33845993191 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Intentional reconfiguration and involuntary persistence in task set switching
    • S. Monsell & J. Driver Eds, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
    • Goschke, T. (2000). Intentional reconfiguration and involuntary persistence in task set switching. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 331-355). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    • (2000) Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII , pp. 331-355
    • Goschke, T.1
  • 23
    • 0032533324 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Abstract and effector-specific representations of motor sequences identified with PET
    • Grafton, S. T., Hazeltine, E., & Ivry, R. (1998). Abstract and effector-specific representations of motor sequences identified with PET. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 9420-9428.
    • (1998) Journal of Neuroscience , vol.18 , pp. 9420-9428
    • Grafton, S.T.1    Hazeltine, E.2    Ivry, R.3
  • 24
    • 0242483997 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The representational nature of sequence learning: Evidence for goal-based codes
    • W. Prinz & B. Hommel Eds, Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press
    • Hazeltine, E. (2002). The representational nature of sequence learning: Evidence for goal-based codes. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention and performance XIX (pp. 673-689). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
    • (2002) Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention and performance XIX , pp. 673-689
    • Hazeltine, E.1
  • 25
    • 0031048408 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Attention and stimulus characteristics determine the locus of motor-sequence encoding: A PET study
    • Hazeltine, E., Grafton, S. T., & Ivry, R. (1997). Attention and stimulus characteristics determine the locus of motor-sequence encoding: A PET study. Brain, 120, 123-140.
    • (1997) Brain , vol.120 , pp. 123-140
    • Hazeltine, E.1    Grafton, S.T.2    Ivry, R.3
  • 26
    • 0034308414 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Sequence learning in Parkinsons disease: A comparison of spatial-attention and numberresponse sequences
    • Helmuth, L. L., Mayr, U., & Daum, I. (2000). Sequence learning in Parkinson"s disease: A comparison of spatial-attention and numberresponse sequences. Neuropsychologia, 38, 1443-1451.
    • (2000) Neuropsychologia , vol.38 , pp. 1443-1451
    • Helmuth, L.L.1    Mayr, U.2    Daum, I.3
  • 27
    • 0242679449 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Selective interference with verbal short-term memory for serial order information: A new paradigm and tests of a timing-signal hypothesis
    • Hcnson, R., Hartley, T., & Burgess, N. (2003). Selective interference with verbal short-term memory for serial order information: A new paradigm and tests of a timing-signal hypothesis. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 56(A). 1307-1334.
    • (2003) Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology , vol.56 , Issue.A , pp. 1307-1334
    • Hcnson, R.1    Hartley, T.2    Burgess, N.3
  • 28
    • 0003368350 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Stimulus-response compatibility and sequential learning in the serial reaction time task
    • Hoffmann, J., & Koch, I. (1997). Stimulus-response compatibility and sequential learning in the serial reaction time task. Psychological Research, 60, 87-97.
    • (1997) Psychological Research , vol.60 , pp. 87-97
    • Hoffmann, J.1    Koch, I.2
  • 29
    • 0142155617 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Unique transitions between stimuli and responses in SRT tasks: Evidence for the primacy of response predictions
    • Hoffmann, J., Martin, C., & Schilling, A. (2003). Unique transitions between stimuli and responses in SRT tasks: Evidence for the primacy of response predictions. Psychological Research, 67, 160-173.
    • (2003) Psychological Research , vol.67 , pp. 160-173
    • Hoffmann, J.1    Martin, C.2    Schilling, A.3
  • 31
    • 33845891768 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Hommel, B. (1993). Inverting the Simon effect by intention: Determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information. Psychological Research, .5.5, 270-279.
    • Hommel, B. (1993). Inverting the Simon effect by intention: Determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information. Psychological Research, .5.5, 270-279.
  • 32
    • 0030345484 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The cognitive representation of action: Automatic integration of perceived action effects
    • Hommel, B. (1996). The cognitive representation of action: Automatic integration of perceived action effects. Psychological Research, 59, 176-186.
    • (1996) Psychological Research , vol.59 , pp. 176-186
    • Hommel, B.1
  • 33
    • 77956744698 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Toward an action-concept model of stimulus-response compatibility
    • B. Hommel & W. Prinz Eds, Amsterdam: North-Holland
    • Hommel, B. (1997). Toward an action-concept model of stimulus-response compatibility. In B. Hommel & W. Prinz (Eds.), Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility (pp. 281-320). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
    • (1997) Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility , pp. 281-320
    • Hommel, B.1
  • 34
    • 52849109674 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The prepared reflex: Automaticity and control in stimulus-response translation
    • S. Monsell & J. Driver Eds, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
    • Hommel, B. (2000). The prepared reflex: Automaticity and control in stimulus-response translation. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 247-273). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    • (2000) Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII , pp. 247-273
    • Hommel, B.1
  • 35
    • 0040077742 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Responding to object files: Automatic integration of spatial information revealed by stimulus-response compatibility effects
    • Hommel, B. (2002). Responding to object files: Automatic integration of spatial information revealed by stimulus-response compatibility effects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 55(A), 567-580.
    • (2002) Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology , vol.55 , Issue.A , pp. 567-580
    • Hommel, B.1
  • 36
    • 1542435466 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Planning and representing intentional action
    • Hommel, B. (2003). Planning and representing intentional action. TheScientificWorldJOURNAL 3, 593-608.
    • (2003) TheScientificWorldJOURNAL , vol.3 , pp. 593-608
    • Hommel, B.1
  • 37
    • 0842329091 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Exorcizing the homunculus. Phase two: Editors' introduction
    • Hommel, B., Daum, I., & Kluwe, R. H. (2004). Exorcizing the homunculus. Phase two: Editors' introduction. Acta Psychologica, 115, 99-104.
    • (2004) Acta Psychologica , vol.115 , pp. 99-104
    • Hommel, B.1    Daum, I.2    Kluwe, R.H.3
  • 38
    • 0035495020 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning
    • Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849-878.
    • (2001) Behavioral and Brain Sciences , vol.24 , pp. 849-878
    • Hommel, B.1    Müsseler, J.2    Aschersleben, G.3    Prinz, W.4
  • 39
    • 0035378830 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The dual-task SRT procedure: Fine-tuning the timing
    • Hsiao, A. T., & Reber, A. S. (2001). The dual-task SRT procedure: Fine-tuning the timing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 336-342.
    • (2001) Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , vol.8 , pp. 336-342
    • Hsiao, A.T.1    Reber, A.S.2
  • 40
    • 84971193983 scopus 로고
    • Are there independent lexical and nonlexical routes in word processing? An evaluation of the dual-route theory of reading
    • Humphreys, G. W., & Evett, L. J. (1985). Are there independent lexical and nonlexical routes in word processing? An evaluation of the dual-route theory of reading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, 689-740.
    • (1985) Behavioral and Brain Sciences , vol.8 , pp. 689-740
    • Humphreys, G.W.1    Evett, L.J.2
  • 41
    • 85047681639 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Statistical learning in a serial reaction time task: Access to separable statistical cues by individual learners
    • Hunt, R., & Aslin, R. N. (2001). Statistical learning in a serial reaction time task: Access to separable statistical cues by individual learners. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 658-680.
    • (2001) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , vol.130 , pp. 658-680
    • Hunt, R.1    Aslin, R.N.2
  • 46
    • 4243778160 scopus 로고
    • Irrelevant tones produce an irrelevant speech effect: Implications for phonological coding in working memory
    • Jones, D. M., & Macken, W. J. (1993). Irrelevant tones produce an irrelevant speech effect: Implications for phonological coding in working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 369-381.
    • (1993) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , vol.19 , pp. 369-381
    • Jones, D.M.1    Macken, W.J.2
  • 49
    • 33847628476 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Keele, S. W., Cohen, A., & Ivry, R. (1990). Motor programs: Concepts and issues. In M. Jeannerod (F.d.), Attention and performance Kill: Motor representation and control (pp. 77-110). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
    • Keele, S. W., Cohen, A., & Ivry, R. (1990). Motor programs: Concepts and issues. In M. Jeannerod (F.d.), Attention and performance Kill: Motor representation and control (pp. 77-110). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • 50
    • 0038618950 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation
    • Keele, S. W., Ivry, R., Mayr, U., Hazeltine, E., & Heuer, H. (2003). The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation. Psychological Review, 110, 316-339.
    • (2003) Psychological Review , vol.110 , pp. 316-339
    • Keele, S.W.1    Ivry, R.2    Mayr, U.3    Hazeltine, E.4    Heuer, H.5
  • 51
    • 0002147594 scopus 로고
    • The structure of motor programs
    • G. E. Stelmach Ed, New York: Academic Press
    • Keele, S. W., & Summers, J. J. (1976). The structure of motor programs. In G. E. Stelmach (Ed.), Motor control: Issues and trends (pp. 109-142). New York: Academic Press.
    • (1976) Motor control: Issues and trends , pp. 109-142
    • Keele, S.W.1    Summers, J.J.2
  • 52
    • 0035409510 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A Simon effect induced by induced motion and location: Evidence for a direct linkage of cognitive and motor maps
    • Kerzel, D., Hommel, B., & Bekkering, H. (2001). A Simon effect induced by induced motion and location: Evidence for a direct linkage of cognitive and motor maps. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 862-874.
    • (2001) Perception & Psychophysics , vol.63 , pp. 862-874
    • Kerzel, D.1    Hommel, B.2    Bekkering, H.3
  • 53
    • 0017018804 scopus 로고
    • Sequential effects in two-choice reaction time: Automatic facilitation or subjective expectancy?
    • Kirby, N. H. (1976). Sequential effects in two-choice reaction time: Automatic facilitation or subjective expectancy? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 567-577.
    • (1976) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.2 , pp. 567-577
    • Kirby, N.H.1
  • 55
    • 0842303736 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Age differences in executive functioning across the lifespan: The role of verbalization in task preparation
    • Kray, J., Eber, J., & Lindenberger, U. (2004). Age differences in executive functioning across the lifespan: The role of verbalization in task preparation. Acta Psychologica, 115, 143-165.
    • (2004) Acta Psychologica , vol.115 , pp. 143-165
    • Kray, J.1    Eber, J.2    Lindenberger, U.3
  • 58
    • 0028724958 scopus 로고
    • Modality-specific differences in the processing of spatially, temporally, and spatiotemporally distributed information
    • Mahar, D., Mackenzie, B., & McNicol, D. (1994). Modality-specific differences in the processing of spatially, temporally, and spatiotemporally distributed information. Perception, 23, 1369-1386.
    • (1994) Perception , vol.23 , pp. 1369-1386
    • Mahar, D.1    Mackenzie, B.2    McNicol, D.3
  • 60
    • 0030093919 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Spatial attention and implicit sequence learning: Evidence for independent learning of spatial and nonspatial sequences
    • Mayr, U. (1996). Spatial attention and implicit sequence learning: Evidence for independent learning of spatial and nonspatial sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 350-364.
    • (1996) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , vol.22 , pp. 350-364
    • Mayr, U.1
  • 61
    • 0021117844 scopus 로고
    • The social basis of intentional action
    • Meacham, J. A. (1984). The social basis of intentional action. Human Development, 27, 119-123.
    • (1984) Human Development , vol.27 , pp. 119-123
    • Meacham, J.A.1
  • 62
    • 11344272210 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Role of rhythms in facilitating short-term memory
    • January 6
    • Mehta, M. R. (2005, January 6). Role of rhythms in facilitating short-term memory. Neuron, 45, 7-9.
    • (2005) Neuron , vol.45 , pp. 7-9
    • Mehta, M.R.1
  • 65
    • 0842329085 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Inner speech as a retrieval aid for task goals: The effects of cue type and articulatory suppression in the random task cuing paradigm
    • Miyake, A., Emerson, M. J., Padilla, F., & Ahn, J. (2004). Inner speech as a retrieval aid for task goals: The effects of cue type and articulatory suppression in the random task cuing paradigm. Acta Psychologica, 115, 123-142.
    • (2004) Acta Psychologica , vol.115 , pp. 123-142
    • Miyake, A.1    Emerson, M.J.2    Padilla, F.3    Ahn, J.4
  • 68
    • 0001847553 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Stimulus and response anticipation in a serial reaction task
    • Nattkemper, D., & Prinz, W. (1997). Stimulus and response anticipation in a serial reaction task. Psychological Research, 60, 98-112.
    • (1997) Psychological Research , vol.60 , pp. 98-112
    • Nattkemper, D.1    Prinz, W.2
  • 69
    • 0347123003 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Learning at distance: I. Statistical learning of non-adjacent dependencies
    • Newport, E., & Aslin, R. (2004). Learning at distance: I. Statistical learning of non-adjacent dependencies. Cognitive Psychology, 48, 127-162.
    • (2004) Cognitive Psychology , vol.48 , pp. 127-162
    • Newport, E.1    Aslin, R.2
  • 70
    • 0003109533 scopus 로고
    • Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures
    • Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1-32.
    • (1987) Cognitive Psychology , vol.19 , pp. 1-32
    • Nissen, M.J.1    Bullemer, P.2
  • 71
    • 0035224842 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Sequential analysis of a Simon task: Evidence for an attention-shift account
    • Notebaert, W., Soetens, E., & Melis, A. (2001). Sequential analysis of a Simon task: Evidence for an attention-shift account. Psychological. Research, 65, 170-184.
    • (2001) Psychological. Research , vol.65 , pp. 170-184
    • Notebaert, W.1    Soetens, E.2    Melis, A.3
  • 74
    • 85004816705 scopus 로고
    • Graphemic and semantic similarity effects in the picture-word interference task
    • Rayner, K., & Springer, C. J. (1986). Graphemic and semantic similarity effects in the picture-word interference task. British Journal of Psychology, 77, 207-222.
    • (1986) British Journal of Psychology , vol.77 , pp. 207-222
    • Rayner, K.1    Springer, C.J.2
  • 75
    • 0028535482 scopus 로고
    • Parallel brain systems for learning with and without awareness
    • Reber, P. J., & Squire, L. R. (1994). Parallel brain systems for learning with and without awareness. Learning & Memory, 1, 217-229.
    • (1994) Learning & Memory , vol.1 , pp. 217-229
    • Reber, P.J.1    Squire, L.R.2
  • 76
    • 0031920432 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Encapsulation of implicit and explicit memory in sequence learning
    • Reber, P. J., & Squire, L. R. (1998). Encapsulation of implicit and explicit memory in sequence learning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 248-263.
    • (1998) Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , vol.10 , pp. 248-263
    • Reber, P.J.1    Squire, L.R.2
  • 77
    • 0036629220 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Constraints on statistical learning
    • Saffran, J. (2002). Constraints on statistical learning. Journal of Memory and language, 47, 172-196.
    • (2002) Journal of Memory and language , vol.47 , pp. 172-196
    • Saffran, J.1
  • 78
    • 7444245509 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Irrelevant sound disrupts speech production: Exploring the relationship between short-term memory and experimentally induced slips of the tongue
    • Saito, S., & Baddeley, A. (2004). Irrelevant sound disrupts speech production: Exploring the relationship between short-term memory and experimentally induced slips of the tongue. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 57(A), 1309-1340.
    • (2004) Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology , vol.57 , Issue.A , pp. 1309-1340
    • Saito, S.1    Baddeley, A.2
  • 80
    • 0001884642 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Task integration as a factor in secondary-task effects in sequence learning
    • Schmidtke, V., & Heuer, H. (1997). Task integration as a factor in secondary-task effects in sequence learning. Psychological Research, 60, 53-71.
    • (1997) Psychological Research , vol.60 , pp. 53-71
    • Schmidtke, V.1    Heuer, H.2
  • 81
    • 0033217891 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Evaluating the relationship between explicit and implicit knowledge in a sequential reaction time task
    • Shanks, D. R., & Johnstone, T. (1999). Evaluating the relationship between explicit and implicit knowledge in a sequential reaction time task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 25, 1435-1451.
    • (1999) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition , vol.25 , pp. 1435-1451
    • Shanks, D.R.1    Johnstone, T.2
  • 83
    • 0014093453 scopus 로고
    • Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing
    • Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 300-304.
    • (1967) Journal of Applied Psychology , vol.51 , pp. 300-304
    • Simon, J.R.1    Rudell, A.P.2
  • 85
    • 2942755790 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The ideomotor principle and motor sequence acquisition: Tone effects facilitate movement chunking
    • Stocker, C., & Hoffmann, J. (2004). The ideomotor principle and motor sequence acquisition: Tone effects facilitate movement chunking. Psychological Research, 68, 126-137.
    • (2004) Psychological Research , vol.68 , pp. 126-137
    • Stocker, C.1    Hoffmann, J.2
  • 87
    • 0000585977 scopus 로고
    • The role of timing in motor program representation
    • Summers, J. J. (1975). The role of timing in motor program representation. Journal of Motor Behavior, 7, 229-241.
    • (1975) Journal of Motor Behavior , vol.7 , pp. 229-241
    • Summers, J.J.1
  • 89
    • 0031874908 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The time course of changes during motor sequence learning: A whole-brain fMRI study
    • Toni, I., Krams, M., Turner, R., & Passingham, R. E. (1998). The time course of changes during motor sequence learning: A whole-brain fMRI study. NeuroImage, 8, 50-61.
    • (1998) NeuroImage , vol.8 , pp. 50-61
    • Toni, I.1    Krams, M.2    Turner, R.3    Passingham, R.E.4
  • 90
    • 27844484322 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Statistical computations over a speech stream in a rodent
    • Toro, J. M., & Trobalon, J. (2005). Statistical computations over a speech stream in a rodent. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 867-875.
    • (2005) Perception & Psychophysics , vol.67 , pp. 867-875
    • Toro, J.M.1    Trobalon, J.2
  • 91
    • 17444423190 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Spatial interference and response control in sequence learning: 'The role of explicit knowledge
    • Tubau, E., & López-Moliner, J. (2004). Spatial interference and response control in sequence learning: 'The role of explicit knowledge. Psychological Research, 68, 55-63.
    • (2004) Psychological Research , vol.68 , pp. 55-63
    • Tubau, E.1    López-Moliner, J.2
  • 92
    • 0002651020 scopus 로고
    • Two cortical visual systems
    • D. J. Engle, M. A. Goodale, & R. J. Mansfield Eds, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
    • Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. In D. J. Engle, M. A. Goodale, & R. J. Mansfield (Eds.). Analysis of visual behavior (pp. 549-586). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    • (1982) Analysis of visual behavior , pp. 549-586
    • Ungerleider, L.G.1    Mishkin, M.2
  • 93
    • 84958916387 scopus 로고
    • A. Kozulin, Trans, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, Original work published in 1934
    • Vygotsky, L. (1986). Thought and language (A. Kozulin, Trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Original work published in 1934)
    • (1986) Thought and language
    • Vygotsky, L.1
  • 94
    • 0032116811 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning
    • Willingham, D. B. (1998). A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 105, 558-584.
    • (1998) Psychological Review , vol.105 , pp. 558-584
    • Willingham, D.B.1
  • 95
    • 0032916025 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Implicit motor sequence learning is not purely perceptual
    • Willingham, D. D. (1999). Implicit motor sequence learning is not purely perceptual. Memory & Cognition, 27, 561-572.
    • (1999) Memory & Cognition , vol.27 , pp. 561-572
    • Willingham, D.D.1
  • 97
    • 0034090066 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Implicit motor sequence learning is represented in response locations
    • Willingham, D. B., Wells, L., Farrell, J., & Stemwedel, M. (2000). Implicit motor sequence learning is represented in response locations. Memory & Cognition, 28, 366-375.
    • (2000) Memory & Cognition , vol.28 , pp. 366-375
    • Willingham, D.B.1    Wells, L.2    Farrell, J.3    Stemwedel, M.4
  • 98
    • 33847658858 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Zelazo, P. D. (1999). Language, levels of consciousness, and the development of intentional action. In P. D. Zelazo, J. W. Astington, & D. R. Olson (Eds.), Developing theories of intention: Social understanding and self-control (pp. 95-117). Mahwah, N.F: Erlbaum.
    • Zelazo, P. D. (1999). Language, levels of consciousness, and the development of intentional action. In P. D. Zelazo, J. W. Astington, & D. R. Olson (Eds.), Developing theories of intention: Social understanding and self-control (pp. 95-117). Mahwah, N.F: Erlbaum.


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.