메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 38, Issue 5, 2012, Pages 1389-1407

Implicit sequence learning based on instructed task set

Author keywords

Action control; Implicit sequence learning; Response coding

Indexed keywords

ADOLESCENT; ADULT; ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE; ARTICLE; ASSOCIATION; DECISION MAKING; FEMALE; HUMAN; LEARNING; MALE; NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL TEST; PATTERN RECOGNITION; PHYSIOLOGY; PSYCHOMOTOR PERFORMANCE; REACTION TIME;

EID: 84872676566     PISSN: 02787393     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1037/a0028071     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (31)

References (81)
  • 2
    • 68349125532 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Sensory information in perceptual-motor sequence learning: Visual and/or tactile stimuli
    • doi:10.1007/s00221-009-1903-5
    • Abrahamse, E. L., Van der Lubbe, R. H. J., & Verwey, W. B. (2009). Sensory information in perceptual-motor sequence learning: Visual and/or tactile stimuli. Experimental Brain Research, 197, 175-183. doi:10.1007/s00221-009-1903-5
    • (2009) Experimental Brain Research , vol.197 , pp. 175-183
    • Abrahamse, E.L.1    Van der Lubbe, R.H.J.2    Verwey, W.B.3
  • 5
    • 0026815614 scopus 로고
    • Visual parsing and response competition: The effect of grouping factors
    • doi:10.3758/BF03212239
    • Baylis, G. C., & Driver, J. (1992). Visual parsing and response competition: The effect of grouping factors. Perception & Psychophysics, 51, 145-162. doi:10.3758/BF03212239
    • (1992) Perception & Psychophysics , vol.51 , pp. 145-162
    • Baylis, G.C.1    Driver, J.2
  • 6
    • 67650770603 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Sequence learning in serial reaction time tasks: Integrated and independent learning of hand-related constituent sequences
    • doi:10.1037/a0015505
    • Berner, M. P., & Hoffmann, J. (2009). Sequence learning in serial reaction time tasks: Integrated and independent learning of hand-related constituent sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 890-904. doi:10.1037/a0015505
    • (2009) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , vol.35 , pp. 890-904
    • Berner, M.P.1    Hoffmann, J.2
  • 8
    • 0012159271 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Implicit learning and consciousness: A graded, dynamic perspective
    • R. M. French & A. Cleeremans (Eds.), Hove, England: Psychology Press.
    • Cleeremans, A., & Jiménez, L. (2002). Implicit learning and consciousness: A graded, dynamic perspective. In R. M. French & A. Cleeremans (Eds.), Implicit learning and consciousness (pp. 1-40). Hove, England: Psychology Press.
    • (2002) Implicit learning and consciousness , pp. 1-40
    • Cleeremans, A.1    Jiménez, L.2
  • 10
    • 0031090693 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Perceptual dimensional constraints on response selection processes
    • doi:10.1006/cogp.1997.0648
    • Cohen, A., & Shoup, R. (1997). Perceptual dimensional constraints on response selection processes. Cognitive Psychology, 32, 128-181. doi:10.1006/cogp.1997.0648
    • (1997) Cognitive Psychology , vol.32 , pp. 128-181
    • Cohen, A.1    Shoup, R.2
  • 11
  • 12
    • 0028489201 scopus 로고
    • Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence
    • doi:10.1037/0096-1523.20.4.731
    • De Jong, R., Liang, C. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731-750. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.20.4.731
    • (1994) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.20 , pp. 731-750
    • De Jong, R.1    Liang, C.C.2    Lauber, E.3
  • 13
    • 33645070583 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The role of response selection in sequence learning
    • doi:10.1080/17470210500462684
    • Deroost, N., & Soetens, E. (2006a). The role of response selection in sequence learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 449-456. doi:10.1080/17470210500462684
    • (2006) Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , vol.59 , pp. 449-456
    • Deroost, N.1    Soetens, E.2
  • 14
    • 33645292328 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Spatial processing and perceptual sequence learning in SRT tasks
    • doi:10.1027/1618-3169.53.1.16
    • Deroost, N., & Soetens, E. (2006b). Spatial processing and perceptual sequence learning in SRT tasks. Experimental Psychology, 53, 16-30. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.53.1.16
    • (2006) Experimental Psychology , vol.53 , pp. 16-30
    • Deroost, N.1    Soetens, E.2
  • 15
    • 65349157829 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • How task representations guide attention: Further evidence for the shielding function of task sets
    • doi:10.1037/a0014647
    • Dreisbach, G., & Haider, H. (2009). How task representations guide attention: Further evidence for the shielding function of task sets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 477-486. doi:10.1037/a0014647
    • (2009) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , vol.35 , pp. 477-486
    • Dreisbach, G.1    Haider, H.2
  • 16
    • 0004141817 scopus 로고
    • über das Gedächtnis [Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology] (H. A.Ruger & C.E.Bussenius Trans.). doi:10.1037/10011-000 (Original work published 1885)
    • Ebbinghaus, H. (1913). über das Gedächtnis [Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology] (H. A. Ruger & C. E. Bussenius, Trans.). doi:10.1037/10011-000 (Original work published 1885)
    • (1913)
    • Ebbinghaus, H.1
  • 18
    • 0028012696 scopus 로고
    • Effects of presentation rate and of individual differences in short-term memory capacity on an indirect measure of serial learning
    • doi:10.3758/BF03202765
    • Frensch, P. A., & Miner, C. S. (1994). Effects of presentation rate and of individual differences in short-term memory capacity on an indirect measure of serial learning. Memory & Cognition, 22, 95-110. doi:10.3758/BF03202765
    • (1994) Memory & Cognition , vol.22 , pp. 95-110
    • Frensch, P.A.1    Miner, C.S.2
  • 19
    • 34547480747 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Is information reduction an itemspecific or an item-general process?
    • doi:10.1080/00207590701396526
    • Gaschler, R., & Frensch, P. A. (2007). Is information reduction an itemspecific or an item-general process? International Journal of Psychology, 42, 218-228. doi:10.1080/00207590701396526
    • (2007) International Journal of Psychology , vol.42 , pp. 218-228
    • Gaschler, R.1    Frensch, P.A.2
  • 20
    • 0242483997 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The representational nature of sequence learning: Evidence for goal-based codes
    • W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
    • Hazeltine, E. (2002). The representational nature of sequence learning: Evidence for goal-based codes. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Attention and Performance XIX: Common mechanisms in perception and action (pp. 673-689). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
    • (2002) Attention and Performance XIX: Common mechanisms in perception and action , pp. 673-689
    • Hazeltine, E.1
  • 21
    • 77956754145 scopus 로고
    • Spatial coding and spatio-anatomical mapping: Evidence for a hierarchical model of spatial stimulus-response compatibility
    • R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Amsterdam, the Netherlands: North-Holland. doi:10.1016/S0166-4115(08)61220-0
    • Heister, G., Schroeder-Heister, P., & Ehrenstein, W. H. (1990). Spatial coding and spatio-anatomical mapping: Evidence for a hierarchical model of spatial stimulus-response compatibility. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 117-143). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: North-Holland. doi:10.1016/S0166-4115(08)61220-0
    • (1990) Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective , pp. 117-143
    • Heister, G.1    Schroeder-Heister, P.2    Ehrenstein, W.H.3
  • 22
    • 0141786426 scopus 로고
    • A study of the relative amounts of forward and backward associations of verbal material
    • doi:10.1037/h0053740
    • Hermans, T. G. (1936). A study of the relative amounts of forward and backward associations of verbal material. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 19, 769-775. doi:10.1037/h0053740
    • (1936) Journal of Experimental Psychology , vol.19 , pp. 769-775
    • Hermans, T.G.1
  • 24
    • 33845891768 scopus 로고
    • Inverting the Simon effect by intention: Determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information
    • doi:10.1007/BF00419687
    • Hommel, B. (1993). Inverting the Simon effect by intention: Determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information. Psychological Research, 55, 270-279. doi:10.1007/BF00419687
    • (1993) Psychological Research , vol.55 , pp. 270-279
    • Hommel, B.1
  • 25
    • 0032177002 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance
    • doi:10.1037/0096-1523.24.5.1368
    • Hommel, B. (1998). Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1368-1384. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.24.5.1368
    • (1998) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.24 , pp. 1368-1384
    • Hommel, B.1
  • 26
    • 6344251612 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action
    • doi:10.1016/ j.tics.2004.08.007
    • Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 494-500. doi:10.1016/ j.tics.2004.08.007
    • (2004) Trends in Cognitive Sciences , vol.8 , pp. 494-500
    • Hommel, B.1
  • 27
    • 0035495020 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning
    • doi:10.1017/S0140525X01000103
    • Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849-878. doi:10.1017/S0140525X01000103
    • (2001) Behavioral and Brain Sciences , vol.24 , pp. 849-878
    • Hommel, B.1    Müsseler, J.2    Aschersleben, G.3    Prinz, W.4
  • 28
    • 0035286497 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Computational modelling of visual attention
    • doi:10.1038/35058500
    • Itti, L., & Koch, C. (2001). Computational modelling of visual attention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 194-203. doi:10.1038/35058500
    • (2001) Nature Reviews Neuroscience , vol.2 , pp. 194-203
    • Itti, L.1    Koch, C.2
  • 30
    • 0038618950 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation
    • doi:10.1037/0033-295X.110.2.316
    • Keele, S. W., Ivry, R. B., Mayr, U., Hazeltine, E., & Heuer, H. (2003). The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation. Psychological Review, 110, 316-339. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.110.2.316
    • (2003) Psychological Review , vol.110 , pp. 316-339
    • Keele, S.W.1    Ivry, R.B.2    Mayr, U.3    Hazeltine, E.4    Heuer, H.5
  • 31
    • 33947426497 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Anticipatory response control in motor sequence learning: Evidence from stimulus-response compatibility
    • doi:10.1016/j.humov.2007.01.004
    • Koch, I. (2007). Anticipatory response control in motor sequence learning: Evidence from stimulus-response compatibility. Human Movement Science, 26, 257-274. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2007.01.004
    • (2007) Human Movement Science , vol.26 , pp. 257-274
    • Koch, I.1
  • 32
    • 0034221898 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The role of stimulus-based and responsebased spatial information in sequence learning
    • doi:10.1037/0278-7393.26.4.863
    • Koch, I., & Hoffmann, J. (2000). The role of stimulus-based and responsebased spatial information in sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 863-882. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.26.4.863
    • (2000) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , vol.26 , pp. 863-882
    • Koch, I.1    Hoffmann, J.2
  • 33
    • 0025410809 scopus 로고
    • Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility-A model and taxonomy
    • doi:10.1037/0033295X.97.2.253
    • Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility-A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253-270. doi:10.1037/0033295X.97.2.253
    • (1990) Psychological Review , vol.97 , pp. 253-270
    • Kornblum, S.1    Hasbroucq, T.2    Osman, A.3
  • 34
    • 34249726244 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • No anticipation-no action: The role of anticipation in action and perception
    • doi:10.1007/s10339-007-0162-2
    • Kunde, W., Elsner, K., & Kiesel, A. (2007). No anticipation-no action: The role of anticipation in action and perception. Cognitive Processing, 8, 71-78. doi:10.1007/s10339-007-0162-2
    • (2007) Cognitive Processing , vol.8 , pp. 71-78
    • Kunde, W.1    Elsner, K.2    Kiesel, A.3
  • 35
    • 0034935039 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • On the status of location in visual attention
    • doi:10.1080/ 09541440125789
    • Lamy, D., & Tsal, Y. (2001). On the status of location in visual attention. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 13, 305-342. doi:10.1080/ 09541440125789
    • (2001) European Journal of Cognitive Psychology , vol.13 , pp. 305-342
    • Lamy, D.1    Tsal, Y.2
  • 36
    • 0034231860 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The role of inhibition in the regulation of sequential action
    • doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00268
    • Li, K. Z. H., Lindenberger, U., Rünger, D., & Frensch, P. A. (2000). The role of inhibition in the regulation of sequential action. Psychological Science, 11, 343-347. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00268
    • (2000) Psychological Science , vol.11 , pp. 343-347
    • Li, K.Z.H.1    Lindenberger, U.2    Rünger, D.3    Frensch, P.A.4
  • 37
    • 85047686232 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations
    • doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.393
    • Logan, G. D., & Gordon, R. D. (2001). Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. Psychological Review, 108, 393-434. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.393
    • (2001) Psychological Review , vol.108 , pp. 393-434
    • Logan, G.D.1    Gordon, R.D.2
  • 39
    • 0001600455 scopus 로고
    • The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects
    • doi:10.3758/ BF03210959
    • Lu, C.-H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 174-207. doi:10.3758/ BF03210959
    • (1995) Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , vol.2 , pp. 174-207
    • Lu, C.-H.1    Proctor, R.W.2
  • 40
    • 0003552386 scopus 로고
    • The role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behavior
    • New York, NY: Liveright
    • Luria, A. R. (1961). The role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behavior. New York, NY: Liveright.
    • (1961)
    • Luria, A.R.1
  • 41
    • 25644454368 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Location specificity in response selection processes for visual stimuli
    • doi:10.3758/BF03193802
    • Magen, H., & Cohen, A. (2005). Location specificity in response selection processes for visual stimuli. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 541-548. doi:10.3758/BF03193802
    • (2005) Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , vol.12 , pp. 541-548
    • Magen, H.1    Cohen, A.2
  • 43
    • 0141679266 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Using confidence intervals for graphically based data interpretation
    • doi:10.1037/h0087426
    • Masson, M. E. J., & Loftus, G. R. (2003). Using confidence intervals for graphically based data interpretation. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 203-220. doi:10.1037/h0087426
    • (2003) Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology , vol.57 , pp. 203-220
    • Masson, M.E.J.1    Loftus, G.R.2
  • 44
    • 0030093919 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Spatial attention and implicit sequence learning: Evidence for independent learning of spatial and nonspatial sequences
    • doi:10.1037/0278-7393.22.2.350
    • Mayr, U. (1996). Spatial attention and implicit sequence learning: Evidence for independent learning of spatial and nonspatial sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 350-364. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.22.2.350
    • (1996) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , vol.22 , pp. 350-364
    • Mayr, U.1
  • 45
    • 46349097236 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Control by action representation and input selection (CARIS): A framework for task switching involving speeded perceptual classification
    • doi:10.1007/s00426-008-0136-8
    • Meiran, N., Kessler, Y., & Adi-Japha, E. (2008). Control by action representation and input selection (CARIS): A framework for task switching involving speeded perceptual classification. Psychological Research, 72, 473-500. doi:10.1007/s00426-008-0136-8
    • (2008) Psychological Research , vol.72 , pp. 473-500
    • Meiran, N.1    Kessler, Y.2    Adi-Japha, E.3
  • 46
    • 33745345068 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Tailoring perception and action to the task at hand
    • doi:10.1080/09541440500423228
    • Memelink, J., & Hommel, B. (2006). Tailoring perception and action to the task at hand. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 18, 579-592. doi:10.1080/09541440500423228
    • (2006) European Journal of Cognitive Psychology , vol.18 , pp. 579-592
    • Memelink, J.1    Hommel, B.2
  • 47
    • 0008668948 scopus 로고
    • Plasticity and mechanization of the problem box habit in guinea pigs
    • doi:10.1037/h0074566
    • Muenzinger, K. F. (1928). Plasticity and mechanization of the problem box habit in guinea pigs. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 8, 45-69. doi:10.1037/h0074566
    • (1928) Journal of Comparative Psychology , vol.8 , pp. 45-69
    • Muenzinger, K.F.1
  • 48
    • 0242594008 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Visual search for singleton feature targets across dimensions: Stimulusand expectancydriven effects in dimensional weighting
    • doi:10.1037/0096-1523.29.5.1021
    • Müller, H. J., Reimann, B., & Krummenacher, J. (2003). Visual search for singleton feature targets across dimensions: Stimulusand expectancydriven effects in dimensional weighting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 1021-1035. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.29.5.1021
    • (2003) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.29 , pp. 1021-1035
    • Müller, H.J.1    Reimann, B.2    Krummenacher, J.3
  • 50
    • 0003109533 scopus 로고
    • Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures
    • doi:10.1016/0010-0285(87)90002-8
    • Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1-32. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(87)90002-8
    • (1987) Cognitive Psychology , vol.19 , pp. 1-32
    • Nissen, M.J.1    Bullemer, P.2
  • 51
    • 33244474205 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Interactions between encoding and retrieval in the domain of sequence-learning
    • doi:10.1037/0278-7393.32.1.118
    • Perlman, A., & Tzelgov, J. (2006). Interactions between encoding and retrieval in the domain of sequence-learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 118-130. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.32.1.118
    • (2006) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , vol.32 , pp. 118-130
    • Perlman, A.1    Tzelgov, J.2
  • 52
    • 77958153249 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Learning at any rate: Action-effect learning for stimulus-based actions
    • doi:10.1007/s00426-010-0288-1
    • Pfister, R., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2011). Learning at any rate: Action-effect learning for stimulus-based actions. Psychological Research, 75, 61-65. doi:10.1007/s00426-010-0288-1
    • (2011) Psychological Research , vol.75 , pp. 61-65
    • Pfister, R.1    Kiesel, A.2    Hoffmann, J.3
  • 56
    • 0033656931 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Auditory stimulus-response compatibility: Is there a contribution of stimulus-hand correspondence?
    • doi:10.1007/PL00008173
    • Roswarski, T. E., & Proctor, R. W. (2000). Auditory stimulus-response compatibility: Is there a contribution of stimulus-hand correspondence? Psychological Research, 63, 148-158. doi:10.1007/PL00008173
    • (2000) Psychological Research , vol.63 , pp. 148-158
    • Roswarski, T.E.1    Proctor, R.W.2
  • 57
    • 0038530836 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The role of instructions, practice, and stimulus-hand correspondence on the Simon effect
    • doi10.1007/s00426-002-0107-4
    • Roswarski, T. E., & Proctor, R. W. (2003). The role of instructions, practice, and stimulus-hand correspondence on the Simon effect. Psychological Research, 67, 43-55. doi10.1007/s00426-002-0107-4
    • (2003) Psychological Research , vol.67 , pp. 43-55
    • Roswarski, T.E.1    Proctor, R.W.2
  • 58
    • 12144266520 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Right-left prevalence with task-irrelevant spatial codes
    • doi:10.1007/s00426-003-0168-z
    • Rubichi, S., Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (2005). Right-left prevalence with task-irrelevant spatial codes. Psychological Research, 69, 167-178. doi:10.1007/s00426-003-0168-z
    • (2005) Psychological Research , vol.69 , pp. 167-178
    • Rubichi, S.1    Nicoletti, R.2    Umiltà, C.3
  • 60
    • 55049114299 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • How incidental sequence learning creates reportable knowledge: The role of unexpected events
    • doi:10.1037/a0012942
    • Rünger, D., & Frensch, P. A. (2008). How incidental sequence learning creates reportable knowledge: The role of unexpected events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 1011-1026. doi:10.1037/a0012942
    • (2008) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , vol.34 , pp. 1011-1026
    • Rünger, D.1    Frensch, P.A.2
  • 61
    • 77950348504 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Defining consciousness in the context of incidental sequence learning: Theoretical considerations and empirical implications
    • doi:10.1007/s00426-008-0225-8
    • Rünger, D., & Frensch, P. A. (2010). Defining consciousness in the context of incidental sequence learning: Theoretical considerations and empirical implications. Psychological Research, 74, 121-137. doi:10.1007/s00426-008-0225-8
    • (2010) Psychological Research , vol.74 , pp. 121-137
    • Rünger, D.1    Frensch, P.A.2
  • 62
    • 48249115158 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Task set and prefrontal cortex
    • doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125642
    • Sakai, K. (2008). Task set and prefrontal cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 31, 219-245. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125642
    • (2008) Annual Review of Neuroscience , vol.31 , pp. 219-245
    • Sakai, K.1
  • 63
    • 34447301822 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Defining task-set reconfiguration: The case of reference point switching
    • doi:10.3758/BF03194038
    • Schneider, D. W., & Logan, G. D. (2007). Defining task-set reconfiguration: The case of reference point switching. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 118-125. doi:10.3758/BF03194038
    • (2007) Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , vol.14 , pp. 118-125
    • Schneider, D.W.1    Logan, G.D.2
  • 64
    • 77957577833 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Implicit sequence learning is represented by stimulus-response rules
    • doi:10.3758/MC.38.6.677
    • Schwarb, H., & Schumacher, E. H. (2010). Implicit sequence learning is represented by stimulus-response rules. Memory & Cognition, 38, 677-688. doi:10.3758/MC.38.6.677
    • (2010) Memory & Cognition , vol.38 , pp. 677-688
    • Schwarb, H.1    Schumacher, E.H.2
  • 65
    • 0001395132 scopus 로고
    • Graded state machines: The representation of temporal contingencies in simple recurrent networks
    • doi:10.1007/BF00114843
    • Servan-Schreiber, D., Cleeremans, A., & McClelland, J. L. (1991). Graded state machines: The representation of temporal contingencies in simple recurrent networks. Machine Learning, 7, 161-193. doi:10.1007/BF00114843
    • (1991) Machine Learning , vol.7 , pp. 161-193
    • Servan-Schreiber, D.1    Cleeremans, A.2    McClelland, J.L.3
  • 66
    • 77956783876 scopus 로고
    • The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing
    • R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Amsterdam, the Netherlands: North Holland. doi:10.1016/S01664115(08)61218-2
    • Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 183-223). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: North Holland. doi:10.1016/S01664115(08)61218-2
    • (1990) Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective , pp. 183-223
    • Simon, J.R.1
  • 67
    • 0014858927 scopus 로고
    • Auditory S-R compatibility: Reaction time as a function of ear-hand correspondence and ear-response-location correspondence
    • doi:10.1037/h0029783
    • Simon, J. R., Hinrichs, J. V., & Craft, J. L. (1970). Auditory S-R compatibility: Reaction time as a function of ear-hand correspondence and ear-response-location correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86, 97-102. doi:10.1037/h0029783
    • (1970) Journal of Experimental Psychology , vol.86 , pp. 97-102
    • Simon, J.R.1    Hinrichs, J.V.2    Craft, J.L.3
  • 69
    • 0042643747 scopus 로고
    • The type-theory of the simple reaction
    • doi:10.1093/mind/IV.16.506
    • Titchener, E. B. (1895). The type-theory of the simple reaction. Mind, 4, 506-514. doi:10.1093/mind/IV.16.506
    • (1895) Mind , vol.4 , pp. 506-514
    • Titchener, E.B.1
  • 70
    • 0000622341 scopus 로고
    • Properties, parts and objects
    • K. Boff, L. Kaufman, & J. Thomas (Eds.), New York, NY: Wiley.
    • Treisman, A. M. (1986). Properties, parts and objects. In K. Boff, L. Kaufman, & J. Thomas (Eds.), Handbook of perception and human performance (pp. 1-70). New York, NY: Wiley.
    • (1986) Handbook of perception and human performance , pp. 1-70
    • Treisman, A.M.1
  • 72
    • 33847668719 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Modes of executive control in sequence learning: From stimulus-based to plan-based control
    • doi:10.1037/0096-3445.136.1.43
    • Tubau, E., Hommel, B., & López-Moliner, J. (2007). Modes of executive control in sequence learning: From stimulus-based to plan-based control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 43-63. doi:10.1037/ 0096-3445.136.1.43
    • (2007) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , vol.136 , pp. 43-63
    • Tubau, E.1    Hommel, B.2    López-Moliner, J.3
  • 73
    • 0015081595 scopus 로고
    • S-R compatibility and the idea of a response code
    • doi:10.1037/h0030892
    • Wallace, R. J. (1971). S-R compatibility and the idea of a response code. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 88, 354-360. doi:10.1037/h0030892
    • (1971) Journal of Experimental Psychology , vol.88 , pp. 354-360
    • Wallace, R.J.1
  • 74
    • 24944575128 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The influence of task instructions on action coding: Constraint setting or direct coding?
    • doi:10.1037/0096-1523.31.4.803
    • Wenke, D., & Frensch, P. A. (2005). The influence of task instructions on action coding: Constraint setting or direct coding? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 803-819. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.31.4.803
    • (2005) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.31 , pp. 803-819
    • Wenke, D.1    Frensch, P.A.2
  • 75
    • 33846371912 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Instruction-induced feature binding
    • doi:10.1007/s00426-005-0038-y
    • Wenke, D., Gaschler, R., & Nattkemper, D. (2007). Instruction-induced feature binding. Psychological Research, 71, 92-106. doi:10.1007/s00426-005-0038-y
    • (2007) Psychological Research , vol.71 , pp. 92-106
    • Wenke, D.1    Gaschler, R.2    Nattkemper, D.3
  • 76
    • 67449146903 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Strategic influences on implementing instructions for future actions
    • doi:10.1007/s00426-009-0239-x
    • Wenke, D., Gaschler, R., Nattkemper, D., & Frensch, P. A. (2009). Strategic influences on implementing instructions for future actions. Psychological Research, 73, 587-601. doi:10.1007/s00426-009-0239-x
    • (2009) Psychological Research , vol.73 , pp. 587-601
    • Wenke, D.1    Gaschler, R.2    Nattkemper, D.3    Frensch, P.A.4
  • 77
    • 0032116811 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning
    • doi:10.1037/0033295X.105.3.558
    • Willingham, D. B. (1998). A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 105, 558-584. doi:10.1037/0033295X.105.3.558
    • (1998) Psychological Review , vol.105 , pp. 558-584
    • Willingham, D.B.1
  • 78
    • 0032916025 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Implicit motor sequence learning is not purely perceptual
    • doi:10.3758/BE03211549
    • Willingham, D. B. (1999). Implicit motor sequence learning is not purely perceptual. Memory & Cognition, 27, 561-572. doi:10.3758/BF03211549
    • (1999) Memory & Cognition , vol.27 , pp. 561-572
    • Willingham, D.B.1
  • 79
    • 0034090066 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Implicit motor sequence learning is represented in response locations
    • doi:10.3758/BF03198552
    • Willingham, D. B., Wells, L. A., Farrell, J. M., & Stemwedel, M. E. (2000). Implicit motor sequence learning is represented in response locations. Memory & Cognition, 28, 366-375. doi:10.3758/BF03198552
    • (2000) Memory & Cognition , vol.28 , pp. 366-375
    • Willingham, D.B.1    Wells, L.A.2    Farrell, J.M.3    Stemwedel, M.E.4
  • 80
    • 0032381658 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Response-effect learning as a major component of implicit serial learning
    • doi:10.1037/0278-7393.24.4.962
    • Ziessler, M. (1998). Response-effect learning as a major component of implicit serial learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 962-978. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.24.4.962
    • (1998) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , vol.24 , pp. 962-978
    • Ziessler, M.1
  • 81
    • 0036365387 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The impact of response mode on implicit and explicit sequence learning
    • doi:10.1027//1618-3169.49.2.153
    • Zirngibl, C., & Koch, I. (2002). The impact of response mode on implicit and explicit sequence learning. Experimental Psychology, 49, 153-162. doi:10.1027//1618-3169.49.2.153
    • (2002) Experimental Psychology , vol.49 , pp. 153-162
    • Zirngibl, C.1    Koch, I.2


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.