-
1
-
-
78149459488
-
Representing serial action and perception
-
doi:10.3758/PBR.17.5.603
-
Abrahamse, E. L., Jiménez, J., Verwey, W. B., & Clegg, B. A. (2010). Representing serial action and perception. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 603-623. doi:10.3758/PBR.17.5.603
-
(2010)
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
, vol.17
, pp. 603-623
-
-
Abrahamse, E.L.1
Jiménez, J.2
Verwey, W.B.3
Clegg, B.A.4
-
2
-
-
68349125532
-
Sensory information in perceptual-motor sequence learning: Visual and/or tactile stimuli
-
doi:10.1007/s00221-009-1903-5
-
Abrahamse, E. L., Van der Lubbe, R. H. J., & Verwey, W. B. (2009). Sensory information in perceptual-motor sequence learning: Visual and/or tactile stimuli. Experimental Brain Research, 197, 175-183. doi:10.1007/s00221-009-1903-5
-
(2009)
Experimental Brain Research
, vol.197
, pp. 175-183
-
-
Abrahamse, E.L.1
Van der Lubbe, R.H.J.2
Verwey, W.B.3
-
4
-
-
23844499079
-
Bidirectional associations in humans and rats
-
doi:10.1037/0097-7403.31.3.301
-
Arcediano, F., Escobar, M., & Miller, R. R. (2005). Bidirectional associations in humans and rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 31, 301-318. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.31.3.301
-
(2005)
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes
, vol.31
, pp. 301-318
-
-
Arcediano, F.1
Escobar, M.2
Miller, R.R.3
-
5
-
-
0026815614
-
Visual parsing and response competition: The effect of grouping factors
-
doi:10.3758/BF03212239
-
Baylis, G. C., & Driver, J. (1992). Visual parsing and response competition: The effect of grouping factors. Perception & Psychophysics, 51, 145-162. doi:10.3758/BF03212239
-
(1992)
Perception & Psychophysics
, vol.51
, pp. 145-162
-
-
Baylis, G.C.1
Driver, J.2
-
6
-
-
67650770603
-
Sequence learning in serial reaction time tasks: Integrated and independent learning of hand-related constituent sequences
-
doi:10.1037/a0015505
-
Berner, M. P., & Hoffmann, J. (2009). Sequence learning in serial reaction time tasks: Integrated and independent learning of hand-related constituent sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 890-904. doi:10.1037/a0015505
-
(2009)
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
, vol.35
, pp. 890-904
-
-
Berner, M.P.1
Hoffmann, J.2
-
7
-
-
1642503299
-
Neural substrates of response-based sequence learning using fMRI
-
doi:10.1162/089892904322755610
-
Bischoff-Grethe, A., Goedert, K. M., Willingham, D. T., & Grafton, S. T. (2004). Neural substrates of response-based sequence learning using fMRI. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 127-138. doi:10.1162/089892904322755610
-
(2004)
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
, vol.16
, pp. 127-138
-
-
Bischoff-Grethe, A.1
Goedert, K.M.2
Willingham, D.T.3
Grafton, S.T.4
-
8
-
-
0012159271
-
Implicit learning and consciousness: A graded, dynamic perspective
-
R. M. French & A. Cleeremans (Eds.), Hove, England: Psychology Press.
-
Cleeremans, A., & Jiménez, L. (2002). Implicit learning and consciousness: A graded, dynamic perspective. In R. M. French & A. Cleeremans (Eds.), Implicit learning and consciousness (pp. 1-40). Hove, England: Psychology Press.
-
(2002)
Implicit learning and consciousness
, pp. 1-40
-
-
Cleeremans, A.1
Jiménez, L.2
-
9
-
-
0002355821
-
Attention and structure in sequence learning
-
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.16.1.17
-
Cohen, A., Ivry, R. I., & Keele, S. W. (1990). Attention and structure in sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 17-30. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.16.1.17
-
(1990)
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
, vol.16
, pp. 17-30
-
-
Cohen, A.1
Ivry, R.I.2
Keele, S.W.3
-
10
-
-
0031090693
-
Perceptual dimensional constraints on response selection processes
-
doi:10.1006/cogp.1997.0648
-
Cohen, A., & Shoup, R. (1997). Perceptual dimensional constraints on response selection processes. Cognitive Psychology, 32, 128-181. doi:10.1006/cogp.1997.0648
-
(1997)
Cognitive Psychology
, vol.32
, pp. 128-181
-
-
Cohen, A.1
Shoup, R.2
-
11
-
-
33746868269
-
What do we learn from binding features? Evidence for multilevel feature integration
-
doi:10.1037/0096-1523.32.3.705
-
Colzato, L. S., Raffone, A., & Hommel, B. (2006). What do we learn from binding features? Evidence for multilevel feature integration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 705-716. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.32.3.705
-
(2006)
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
, vol.32
, pp. 705-716
-
-
Colzato, L.S.1
Raffone, A.2
Hommel, B.3
-
12
-
-
0028489201
-
Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence
-
doi:10.1037/0096-1523.20.4.731
-
De Jong, R., Liang, C. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731-750. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.20.4.731
-
(1994)
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
, vol.20
, pp. 731-750
-
-
De Jong, R.1
Liang, C.C.2
Lauber, E.3
-
13
-
-
33645070583
-
The role of response selection in sequence learning
-
doi:10.1080/17470210500462684
-
Deroost, N., & Soetens, E. (2006a). The role of response selection in sequence learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 449-456. doi:10.1080/17470210500462684
-
(2006)
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
, vol.59
, pp. 449-456
-
-
Deroost, N.1
Soetens, E.2
-
14
-
-
33645292328
-
Spatial processing and perceptual sequence learning in SRT tasks
-
doi:10.1027/1618-3169.53.1.16
-
Deroost, N., & Soetens, E. (2006b). Spatial processing and perceptual sequence learning in SRT tasks. Experimental Psychology, 53, 16-30. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.53.1.16
-
(2006)
Experimental Psychology
, vol.53
, pp. 16-30
-
-
Deroost, N.1
Soetens, E.2
-
15
-
-
65349157829
-
How task representations guide attention: Further evidence for the shielding function of task sets
-
doi:10.1037/a0014647
-
Dreisbach, G., & Haider, H. (2009). How task representations guide attention: Further evidence for the shielding function of task sets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 477-486. doi:10.1037/a0014647
-
(2009)
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
, vol.35
, pp. 477-486
-
-
Dreisbach, G.1
Haider, H.2
-
16
-
-
0004141817
-
-
über das Gedächtnis [Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology] (H. A.Ruger & C.E.Bussenius Trans.). doi:10.1037/10011-000 (Original work published 1885)
-
Ebbinghaus, H. (1913). über das Gedächtnis [Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology] (H. A. Ruger & C. E. Bussenius, Trans.). doi:10.1037/10011-000 (Original work published 1885)
-
(1913)
-
-
Ebbinghaus, H.1
-
18
-
-
0028012696
-
Effects of presentation rate and of individual differences in short-term memory capacity on an indirect measure of serial learning
-
doi:10.3758/BF03202765
-
Frensch, P. A., & Miner, C. S. (1994). Effects of presentation rate and of individual differences in short-term memory capacity on an indirect measure of serial learning. Memory & Cognition, 22, 95-110. doi:10.3758/BF03202765
-
(1994)
Memory & Cognition
, vol.22
, pp. 95-110
-
-
Frensch, P.A.1
Miner, C.S.2
-
19
-
-
34547480747
-
Is information reduction an itemspecific or an item-general process?
-
doi:10.1080/00207590701396526
-
Gaschler, R., & Frensch, P. A. (2007). Is information reduction an itemspecific or an item-general process? International Journal of Psychology, 42, 218-228. doi:10.1080/00207590701396526
-
(2007)
International Journal of Psychology
, vol.42
, pp. 218-228
-
-
Gaschler, R.1
Frensch, P.A.2
-
20
-
-
0242483997
-
The representational nature of sequence learning: Evidence for goal-based codes
-
W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
-
Hazeltine, E. (2002). The representational nature of sequence learning: Evidence for goal-based codes. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Attention and Performance XIX: Common mechanisms in perception and action (pp. 673-689). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
-
(2002)
Attention and Performance XIX: Common mechanisms in perception and action
, pp. 673-689
-
-
Hazeltine, E.1
-
21
-
-
77956754145
-
Spatial coding and spatio-anatomical mapping: Evidence for a hierarchical model of spatial stimulus-response compatibility
-
R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Amsterdam, the Netherlands: North-Holland. doi:10.1016/S0166-4115(08)61220-0
-
Heister, G., Schroeder-Heister, P., & Ehrenstein, W. H. (1990). Spatial coding and spatio-anatomical mapping: Evidence for a hierarchical model of spatial stimulus-response compatibility. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 117-143). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: North-Holland. doi:10.1016/S0166-4115(08)61220-0
-
(1990)
Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective
, pp. 117-143
-
-
Heister, G.1
Schroeder-Heister, P.2
Ehrenstein, W.H.3
-
22
-
-
0141786426
-
A study of the relative amounts of forward and backward associations of verbal material
-
doi:10.1037/h0053740
-
Hermans, T. G. (1936). A study of the relative amounts of forward and backward associations of verbal material. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 19, 769-775. doi:10.1037/h0053740
-
(1936)
Journal of Experimental Psychology
, vol.19
, pp. 769-775
-
-
Hermans, T.G.1
-
23
-
-
0035294215
-
Irrelevant response effects improve serial learning in serial reaction time tasks
-
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.27.2.470
-
Hoffmann, J., Sebald, A., & Stöcker, C. (2001). Irrelevant response effects improve serial learning in serial reaction time tasks. Journal of Exper-imental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 470-482. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.27.2.470
-
(2001)
Journal of Exper-imental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
, vol.27
, pp. 470-482
-
-
Hoffmann, J.1
Sebald, A.2
Stöcker, C.3
-
24
-
-
33845891768
-
Inverting the Simon effect by intention: Determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information
-
doi:10.1007/BF00419687
-
Hommel, B. (1993). Inverting the Simon effect by intention: Determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant spatial information. Psychological Research, 55, 270-279. doi:10.1007/BF00419687
-
(1993)
Psychological Research
, vol.55
, pp. 270-279
-
-
Hommel, B.1
-
25
-
-
0032177002
-
Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance
-
doi:10.1037/0096-1523.24.5.1368
-
Hommel, B. (1998). Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1368-1384. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.24.5.1368
-
(1998)
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
, vol.24
, pp. 1368-1384
-
-
Hommel, B.1
-
26
-
-
6344251612
-
Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action
-
doi:10.1016/ j.tics.2004.08.007
-
Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 494-500. doi:10.1016/ j.tics.2004.08.007
-
(2004)
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
, vol.8
, pp. 494-500
-
-
Hommel, B.1
-
27
-
-
0035495020
-
The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning
-
doi:10.1017/S0140525X01000103
-
Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849-878. doi:10.1017/S0140525X01000103
-
(2001)
Behavioral and Brain Sciences
, vol.24
, pp. 849-878
-
-
Hommel, B.1
Müsseler, J.2
Aschersleben, G.3
Prinz, W.4
-
28
-
-
0035286497
-
Computational modelling of visual attention
-
doi:10.1038/35058500
-
Itti, L., & Koch, C. (2001). Computational modelling of visual attention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 194-203. doi:10.1038/35058500
-
(2001)
Nature Reviews Neuroscience
, vol.2
, pp. 194-203
-
-
Itti, L.1
Koch, C.2
-
29
-
-
0033484919
-
Which attention is needed for implicit sequence learning?
-
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.25.1.236
-
Jiménez, L., & Méndez, C. (1999). Which attention is needed for implicit sequence learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 236-259. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.25.1.236
-
(1999)
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
, vol.25
, pp. 236-259
-
-
Jiménez, L.1
Méndez, C.2
-
30
-
-
0038618950
-
The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation
-
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.110.2.316
-
Keele, S. W., Ivry, R. B., Mayr, U., Hazeltine, E., & Heuer, H. (2003). The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation. Psychological Review, 110, 316-339. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.110.2.316
-
(2003)
Psychological Review
, vol.110
, pp. 316-339
-
-
Keele, S.W.1
Ivry, R.B.2
Mayr, U.3
Hazeltine, E.4
Heuer, H.5
-
31
-
-
33947426497
-
Anticipatory response control in motor sequence learning: Evidence from stimulus-response compatibility
-
doi:10.1016/j.humov.2007.01.004
-
Koch, I. (2007). Anticipatory response control in motor sequence learning: Evidence from stimulus-response compatibility. Human Movement Science, 26, 257-274. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2007.01.004
-
(2007)
Human Movement Science
, vol.26
, pp. 257-274
-
-
Koch, I.1
-
32
-
-
0034221898
-
The role of stimulus-based and responsebased spatial information in sequence learning
-
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.26.4.863
-
Koch, I., & Hoffmann, J. (2000). The role of stimulus-based and responsebased spatial information in sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 863-882. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.26.4.863
-
(2000)
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
, vol.26
, pp. 863-882
-
-
Koch, I.1
Hoffmann, J.2
-
33
-
-
0025410809
-
Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility-A model and taxonomy
-
doi:10.1037/0033295X.97.2.253
-
Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility-A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253-270. doi:10.1037/0033295X.97.2.253
-
(1990)
Psychological Review
, vol.97
, pp. 253-270
-
-
Kornblum, S.1
Hasbroucq, T.2
Osman, A.3
-
34
-
-
34249726244
-
No anticipation-no action: The role of anticipation in action and perception
-
doi:10.1007/s10339-007-0162-2
-
Kunde, W., Elsner, K., & Kiesel, A. (2007). No anticipation-no action: The role of anticipation in action and perception. Cognitive Processing, 8, 71-78. doi:10.1007/s10339-007-0162-2
-
(2007)
Cognitive Processing
, vol.8
, pp. 71-78
-
-
Kunde, W.1
Elsner, K.2
Kiesel, A.3
-
35
-
-
0034935039
-
On the status of location in visual attention
-
doi:10.1080/ 09541440125789
-
Lamy, D., & Tsal, Y. (2001). On the status of location in visual attention. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 13, 305-342. doi:10.1080/ 09541440125789
-
(2001)
European Journal of Cognitive Psychology
, vol.13
, pp. 305-342
-
-
Lamy, D.1
Tsal, Y.2
-
36
-
-
0034231860
-
The role of inhibition in the regulation of sequential action
-
doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00268
-
Li, K. Z. H., Lindenberger, U., Rünger, D., & Frensch, P. A. (2000). The role of inhibition in the regulation of sequential action. Psychological Science, 11, 343-347. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00268
-
(2000)
Psychological Science
, vol.11
, pp. 343-347
-
-
Li, K.Z.H.1
Lindenberger, U.2
Rünger, D.3
Frensch, P.A.4
-
37
-
-
85047686232
-
Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations
-
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.393
-
Logan, G. D., & Gordon, R. D. (2001). Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. Psychological Review, 108, 393-434. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.393
-
(2001)
Psychological Review
, vol.108
, pp. 393-434
-
-
Logan, G.D.1
Gordon, R.D.2
-
38
-
-
0030542333
-
Attention in the acquisition and expression of automaticity
-
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.22.3.620
-
Logan, G. D., Taylor, S. E., & Etherton, J. L. (1996). Attention in the acquisition and expression of automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 620-638. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.22.3.620
-
(1996)
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
, vol.22
, pp. 620-638
-
-
Logan, G.D.1
Taylor, S.E.2
Etherton, J.L.3
-
39
-
-
0001600455
-
The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects
-
doi:10.3758/ BF03210959
-
Lu, C.-H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 174-207. doi:10.3758/ BF03210959
-
(1995)
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
, vol.2
, pp. 174-207
-
-
Lu, C.-H.1
Proctor, R.W.2
-
40
-
-
0003552386
-
The role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behavior
-
New York, NY: Liveright
-
Luria, A. R. (1961). The role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behavior. New York, NY: Liveright.
-
(1961)
-
-
Luria, A.R.1
-
41
-
-
25644454368
-
Location specificity in response selection processes for visual stimuli
-
doi:10.3758/BF03193802
-
Magen, H., & Cohen, A. (2005). Location specificity in response selection processes for visual stimuli. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 541-548. doi:10.3758/BF03193802
-
(2005)
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
, vol.12
, pp. 541-548
-
-
Magen, H.1
Cohen, A.2
-
43
-
-
0141679266
-
Using confidence intervals for graphically based data interpretation
-
doi:10.1037/h0087426
-
Masson, M. E. J., & Loftus, G. R. (2003). Using confidence intervals for graphically based data interpretation. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 203-220. doi:10.1037/h0087426
-
(2003)
Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology
, vol.57
, pp. 203-220
-
-
Masson, M.E.J.1
Loftus, G.R.2
-
44
-
-
0030093919
-
Spatial attention and implicit sequence learning: Evidence for independent learning of spatial and nonspatial sequences
-
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.22.2.350
-
Mayr, U. (1996). Spatial attention and implicit sequence learning: Evidence for independent learning of spatial and nonspatial sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 350-364. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.22.2.350
-
(1996)
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
, vol.22
, pp. 350-364
-
-
Mayr, U.1
-
45
-
-
46349097236
-
Control by action representation and input selection (CARIS): A framework for task switching involving speeded perceptual classification
-
doi:10.1007/s00426-008-0136-8
-
Meiran, N., Kessler, Y., & Adi-Japha, E. (2008). Control by action representation and input selection (CARIS): A framework for task switching involving speeded perceptual classification. Psychological Research, 72, 473-500. doi:10.1007/s00426-008-0136-8
-
(2008)
Psychological Research
, vol.72
, pp. 473-500
-
-
Meiran, N.1
Kessler, Y.2
Adi-Japha, E.3
-
46
-
-
33745345068
-
Tailoring perception and action to the task at hand
-
doi:10.1080/09541440500423228
-
Memelink, J., & Hommel, B. (2006). Tailoring perception and action to the task at hand. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 18, 579-592. doi:10.1080/09541440500423228
-
(2006)
European Journal of Cognitive Psychology
, vol.18
, pp. 579-592
-
-
Memelink, J.1
Hommel, B.2
-
47
-
-
0008668948
-
Plasticity and mechanization of the problem box habit in guinea pigs
-
doi:10.1037/h0074566
-
Muenzinger, K. F. (1928). Plasticity and mechanization of the problem box habit in guinea pigs. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 8, 45-69. doi:10.1037/h0074566
-
(1928)
Journal of Comparative Psychology
, vol.8
, pp. 45-69
-
-
Muenzinger, K.F.1
-
48
-
-
0242594008
-
Visual search for singleton feature targets across dimensions: Stimulusand expectancydriven effects in dimensional weighting
-
doi:10.1037/0096-1523.29.5.1021
-
Müller, H. J., Reimann, B., & Krummenacher, J. (2003). Visual search for singleton feature targets across dimensions: Stimulusand expectancydriven effects in dimensional weighting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 1021-1035. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.29.5.1021
-
(2003)
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
, vol.29
, pp. 1021-1035
-
-
Müller, H.J.1
Reimann, B.2
Krummenacher, J.3
-
50
-
-
0003109533
-
Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures
-
doi:10.1016/0010-0285(87)90002-8
-
Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1-32. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(87)90002-8
-
(1987)
Cognitive Psychology
, vol.19
, pp. 1-32
-
-
Nissen, M.J.1
Bullemer, P.2
-
51
-
-
33244474205
-
Interactions between encoding and retrieval in the domain of sequence-learning
-
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.32.1.118
-
Perlman, A., & Tzelgov, J. (2006). Interactions between encoding and retrieval in the domain of sequence-learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 118-130. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.32.1.118
-
(2006)
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
, vol.32
, pp. 118-130
-
-
Perlman, A.1
Tzelgov, J.2
-
52
-
-
77958153249
-
Learning at any rate: Action-effect learning for stimulus-based actions
-
doi:10.1007/s00426-010-0288-1
-
Pfister, R., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2011). Learning at any rate: Action-effect learning for stimulus-based actions. Psychological Research, 75, 61-65. doi:10.1007/s00426-010-0288-1
-
(2011)
Psychological Research
, vol.75
, pp. 61-65
-
-
Pfister, R.1
Kiesel, A.2
Hoffmann, J.3
-
53
-
-
0019022633
-
Attention and the detection of signals
-
doi:10.1037/0096-3445.109.2.160
-
Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 160-174. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.109.2.160
-
(1980)
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
, vol.109
, pp. 160-174
-
-
Posner, M.I.1
Snyder, C.R.2
Davidson, B.J.3
-
55
-
-
0141864530
-
Pure perceptual-based sequence learning
-
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.29.4.581
-
Remillard, G. (2003). Pure perceptual-based sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 581-597. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.29.4.581
-
(2003)
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
, vol.29
, pp. 581-597
-
-
Remillard, G.1
-
56
-
-
0033656931
-
Auditory stimulus-response compatibility: Is there a contribution of stimulus-hand correspondence?
-
doi:10.1007/PL00008173
-
Roswarski, T. E., & Proctor, R. W. (2000). Auditory stimulus-response compatibility: Is there a contribution of stimulus-hand correspondence? Psychological Research, 63, 148-158. doi:10.1007/PL00008173
-
(2000)
Psychological Research
, vol.63
, pp. 148-158
-
-
Roswarski, T.E.1
Proctor, R.W.2
-
57
-
-
0038530836
-
The role of instructions, practice, and stimulus-hand correspondence on the Simon effect
-
doi10.1007/s00426-002-0107-4
-
Roswarski, T. E., & Proctor, R. W. (2003). The role of instructions, practice, and stimulus-hand correspondence on the Simon effect. Psychological Research, 67, 43-55. doi10.1007/s00426-002-0107-4
-
(2003)
Psychological Research
, vol.67
, pp. 43-55
-
-
Roswarski, T.E.1
Proctor, R.W.2
-
58
-
-
12144266520
-
Right-left prevalence with task-irrelevant spatial codes
-
doi:10.1007/s00426-003-0168-z
-
Rubichi, S., Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (2005). Right-left prevalence with task-irrelevant spatial codes. Psychological Research, 69, 167-178. doi:10.1007/s00426-003-0168-z
-
(2005)
Psychological Research
, vol.69
, pp. 167-178
-
-
Rubichi, S.1
Nicoletti, R.2
Umiltà, C.3
-
59
-
-
33748803043
-
Spatial coding in two dimensions
-
doi:10.3758/BF03193832
-
Rubichi, S., Vu, K.-P. L., Nicoletti, R., & Proctor, R. W. (2006). Spatial coding in two dimensions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 201-216. doi:10.3758/BF03193832
-
(2006)
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
, vol.13
, pp. 201-216
-
-
Rubichi, S.1
Vu, K.-P.L.2
Nicoletti, R.3
Proctor, R.W.4
-
60
-
-
55049114299
-
How incidental sequence learning creates reportable knowledge: The role of unexpected events
-
doi:10.1037/a0012942
-
Rünger, D., & Frensch, P. A. (2008). How incidental sequence learning creates reportable knowledge: The role of unexpected events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 1011-1026. doi:10.1037/a0012942
-
(2008)
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
, vol.34
, pp. 1011-1026
-
-
Rünger, D.1
Frensch, P.A.2
-
61
-
-
77950348504
-
Defining consciousness in the context of incidental sequence learning: Theoretical considerations and empirical implications
-
doi:10.1007/s00426-008-0225-8
-
Rünger, D., & Frensch, P. A. (2010). Defining consciousness in the context of incidental sequence learning: Theoretical considerations and empirical implications. Psychological Research, 74, 121-137. doi:10.1007/s00426-008-0225-8
-
(2010)
Psychological Research
, vol.74
, pp. 121-137
-
-
Rünger, D.1
Frensch, P.A.2
-
62
-
-
48249115158
-
Task set and prefrontal cortex
-
doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125642
-
Sakai, K. (2008). Task set and prefrontal cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 31, 219-245. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125642
-
(2008)
Annual Review of Neuroscience
, vol.31
, pp. 219-245
-
-
Sakai, K.1
-
63
-
-
34447301822
-
Defining task-set reconfiguration: The case of reference point switching
-
doi:10.3758/BF03194038
-
Schneider, D. W., & Logan, G. D. (2007). Defining task-set reconfiguration: The case of reference point switching. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 118-125. doi:10.3758/BF03194038
-
(2007)
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
, vol.14
, pp. 118-125
-
-
Schneider, D.W.1
Logan, G.D.2
-
64
-
-
77957577833
-
Implicit sequence learning is represented by stimulus-response rules
-
doi:10.3758/MC.38.6.677
-
Schwarb, H., & Schumacher, E. H. (2010). Implicit sequence learning is represented by stimulus-response rules. Memory & Cognition, 38, 677-688. doi:10.3758/MC.38.6.677
-
(2010)
Memory & Cognition
, vol.38
, pp. 677-688
-
-
Schwarb, H.1
Schumacher, E.H.2
-
65
-
-
0001395132
-
Graded state machines: The representation of temporal contingencies in simple recurrent networks
-
doi:10.1007/BF00114843
-
Servan-Schreiber, D., Cleeremans, A., & McClelland, J. L. (1991). Graded state machines: The representation of temporal contingencies in simple recurrent networks. Machine Learning, 7, 161-193. doi:10.1007/BF00114843
-
(1991)
Machine Learning
, vol.7
, pp. 161-193
-
-
Servan-Schreiber, D.1
Cleeremans, A.2
McClelland, J.L.3
-
66
-
-
77956783876
-
The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing
-
R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Amsterdam, the Netherlands: North Holland. doi:10.1016/S01664115(08)61218-2
-
Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 183-223). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: North Holland. doi:10.1016/S01664115(08)61218-2
-
(1990)
Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective
, pp. 183-223
-
-
Simon, J.R.1
-
67
-
-
0014858927
-
Auditory S-R compatibility: Reaction time as a function of ear-hand correspondence and ear-response-location correspondence
-
doi:10.1037/h0029783
-
Simon, J. R., Hinrichs, J. V., & Craft, J. L. (1970). Auditory S-R compatibility: Reaction time as a function of ear-hand correspondence and ear-response-location correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86, 97-102. doi:10.1037/h0029783
-
(1970)
Journal of Experimental Psychology
, vol.86
, pp. 97-102
-
-
Simon, J.R.1
Hinrichs, J.V.2
Craft, J.L.3
-
69
-
-
0042643747
-
The type-theory of the simple reaction
-
doi:10.1093/mind/IV.16.506
-
Titchener, E. B. (1895). The type-theory of the simple reaction. Mind, 4, 506-514. doi:10.1093/mind/IV.16.506
-
(1895)
Mind
, vol.4
, pp. 506-514
-
-
Titchener, E.B.1
-
70
-
-
0000622341
-
Properties, parts and objects
-
K. Boff, L. Kaufman, & J. Thomas (Eds.), New York, NY: Wiley.
-
Treisman, A. M. (1986). Properties, parts and objects. In K. Boff, L. Kaufman, & J. Thomas (Eds.), Handbook of perception and human performance (pp. 1-70). New York, NY: Wiley.
-
(1986)
Handbook of perception and human performance
, pp. 1-70
-
-
Treisman, A.M.1
-
72
-
-
33847668719
-
Modes of executive control in sequence learning: From stimulus-based to plan-based control
-
doi:10.1037/0096-3445.136.1.43
-
Tubau, E., Hommel, B., & López-Moliner, J. (2007). Modes of executive control in sequence learning: From stimulus-based to plan-based control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 43-63. doi:10.1037/ 0096-3445.136.1.43
-
(2007)
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
, vol.136
, pp. 43-63
-
-
Tubau, E.1
Hommel, B.2
López-Moliner, J.3
-
73
-
-
0015081595
-
S-R compatibility and the idea of a response code
-
doi:10.1037/h0030892
-
Wallace, R. J. (1971). S-R compatibility and the idea of a response code. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 88, 354-360. doi:10.1037/h0030892
-
(1971)
Journal of Experimental Psychology
, vol.88
, pp. 354-360
-
-
Wallace, R.J.1
-
74
-
-
24944575128
-
The influence of task instructions on action coding: Constraint setting or direct coding?
-
doi:10.1037/0096-1523.31.4.803
-
Wenke, D., & Frensch, P. A. (2005). The influence of task instructions on action coding: Constraint setting or direct coding? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 803-819. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.31.4.803
-
(2005)
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
, vol.31
, pp. 803-819
-
-
Wenke, D.1
Frensch, P.A.2
-
75
-
-
33846371912
-
Instruction-induced feature binding
-
doi:10.1007/s00426-005-0038-y
-
Wenke, D., Gaschler, R., & Nattkemper, D. (2007). Instruction-induced feature binding. Psychological Research, 71, 92-106. doi:10.1007/s00426-005-0038-y
-
(2007)
Psychological Research
, vol.71
, pp. 92-106
-
-
Wenke, D.1
Gaschler, R.2
Nattkemper, D.3
-
76
-
-
67449146903
-
Strategic influences on implementing instructions for future actions
-
doi:10.1007/s00426-009-0239-x
-
Wenke, D., Gaschler, R., Nattkemper, D., & Frensch, P. A. (2009). Strategic influences on implementing instructions for future actions. Psychological Research, 73, 587-601. doi:10.1007/s00426-009-0239-x
-
(2009)
Psychological Research
, vol.73
, pp. 587-601
-
-
Wenke, D.1
Gaschler, R.2
Nattkemper, D.3
Frensch, P.A.4
-
77
-
-
0032116811
-
A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning
-
doi:10.1037/0033295X.105.3.558
-
Willingham, D. B. (1998). A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 105, 558-584. doi:10.1037/0033295X.105.3.558
-
(1998)
Psychological Review
, vol.105
, pp. 558-584
-
-
Willingham, D.B.1
-
78
-
-
0032916025
-
Implicit motor sequence learning is not purely perceptual
-
doi:10.3758/BE03211549
-
Willingham, D. B. (1999). Implicit motor sequence learning is not purely perceptual. Memory & Cognition, 27, 561-572. doi:10.3758/BF03211549
-
(1999)
Memory & Cognition
, vol.27
, pp. 561-572
-
-
Willingham, D.B.1
-
79
-
-
0034090066
-
Implicit motor sequence learning is represented in response locations
-
doi:10.3758/BF03198552
-
Willingham, D. B., Wells, L. A., Farrell, J. M., & Stemwedel, M. E. (2000). Implicit motor sequence learning is represented in response locations. Memory & Cognition, 28, 366-375. doi:10.3758/BF03198552
-
(2000)
Memory & Cognition
, vol.28
, pp. 366-375
-
-
Willingham, D.B.1
Wells, L.A.2
Farrell, J.M.3
Stemwedel, M.E.4
-
80
-
-
0032381658
-
Response-effect learning as a major component of implicit serial learning
-
doi:10.1037/0278-7393.24.4.962
-
Ziessler, M. (1998). Response-effect learning as a major component of implicit serial learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 962-978. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.24.4.962
-
(1998)
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
, vol.24
, pp. 962-978
-
-
Ziessler, M.1
-
81
-
-
0036365387
-
The impact of response mode on implicit and explicit sequence learning
-
doi:10.1027//1618-3169.49.2.153
-
Zirngibl, C., & Koch, I. (2002). The impact of response mode on implicit and explicit sequence learning. Experimental Psychology, 49, 153-162. doi:10.1027//1618-3169.49.2.153
-
(2002)
Experimental Psychology
, vol.49
, pp. 153-162
-
-
Zirngibl, C.1
Koch, I.2
|