메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 82, Issue 4, 2018, Pages 759-770

Hierarchical task organization in dual tasks: evidence for higher level task representations

Author keywords

Dual tasks processing; Hierarchical higher level tasks; Psychological refractory period; Task pairs

Indexed keywords

ATTENTION; BEHAVIOR; FEMALE; HUMAN; MALE; PSYCHOLOGICAL REFRACTORY PERIOD; REACTION TIME; REPETITION PRIMING; YOUNG ADULT;

EID: 85014913140     PISSN: 03400727     EISSN: 14302772     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1007/s00426-017-0851-0     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (27)

References (43)
  • 2
    • 34248223602 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Task switching is not cue switching
    • Altmann, E. M. (2006). Task switching is not cue switching. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 1016–1022. doi:10.3758/BF03213918.
    • (2006) Psychonomic Bulletin and Review , vol.13 , pp. 1016-1022
    • Altmann, E.M.1
  • 3
    • 34547413840 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Separating cue encoding from target processing in the explicit task-cuing paradigm: Are there true task switch effects?
    • Arrington, C. M., Logan, G. D., & Schneider, D. W. (2007). Separating cue encoding from target processing in the explicit task-cuing paradigm: Are there true task switch effects? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 484–502. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.484.
    • (2007) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , vol.33 , pp. 484-502
    • Arrington, C.M.1    Logan, G.D.2    Schneider, D.W.3
  • 5
    • 0029242625 scopus 로고
    • The role of preparation in overlapping-task performance
    • De Jong, R. (1995). The role of preparation in overlapping-task performance. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48 A, 2–25. doi:10.1080/14640749508401372.
    • (1995) Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , pp. 25-48
    • De Jong, R.1
  • 7
    • 34547882359 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The influence of overlapping response sets on task inhibition
    • Gade, M., & Koch, I. (2007). The influence of overlapping response sets on task inhibition. Memory & Cognition, 35, 603–609. doi:10.3758/BF03193298.
    • (2007) Memory & Cognition , vol.35 , pp. 603-609
    • Gade, M.1    Koch, I.2
  • 9
    • 0002568457 scopus 로고
    • Motor constraints in dual-task performance
    • Damos DL, (ed), Taylor & Francis, London
    • Heuer, H. (1991). Motor constraints in dual-task performance. In D. L. Damos (Ed.), Multiple task performance (pp. 173–204). London: Taylor & Francis.
    • (1991) Multiple task performance , pp. 173-204
    • Heuer, H.1
  • 12
    • 6344251612 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action
    • Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 494–500. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.08.007.
    • (2004) Trends in Cognitive Science , vol.8 , pp. 494-500
    • Hommel, B.1
  • 13
  • 15
    • 44349100003 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Exogenous and endogenous response priming with auditory stimuli
    • Keller, P. E., & Koch, I. (2006). Exogenous and endogenous response priming with auditory stimuli. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 2, 269–276. doi:10.2478/v10053-008-0061-9.
    • (2006) Advances in Cognitive Psychology , vol.2 , pp. 269-276
    • Keller, P.E.1    Koch, I.2
  • 16
    • 37848999420 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Action planning in sequential skills: Relations to music performance
    • Keller, P. E., & Koch, I. (2008). Action planning in sequential skills: Relations to music performance. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 6, 275–291. doi:10.1080/17470210601160864.
    • (2008) Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , vol.6 , pp. 275-291
    • Keller, P.E.1    Koch, I.2
  • 18
    • 1842810943 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Hierarchical switching with two types of judgment and two stimulus dimensions
    • Kleinsorge, T. (2004). Hierarchical switching with two types of judgment and two stimulus dimensions. Experimental Psychology, 51, 145–149. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.51.2.145.
    • (2004) Experimental Psychology , vol.51 , pp. 145-149
    • Kleinsorge, T.1
  • 19
    • 0000935994 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Hierarchical switching in a multi-dimensional task space
    • Kleinsorge, T., & Heuer, H. (1999). Hierarchical switching in a multi-dimensional task space. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 62, 300–312. doi:10.1007/s004260050060.
    • (1999) Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung , vol.62 , pp. 300-312
    • Kleinsorge, T.1    Heuer, H.2
  • 21
    • 3042802029 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Assembling a task space: Global determination of local shift costs
    • Kleinsorge, T., Heuer, H., & Schmidtke, V. (2004). Assembling a task space: Global determination of local shift costs. Psychological Research, 68, 31–40. doi:10.1007/s00426-003-0134-9.
    • (2004) Psychological Research , vol.68 , pp. 31-40
    • Kleinsorge, T.1    Heuer, H.2    Schmidtke, V.3
  • 23
    • 2442574782 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Task switching in a hierarchical task structure: Evidence for the fragility of the task repetition benefit
    • Lien, M. C., & Ruthruff, E. (2004). Task switching in a hierarchical task structure: Evidence for the fragility of the task repetition benefit. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 697–713. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.30.3.697.
    • (2004) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , vol.30 , pp. 697-713
    • Lien, M.C.1    Ruthruff, E.2
  • 24
    • 1842646010 scopus 로고
    • Toward an instance theory of automatization
    • Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492–527. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.95.4.492.
    • (1988) Psychological Review , vol.95 , pp. 492-527
    • Logan, G.D.1
  • 26
    • 33645083251 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Dual route for subtask order control: Evidence from the psychological refractory paradigm
    • Luria, R., & Meiran, N. (2006). Dual route for subtask order control: Evidence from the psychological refractory paradigm. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 720–744. doi:10.1080/02724980543000060.
    • (2006) The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , vol.59 , pp. 720-744
    • Luria, R.1    Meiran, N.2
  • 27
    • 0034150366 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Changing internal constraints on action: the role of backward inhibition
    • Mayr, U., & Keele, S. W. (2000). Changing internal constraints on action: the role of backward inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1, 4–26. doi:10.10371/0096-3445.129.1.4.
    • (2000) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , vol.1 , pp. 4-26
    • Mayr, U.1    Keele, S.W.2
  • 28
    • 33750002508 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Backward crosstalk effects in psychological refractory period paradigms: Effects of second-task response type on first-task response latencies
    • Miller, J. (2006). Backward crosstalk effects in psychological refractory period paradigms: Effects of second-task response type on first-task response latencies. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 70, 484–493. doi:10.1007/s00426-005-0011-9.
    • (2006) Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung , vol.70 , pp. 484-493
    • Miller, J.1
  • 29
    • 33746923428 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Can the task-cuing paradigm measure an endogenous task-set reconfiguration process?
    • Monsell, S., & Mizon, G. A. (2006). Can the task-cuing paradigm measure an endogenous task-set reconfiguration process? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 493–516. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.32.3.493.
    • (2006) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.32 , pp. 493-516
    • Monsell, S.1    Mizon, G.A.2
  • 30
    • 70349772546 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Confidence intervals from normalized data: A correction to Cousineau (2005)
    • Morey, R. D. (2008). Confidence intervals from normalized data: A correction to Cousineau (2005). Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 4, 61–64.
    • (2008) Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology , vol.4 , pp. 61-64
    • Morey, R.D.1
  • 31
    • 0036583139 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Queuing or sharing? A critical evaluation of the single-bottleneck notion
    • Navon, D., & Miller, J. (2002). Queuing or sharing? A critical evaluation of the single-bottleneck notion. Cognitive Psychology, 44, 193–251. doi:10.1006/cogp.2001.0767.
    • (2002) Cognitive Psychology , vol.44 , pp. 193-251
    • Navon, D.1    Miller, J.2
  • 32
    • 0028502653 scopus 로고
    • Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory
    • Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 358–377. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.116.2.220.
    • (1994) Psychological Bulletin , vol.116 , pp. 358-377
    • Pashler, H.1
  • 33
    • 77957838609 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The integration of task-set components into cognitive task representations
    • Philipp, A., & Koch, I. (2010). The integration of task-set components into cognitive task representations. Psychologica Belgica, 50, 383–411. doi:10.5334/pb-50-3-4-383.
    • (2010) Psychologica Belgica , vol.50 , pp. 383-411
    • Philipp, A.1    Koch, I.2
  • 34
    • 84885052274 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • What are task-sets: A single, integrated representation or a collection of multiple control representations?
    • Rangelov, D., Töllner, T., Müller, H. J., & Zehetleitner, M. (2013). What are task-sets: A single, integrated representation or a collection of multiple control representations? Frontierts in Human Neuroscience. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00524.
    • (2013) Frontierts in Human Neuroscience
    • Rangelov, D.1    Töllner, T.2    Müller, H.J.3    Zehetleitner, M.4
  • 35
    • 21844510093 scopus 로고
    • Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks
    • Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.207.
    • (1995) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , vol.124 , pp. 207-231
    • Rogers, R.D.1    Monsell, S.2
  • 36
    • 33644908767 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Spatial representation of pitch height: The SMARC effect
    • Rusconi, E., Kwan, B., Giordano, B. L., Umiltà, C., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Spatial representation of pitch height: The SMARC effect. Cognition, 99, 113–129. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2005.01.004.
    • (2006) Cognition , vol.99 , pp. 113-129
    • Rusconi, E.1    Kwan, B.2    Giordano, B.L.3    Umiltà, C.4    Butterworth, B.5
  • 37
    • 33750711410 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Hierarchical control of cognitive processes: Switching tasks in sequences
    • Schneider, D. W., & Logan, G. D. (2006). Hierarchical control of cognitive processes: Switching tasks in sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 623–640. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.135.4.623.
    • (2006) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , vol.135 , pp. 623-640
    • Schneider, D.W.1    Logan, G.D.2
  • 38
    • 2642552096 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The costs of changing the representation of action: Response repetition and response-response compatibility in dual tasks
    • Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2004). The costs of changing the representation of action: Response repetition and response-response compatibility in dual tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 566–582. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.30.3.566.
    • (2004) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.30 , pp. 566-582
    • Schuch, S.1    Koch, I.2
  • 39
    • 33750028978 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Task-order coordination in dual-task performance and the lateral prefrontal cortex: An event-related fMRI study
    • Szameitat, A. J., Lepsien, J., von Cramon, D. Y., Sterr, A., & Schubert, T. (2006). Task-order coordination in dual-task performance and the lateral prefrontal cortex: An event-related fMRI study. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 70, 541–552. doi:10.1007/s00426-005-0015-5.
    • (2006) Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung , vol.70 , pp. 541-552
    • Szameitat, A.J.1    Lepsien, J.2    von Cramon, D.Y.3    Sterr, A.4    Schubert, T.5
  • 41
    • 47949099886 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Response grouping in the psychological refractory period (PRP) paradigm: Models and contamination effects
    • Ulrich, R., & Miller, J. (2008). Response grouping in the psychological refractory period (PRP) paradigm: Models and contamination effects. Cognitive Psychology, 57, 75–121. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2007.06.004.
    • (2008) Cognitive Psychology , vol.57 , pp. 75-121
    • Ulrich, R.1    Miller, J.2
  • 42
    • 54949124928 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • On the representation of taks information in task switching: Evidence from task and dimension switching
    • Vandierendonck, A., Christiaens, E., & Liefooghe, B. (2008). On the representation of taks information in task switching: Evidence from task and dimension switching. Memory & Cognition, 36, 1248–1261. doi:10.3758/MC.36.7.1248.
    • (2008) Memory & Cognition , vol.36 , pp. 1248-1261
    • Vandierendonck, A.1    Christiaens, E.2    Liefooghe, B.3
  • 43
    • 84980111132 scopus 로고
    • The ‘psychological refractory period’ and the timing of high-speed performance – A review and a theory
    • Welford, A. T. (1952). The ‘psychological refractory period’ and the timing of high-speed performance – A review and a theory. British Journal of Psychology, 43, 2–19. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1952.tb00322.x.
    • (1952) British Journal of Psychology , vol.43 , pp. 2-19
    • Welford, A.T.1


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.