-
1
-
-
77952552802
-
Transfer of learning between 2D and 3D sources during infancy: Informing theory and practice
-
Barr, R. (2010). Transfer of learning between 2D and 3D sources during infancy: Informing theory and practice. Developmental Review, 20, 128-154. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2010.03.001
-
(2010)
Developmental Review
, vol.20
, pp. 128-154
-
-
Barr, R.1
-
2
-
-
84887408830
-
Memory constraints on infant learning from picture books, television, and touchscreens
-
Barr, R. (2013). Memory constraints on infant learning from picture books, television, and touchscreens. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 205-210. doi:10.1111/cdep.12041
-
(2013)
Child Development Perspectives
, vol.7
, pp. 205-210
-
-
Barr, R.1
-
3
-
-
84868345467
-
Fast mapping, slow learning: Disambiguation of novel word-object mappings in relation to vocabulary learning at 18, 24, and 30 months
-
Bion, R. A. H., Borovsky, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Fast mapping, slow learning: Disambiguation of novel word-object mappings in relation to vocabulary learning at 18, 24, and 30 months. Cognition, 126, 39-53.
-
(2013)
Cognition
, vol.126
, pp. 39-53
-
-
Bion, R.A.H.1
Borovsky, A.2
Fernald, A.3
-
4
-
-
78651491573
-
Beyond fast mapping
-
Carey, S. (2010). Beyond fast mapping. Language Learning and Development, 6, 184-205. doi:10.1080/15475441.2010.484379
-
(2010)
Language Learning and Development
, vol.6
, pp. 184-205
-
-
Carey, S.1
-
5
-
-
0034413366
-
Short-form versions of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories
-
Fenson, L., Pethick, S., Renda, C., Cox, J. L., Dale, P. S., & Reznick, J. S. (2000). Short-form versions of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 95-115. doi:10.1017/S0142716400001053
-
(2000)
Applied Psycholinguistics
, vol.21
, pp. 95-115
-
-
Fenson, L.1
Pethick, S.2
Renda, C.3
Cox, J.L.4
Dale, P.S.5
Reznick, J.S.6
-
6
-
-
58249130675
-
Development of infants' attention to faces during the first year
-
Frank, M. C., Vul, E., & Johnson, S. P. (2009). Development of infants' attention to faces during the first year. Cognition, 110, 160-170. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.11.010
-
(2009)
Cognition
, vol.110
, pp. 160-170
-
-
Frank, M.C.1
Vul, E.2
Johnson, S.P.3
-
7
-
-
33645559246
-
-
Menlo Park, CA: Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation
-
Garrison, M. M., & Christakis, D. A. (2005). A teacher in the living room? Educational media for babies, toddlers, and preschoolers. Menlo Park, CA: Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation.
-
(2005)
A teacher in the living room? Educational media for babies, toddlers, and preschoolers
-
-
Garrison, M.M.1
Christakis, D.A.2
-
8
-
-
84863399067
-
Age differences in online processing of video: An eye movement study
-
Kirkorian, H. L., Anderson, D. R., & Keen, R. (2012). Age differences in online processing of video: An eye movement study. Child Development, 83, 497-507. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01719.x
-
(2012)
Child Development
, vol.83
, pp. 497-507
-
-
Kirkorian, H.L.1
Anderson, D.R.2
Keen, R.3
-
9
-
-
33845722684
-
Is speech learning "gated" by the social brain?
-
Kuhl, P. K. (2007). Is speech learning "gated" by the social brain? Developmental Science, 10, 110-120. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00572.x
-
(2007)
Developmental Science
, vol.10
, pp. 110-120
-
-
Kuhl, P.K.1
-
10
-
-
77956187528
-
Contingent computer interactions for young children's object retrieval success
-
Lauricella, A. R., Pempek, T. A., Barr, R., & Calvert, S. L. (2010). Contingent computer interactions for young children's object retrieval success. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31, 362-369. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2010.06.002
-
(2010)
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology
, vol.31
, pp. 362-369
-
-
Lauricella, A.R.1
Pempek, T.A.2
Barr, R.3
Calvert, S.L.4
-
11
-
-
21844507055
-
Sixteen- and 24-month-olds' use of mutual exclusivity as a default assumption in second-label learning
-
Liittschwager, J. C., & Markman, E. M. (1994). Sixteen- and 24-month-olds' use of mutual exclusivity as a default assumption in second-label learning. Developmental Psychology, 30, 955-968. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.30.6.955
-
(1994)
Developmental Psychology
, vol.30
, pp. 955-968
-
-
Liittschwager, J.C.1
Markman, E.M.2
-
13
-
-
0141884266
-
Use of the mutual exclusivity assumption by young word learners
-
Markman, E. M., Wasow, J. L., & Hansen, M. B. (2003). Use of the mutual exclusivity assumption by young word learners. Cognitive Psychology, 47, 241-275. doi:10.1016/S0010-0285(03)00034-3
-
(2003)
Cognitive Psychology
, vol.47
, pp. 241-275
-
-
Markman, E.M.1
Wasow, J.L.2
Hansen, M.B.3
-
14
-
-
49549105968
-
The effect of social engagement on 24-month-olds' imitation from live and televised models
-
Nielsen, M., Simcock, G., & Jenkins, L. (2008). The effect of social engagement on 24-month-olds' imitation from live and televised models. Developmental Science, 11, 722-731. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00722.x
-
(2008)
Developmental Science
, vol.11
, pp. 722-731
-
-
Nielsen, M.1
Simcock, G.2
Jenkins, L.3
-
15
-
-
79955607283
-
Third-party social interaction and word learning from video
-
O'Doherty, K., Troseth, G. L., Shimpi, P. M., Goldenberg, E., Akhtar, N., & Saylor, M. M. (2011). Third-party social interaction and word learning from video. Child Development, 82, 902-915. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01579.x
-
(2011)
Child Development
, vol.82
, pp. 902-915
-
-
O'Doherty, K.1
Troseth, G.L.2
Shimpi, P.M.3
Goldenberg, E.4
Akhtar, N.5
Saylor, M.M.6
-
16
-
-
84861555723
-
Zero to eight: Children's media use in America 2013
-
Retrieved from
-
Rideout, V. (2013). Zero to eight: Children's media use in America 2013. Retrieved from http://www.commonsensemedia.org
-
(2013)
-
-
Rideout, V.1
-
17
-
-
84897628072
-
Skype me! Socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language
-
Roseberry, S., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2014). Skype me! Socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language. Child Development, 85, 956-970. doi:10.1111/cdev.12166
-
(2014)
Child Development
, vol.85
, pp. 956-970
-
-
Roseberry, S.1
Hirsh-Pasek, K.2
Golinkoff, R.M.3
-
18
-
-
70349226737
-
Live action: Can young children learn verbs from video?
-
Roseberry, S., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Parish-Morris, J., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2009). Live action: Can young children learn verbs from video? Child Development, 80, 1360-1375. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01338.x
-
(2009)
Child Development
, vol.80
, pp. 1360-1375
-
-
Roseberry, S.1
Hirsh-Pasek, K.2
Parish-Morris, J.3
Golinkoff, R.M.4
-
19
-
-
0036253820
-
Television and reality: Toddlers' use of visual information from video to guide behavior
-
Schmitt, K. L., & Anderson, D. R. (2002). Television and reality: Toddlers' use of visual information from video to guide behavior. Media Psychology, 4, 51-76. doi:10.1207/S1532785XMEP0401_03
-
(2002)
Media Psychology
, vol.4
, pp. 51-76
-
-
Schmitt, K.L.1
Anderson, D.R.2
-
20
-
-
77950721192
-
Do 2-year-olds disambiguate and extend words learned from video?
-
Scofield, J., & Williams, A. (2009). Do 2-year-olds disambiguate and extend words learned from video? First Language, 29, 228-240. doi:10.1177/0142723708101681
-
(2009)
First Language
, vol.29
, pp. 228-240
-
-
Scofield, J.1
Williams, A.2
-
21
-
-
84869885980
-
iLearn II: An analysis of the educational category of Apple's app store
-
Joan Ganz Cooney Center. Retrieved from
-
Shuler, C., Levine, Z., & Ree, J. (2012). iLearn II: An analysis of the educational category of Apple's app store. Joan Ganz Cooney Center. Retrieved from www.joanganzcooneycenter.org
-
(2012)
-
-
Shuler, C.1
Levine, Z.2
Ree, J.3
-
22
-
-
0032140655
-
The medium can obscure the message: Young children's understanding of video
-
Troseth, G. L., & DeLoache, J. S. (1998). The medium can obscure the message: Young children's understanding of video. Child Development, 69, 950-965. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06153.x
-
(1998)
Child Development
, vol.69
, pp. 950-965
-
-
Troseth, G.L.1
DeLoache, J.S.2
-
23
-
-
33646781580
-
Young children's use of video as a source of socially relevant information
-
Troseth, G. L., Saylor, M. M., & Archer, A. H. (2006). Young children's use of video as a source of socially relevant information. Child Development, 77, 786-799. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00903.x
-
(2006)
Child Development
, vol.77
, pp. 786-799
-
-
Troseth, G.L.1
Saylor, M.M.2
Archer, A.H.3
-
24
-
-
84906248916
-
The spacing effect in children's generalization of knowledge: Allowing children time to forget promotes their ability to learn
-
Vlach, H. A. (2014). The spacing effect in children's generalization of knowledge: Allowing children time to forget promotes their ability to learn. Child Development Perspectives, 8, 163-168. doi:10.1111/cdep.12079
-
(2014)
Child Development Perspectives
, vol.8
, pp. 163-168
-
-
Vlach, H.A.1
-
25
-
-
78649964714
-
Developmental differences in children's context-dependent word learning
-
Vlach, H. A., & Sandhofer, C. M. (2011). Developmental differences in children's context-dependent word learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 394-401. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.09.011
-
(2011)
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
, vol.108
, pp. 394-401
-
-
Vlach, H.A.1
Sandhofer, C.M.2
-
26
-
-
84919954229
-
Visual motherese? Signal-to-noise ratios in toddler-directed television
-
Wass, S. V., & Smith, T. J. (2015). Visual motherese? Signal-to-noise ratios in toddler-directed television. Developmental Science, 18, 24-37. doi:10.1111/desc.12156
-
(2015)
Developmental Science
, vol.18
, pp. 24-37
-
-
Wass, S.V.1
Smith, T.J.2
-
27
-
-
67650292859
-
Infant imitation from television using novel touch screen technology
-
Zack, E., Barr, R., Gerhardstein, P., Dickerson, K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2009). Infant imitation from television using novel touch screen technology. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27, 13-26. doi:10.1348/026151008X334700
-
(2009)
British Journal of Developmental Psychology
, vol.27
, pp. 13-26
-
-
Zack, E.1
Barr, R.2
Gerhardstein, P.3
Dickerson, K.4
Meltzoff, A.N.5
|