메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 87, Issue 2, 2016, Pages 405-413

Toddlers' Word Learning From Contingent and Noncontingent Video on Touch Screens

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords

CHILD; HUMAN; LEARNING; MAJOR CLINICAL STUDY; SELECTIVE ATTENTION; TODDLER; TOUCH; VIDEORECORDING; ATTENTION; CHILD DEVELOPMENT; FEMALE; MALE; PHYSIOLOGY; PRESCHOOL CHILD; PSYCHOMOTOR PERFORMANCE;

EID: 84961618725     PISSN: 00093920     EISSN: 14678624     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12508     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (137)

References (27)
  • 1
    • 77952552802 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Transfer of learning between 2D and 3D sources during infancy: Informing theory and practice
    • Barr, R. (2010). Transfer of learning between 2D and 3D sources during infancy: Informing theory and practice. Developmental Review, 20, 128-154. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2010.03.001
    • (2010) Developmental Review , vol.20 , pp. 128-154
    • Barr, R.1
  • 2
    • 84887408830 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Memory constraints on infant learning from picture books, television, and touchscreens
    • Barr, R. (2013). Memory constraints on infant learning from picture books, television, and touchscreens. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 205-210. doi:10.1111/cdep.12041
    • (2013) Child Development Perspectives , vol.7 , pp. 205-210
    • Barr, R.1
  • 3
    • 84868345467 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Fast mapping, slow learning: Disambiguation of novel word-object mappings in relation to vocabulary learning at 18, 24, and 30 months
    • Bion, R. A. H., Borovsky, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Fast mapping, slow learning: Disambiguation of novel word-object mappings in relation to vocabulary learning at 18, 24, and 30 months. Cognition, 126, 39-53.
    • (2013) Cognition , vol.126 , pp. 39-53
    • Bion, R.A.H.1    Borovsky, A.2    Fernald, A.3
  • 4
    • 78651491573 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Beyond fast mapping
    • Carey, S. (2010). Beyond fast mapping. Language Learning and Development, 6, 184-205. doi:10.1080/15475441.2010.484379
    • (2010) Language Learning and Development , vol.6 , pp. 184-205
    • Carey, S.1
  • 5
    • 0034413366 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Short-form versions of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories
    • Fenson, L., Pethick, S., Renda, C., Cox, J. L., Dale, P. S., & Reznick, J. S. (2000). Short-form versions of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 95-115. doi:10.1017/S0142716400001053
    • (2000) Applied Psycholinguistics , vol.21 , pp. 95-115
    • Fenson, L.1    Pethick, S.2    Renda, C.3    Cox, J.L.4    Dale, P.S.5    Reznick, J.S.6
  • 6
    • 58249130675 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Development of infants' attention to faces during the first year
    • Frank, M. C., Vul, E., & Johnson, S. P. (2009). Development of infants' attention to faces during the first year. Cognition, 110, 160-170. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.11.010
    • (2009) Cognition , vol.110 , pp. 160-170
    • Frank, M.C.1    Vul, E.2    Johnson, S.P.3
  • 8
    • 84863399067 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Age differences in online processing of video: An eye movement study
    • Kirkorian, H. L., Anderson, D. R., & Keen, R. (2012). Age differences in online processing of video: An eye movement study. Child Development, 83, 497-507. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01719.x
    • (2012) Child Development , vol.83 , pp. 497-507
    • Kirkorian, H.L.1    Anderson, D.R.2    Keen, R.3
  • 9
    • 33845722684 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Is speech learning "gated" by the social brain?
    • Kuhl, P. K. (2007). Is speech learning "gated" by the social brain? Developmental Science, 10, 110-120. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00572.x
    • (2007) Developmental Science , vol.10 , pp. 110-120
    • Kuhl, P.K.1
  • 11
    • 21844507055 scopus 로고
    • Sixteen- and 24-month-olds' use of mutual exclusivity as a default assumption in second-label learning
    • Liittschwager, J. C., & Markman, E. M. (1994). Sixteen- and 24-month-olds' use of mutual exclusivity as a default assumption in second-label learning. Developmental Psychology, 30, 955-968. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.30.6.955
    • (1994) Developmental Psychology , vol.30 , pp. 955-968
    • Liittschwager, J.C.1    Markman, E.M.2
  • 13
    • 0141884266 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Use of the mutual exclusivity assumption by young word learners
    • Markman, E. M., Wasow, J. L., & Hansen, M. B. (2003). Use of the mutual exclusivity assumption by young word learners. Cognitive Psychology, 47, 241-275. doi:10.1016/S0010-0285(03)00034-3
    • (2003) Cognitive Psychology , vol.47 , pp. 241-275
    • Markman, E.M.1    Wasow, J.L.2    Hansen, M.B.3
  • 14
    • 49549105968 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The effect of social engagement on 24-month-olds' imitation from live and televised models
    • Nielsen, M., Simcock, G., & Jenkins, L. (2008). The effect of social engagement on 24-month-olds' imitation from live and televised models. Developmental Science, 11, 722-731. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00722.x
    • (2008) Developmental Science , vol.11 , pp. 722-731
    • Nielsen, M.1    Simcock, G.2    Jenkins, L.3
  • 16
    • 84861555723 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Zero to eight: Children's media use in America 2013
    • Retrieved from
    • Rideout, V. (2013). Zero to eight: Children's media use in America 2013. Retrieved from http://www.commonsensemedia.org
    • (2013)
    • Rideout, V.1
  • 17
    • 84897628072 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Skype me! Socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language
    • Roseberry, S., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2014). Skype me! Socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language. Child Development, 85, 956-970. doi:10.1111/cdev.12166
    • (2014) Child Development , vol.85 , pp. 956-970
    • Roseberry, S.1    Hirsh-Pasek, K.2    Golinkoff, R.M.3
  • 18
  • 19
    • 0036253820 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Television and reality: Toddlers' use of visual information from video to guide behavior
    • Schmitt, K. L., & Anderson, D. R. (2002). Television and reality: Toddlers' use of visual information from video to guide behavior. Media Psychology, 4, 51-76. doi:10.1207/S1532785XMEP0401_03
    • (2002) Media Psychology , vol.4 , pp. 51-76
    • Schmitt, K.L.1    Anderson, D.R.2
  • 20
    • 77950721192 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Do 2-year-olds disambiguate and extend words learned from video?
    • Scofield, J., & Williams, A. (2009). Do 2-year-olds disambiguate and extend words learned from video? First Language, 29, 228-240. doi:10.1177/0142723708101681
    • (2009) First Language , vol.29 , pp. 228-240
    • Scofield, J.1    Williams, A.2
  • 21
    • 84869885980 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • iLearn II: An analysis of the educational category of Apple's app store
    • Joan Ganz Cooney Center. Retrieved from
    • Shuler, C., Levine, Z., & Ree, J. (2012). iLearn II: An analysis of the educational category of Apple's app store. Joan Ganz Cooney Center. Retrieved from www.joanganzcooneycenter.org
    • (2012)
    • Shuler, C.1    Levine, Z.2    Ree, J.3
  • 22
    • 0032140655 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The medium can obscure the message: Young children's understanding of video
    • Troseth, G. L., & DeLoache, J. S. (1998). The medium can obscure the message: Young children's understanding of video. Child Development, 69, 950-965. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06153.x
    • (1998) Child Development , vol.69 , pp. 950-965
    • Troseth, G.L.1    DeLoache, J.S.2
  • 23
    • 33646781580 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Young children's use of video as a source of socially relevant information
    • Troseth, G. L., Saylor, M. M., & Archer, A. H. (2006). Young children's use of video as a source of socially relevant information. Child Development, 77, 786-799. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00903.x
    • (2006) Child Development , vol.77 , pp. 786-799
    • Troseth, G.L.1    Saylor, M.M.2    Archer, A.H.3
  • 24
    • 84906248916 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The spacing effect in children's generalization of knowledge: Allowing children time to forget promotes their ability to learn
    • Vlach, H. A. (2014). The spacing effect in children's generalization of knowledge: Allowing children time to forget promotes their ability to learn. Child Development Perspectives, 8, 163-168. doi:10.1111/cdep.12079
    • (2014) Child Development Perspectives , vol.8 , pp. 163-168
    • Vlach, H.A.1
  • 25
    • 78649964714 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Developmental differences in children's context-dependent word learning
    • Vlach, H. A., & Sandhofer, C. M. (2011). Developmental differences in children's context-dependent word learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 394-401. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.09.011
    • (2011) Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , vol.108 , pp. 394-401
    • Vlach, H.A.1    Sandhofer, C.M.2
  • 26
    • 84919954229 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Visual motherese? Signal-to-noise ratios in toddler-directed television
    • Wass, S. V., & Smith, T. J. (2015). Visual motherese? Signal-to-noise ratios in toddler-directed television. Developmental Science, 18, 24-37. doi:10.1111/desc.12156
    • (2015) Developmental Science , vol.18 , pp. 24-37
    • Wass, S.V.1    Smith, T.J.2


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.