-
1
-
-
77249144406
-
Two sources of evidence on the non-automaticity of true and false belief ascription
-
Back, E., & Apperly, I. A. (2010). Two sources of evidence on the non-automaticity of true and false belief ascription. Cognition, 115, 54–70. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.11.008
-
(2010)
Cognition
, vol.115
, pp. 54-70
-
-
Back, E.1
Apperly, I.A.2
-
2
-
-
0024550269
-
The autistic child’s theory of mind: A case of specific developmental delay
-
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child’s theory of mind: A case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 285–297. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1989.tb00241.x
-
(1989)
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
, vol.30
, pp. 285-297
-
-
Baron-Cohen, S.1
-
3
-
-
0022142647
-
Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”?
-
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37–46. doi:10.1016/0010-02778590022-8
-
(1985)
Cognition
, vol.21
, pp. 37-46
-
-
Baron-Cohen, S.1
Leslie, A.M.2
Frith, U.3
-
4
-
-
84871868072
-
Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal
-
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 255–278. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
-
(2013)
Journal of Memory and Language
, vol.68
, pp. 255-278
-
-
Barr, D.J.1
Levy, R.2
Scheepers, C.3
Tily, H.J.4
-
6
-
-
84974191574
-
Some remarks about concepts
-
Bennett, J. (1978). Some remarks about concepts. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 1, 557–560. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00076573
-
(1978)
Behavioral & Brain Sciences
, vol.1
, pp. 557-560
-
-
Bennett, J.1
-
7
-
-
85101510947
-
A basic introduction to fixed-effect and randomeffects models for meta-analysis
-
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J., & Rothstein, H. R. (2010). A basic introduction to fixed-effect and randomeffects models for meta-analysis. Research Synthesis Methods, 1, 97–111.
-
(2010)
Research Synthesis Methods
, vol.1
, pp. 97-111
-
-
Borenstein, M.1
Hedges, L.V.2
Higgins, J.3
Rothstein, H.R.4
-
8
-
-
0036384450
-
How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory
-
Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Breton, C. (2002). How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory. Infant and Child Development, 11, 73–92. doi:10.1002/icd.298
-
(2002)
Infant and Child Development
, vol.11
, pp. 73-92
-
-
Carlson, S.M.1
Moses, L.J.2
Breton, C.3
-
9
-
-
67349236675
-
Encoding of others’ beliefs without overt instruction
-
Cohen, A. S., & German, T. C. (2009). Encoding of others’ beliefs without overt instruction. Cognition, 111, 356–363. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.03.004
-
(2009)
Cognition
, vol.111
, pp. 356-363
-
-
Cohen, A.S.1
German, T.C.2
-
14
-
-
0346882749
-
Executive function and false-belief understanding in preschool children: Two tasks are harder than one
-
Hala, S., Hug, S., & Henderson, A. (2003). Executive function and false-belief understanding in preschool children: Two tasks are harder than one. Journal of Cognition and Development, 4, 275–298. doi:10.1207/S15327647JCD040303
-
(2003)
Journal of Cognition and Development
, vol.4
, pp. 275-298
-
-
Hala, S.1
Hug, S.2
Henderson, A.3
-
15
-
-
3042838577
-
Chimpanzees are more skilful in competitive than in cooperative cognitive tasks
-
Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Chimpanzees are more skilful in competitive than in cooperative cognitive tasks. Animal Behaviour, 68, 571–581. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.11.011
-
(2004)
Animal Behaviour
, vol.68
, pp. 571-581
-
-
Hare, B.1
Tomasello, M.2
-
16
-
-
0009243927
-
The psychological refractory period effect: Only half the double-stimulation story?
-
Herman, L. M., & Kantowitz, B. H. (1970). The psychological refractory period effect: Only half the double-stimulation story? Psychological Bulletin, 73, 74–88. doi:10.1037/h0028357
-
(1970)
Psychological Bulletin
, vol.73
, pp. 74-88
-
-
Herman, L.M.1
Kantowitz, B.H.2
-
17
-
-
85027946842
-
False belief in infancy: A fresh look
-
Heyes, C. (2014). False belief in infancy: A fresh look. Developmental Science, 17, 647–659. doi:10.1111/desc.12148
-
(2014)
Developmental Science
, vol.17
, pp. 647-659
-
-
Heyes, C.1
-
18
-
-
55649087772
-
Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models
-
Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 434–446.
-
(2008)
Journal of Memory and Language
, vol.59
, pp. 434-446
-
-
Jaeger, T.F.1
-
19
-
-
0041667942
-
Limits on theory of mind use in adults
-
Keysar, B., Lin, S., & Barr, D. J. (2003). Limits on theory of mind use in adults. Cognition, 89, 25–41. doi:10.1016/S0010-02770300064-7
-
(2003)
Cognition
, vol.89
, pp. 25-41
-
-
Keysar, B.1
Lin, S.2
Barr, D.J.3
-
20
-
-
84855937201
-
Eighteen- and 24-monthold infants correct others in anticipation of action mistakes
-
Knudsen, B., & Liszkowski, U. (2012). Eighteen- and 24-monthold infants correct others in anticipation of action mistakes. Developmental Science, 15, 113–122. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01098.x
-
(2012)
Developmental Science
, vol.15
, pp. 113-122
-
-
Knudsen, B.1
Liszkowski, U.2
-
21
-
-
84907908426
-
Are all beliefs equal? Implicit belief attributions recruiting core brain regions of theory of mind
-
Kovács, A. M., Kühn, S., Gergely, G., Csibra, G., & Brass, M. (2014). Are all beliefs equal? Implicit belief attributions recruiting core brain regions of theory of mind. PLoS ONE, 9(9), Article e106558. Retrieved from http://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0106558
-
(2014)
Plos ONE
, vol.9
, Issue.9
-
-
Kovács, A.M.1
Kühn, S.2
Gergely, G.3
Csibra, G.4
Brass, M.5
-
22
-
-
78650653569
-
The social sense: Susceptibility to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults
-
Kovács, A. M., Téglás, E., & Endress, A. D. (2010). The social sense: Susceptibility to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults. Science, 330, 1830–1834. doi:10.1126/science.1190792
-
(2010)
Science
, vol.330
, pp. 1830-1834
-
-
Kovács, A.M.1
Téglás, E.2
Endress, A.D.3
-
23
-
-
84858603816
-
Implicit and explicit theory of mind: State of the art
-
Low, J., & Perner, J. (2012). Implicit and explicit theory of mind: State of the art. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 1–13. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02074.x
-
(2012)
British Journal of Developmental Psychology
, vol.30
, pp. 1-13
-
-
Low, J.1
Perner, J.2
-
24
-
-
80055096356
-
Do 10-month-old infants understand others’ false beliefs?
-
Luo, Y. (2011). Do 10-month-old infants understand others’ false beliefs? Cognition, 121, 289–298. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.07.011
-
(2011)
Cognition
, vol.121
, pp. 289-298
-
-
Luo, Y.1
-
25
-
-
21744438988
-
Executive function and children’s understanding of false belief: How specific is the relation?
-
Müller, U., Zelazo, P. D., & Imrisek, S. (2005). Executive function and children’s understanding of false belief: How specific is the relation? Cognitive Development, 20, 173–189. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2004.12.004
-
(2005)
Cognitive Development
, vol.20
, pp. 173-189
-
-
Müller, U.1
Zelazo, P.D.2
Imrisek, S.3
-
26
-
-
16444373610
-
Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs?
-
Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308, 255–258. doi:10.1126/science.1107621
-
(2005)
Science
, vol.308
, pp. 255-258
-
-
Onishi, K.H.1
Baillargeon, R.2
-
27
-
-
84877303436
-
An open, large-scale, collaborative effort to estimate the reproducibility of psychological science
-
Open Science Collaboration
-
Open Science Collaboration. (2012). An open, large-scale, collaborative effort to estimate the reproducibility of psychological science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 657–660. doi:10.1177/1745691612462588
-
(2012)
Perspectives on Psychological Science
, vol.7
, pp. 657-660
-
-
-
28
-
-
84972437637
-
When is attribution of beliefs justified?
-
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1978). When is attribution of beliefs justified? Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 1, 592–593. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00076895
-
(1978)
Behavioral & Brain Sciences
, vol.1
, pp. 592-593
-
-
Pylyshyn, Z.W.1
-
29
-
-
84930587816
-
Small telescopes: Detectability and the evaluation of replication results
-
Simonsohn, U. (2015). Small telescopes: Detectability and the evaluation of replication results. Psychological Science, 26, 559–569. doi:10.1177/0956797614567341
-
(2015)
Psychological Science
, vol.26
, pp. 559-569
-
-
Simonsohn, U.1
-
30
-
-
33847184111
-
Attribution of beliefs by 13-month-old infants
-
Surian, L., Caldi, S., & Sperber, D. (2007). Attribution of beliefs by 13-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 18, 580–586. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01943.x
-
(2007)
Psychological Science
, vol.18
, pp. 580-586
-
-
Surian, L.1
Caldi, S.2
Sperber, D.3
-
31
-
-
84858598411
-
Where will the triangle look for it? Attributing false beliefs to a geometric shape at 17 months
-
Surian, L., & Geraci, A. (2012). Where will the triangle look for it? Attributing false beliefs to a geometric shape at 17 months. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 30–44. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02046.x
-
(2012)
British Journal of Developmental Psychology
, vol.30
, pp. 30-44
-
-
Surian, L.1
Geraci, A.2
-
32
-
-
0002527556
-
The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses
-
Telford, C. W. (1931). The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 14, 1–36. doi:10.1037/h0073262
-
(1931)
Journal of Experimental Psychology
, vol.14
, pp. 1-36
-
-
Telford, C.W.1
-
33
-
-
84887339558
-
Do people automatically track others’ beliefs? Evidence from a continuous measure
-
van der Wel, R. P., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2014). Do people automatically track others’ beliefs? Evidence from a continuous measure. Cognition, 130, 128–133. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2013.10.004
-
(2014)
Cognition
, vol.130
, pp. 128-133
-
-
Van Der Wel, R.P.1
Sebanz, N.2
Knoblich, G.3
-
34
-
-
34547687763
-
Belief–desire reasoning in the explanation of behavior: Do actions speak louder than words?
-
Wertz, A. E., & German, T. C. (2007). Belief–desire reasoning in the explanation of behavior: Do actions speak louder than words? Cognition, 105, 184–194. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.08.002
-
(2007)
Cognition
, vol.105
, pp. 184-194
-
-
Wertz, A.E.1
German, T.C.2
-
35
-
-
0020686827
-
Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception
-
Wimmer, H. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128. doi:10.1016/0010-02778390004-5
-
(1983)
Cognition
, vol.13
, pp. 103-128
-
-
Wimmer, H.1
-
36
-
-
34347204143
-
The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
-
Young, L., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., & Saxe, R. (2007). The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 104, 8235–8240. doi:10.1073/pnas.0701408104
-
(2007)
USA
, vol.104
, pp. 8235-8240
-
-
Young, L.1
Cushman, F.2
Hauser, M.3
Saxe, R.4
|