메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 39, Issue 5, 2013, Pages 1313-1325

Visual near space is scaled to parameters of current action plans

Author keywords

Action; Embodied cognition; Perception Action coupling; Spatial perception

Indexed keywords

ADULT; ANTICIPATION; ARTICLE; DISTANCE PERCEPTION; FEMALE; HAND STRENGTH; HUMAN; MALE; MOVEMENT (PHYSIOLOGY); PHYSIOLOGY; PSYCHOMOTOR PERFORMANCE; RANDOMIZATION; YOUNG ADULT;

EID: 84884972381     PISSN: 00961523     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1037/a0031074     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (32)

References (47)
  • 1
    • 0036635775 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Visual search is modulated by action intentions
    • doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2002.00466.x
    • Bekkering, H., & Neggers, S. F. W. (2002). Visual search is modulated by action intentions. Psychological Science, 13, 370-374. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2002.00466.x
    • (2002) Psychological Science , vol.13 , pp. 370-374
    • Bekkering, H.1    Neggers, S.F.W.2
  • 2
    • 0034044716 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • When far space becomes near: Remapping of space by tool use
    • doi:10.1162/089892900562237
    • Berti, A., & Frassinetti, F. (2000). When far space becomes near: Remapping of space by tool use. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 415-420. doi:10.1162/089892900562237
    • (2000) Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , vol.12 , pp. 415-420
    • Berti, A.1    Frassinetti, F.2
  • 5
    • 0034608569 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Dynamic size-change of hand peripersonal space following toll use
    • doi:10.1097/00001756-200006050-00010
    • Farnè, A., & Làdavas, E. (2000). Dynamic size-change of hand peripersonal space following toll use. NeuroReport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research, 11, 1645-1649. doi:10.1097/00001756-200006050-00010
    • (2000) NeuroReport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research , vol.11 , pp. 1645-1649
    • Farnè, A.1    Làdavas, E.2
  • 6
    • 0022417008 scopus 로고
    • The coordination of arm movements: An experimentally confirmed mathematical model
    • Flash, T., & Hogan, N. (1985). The coordination of arm movements: An experimentally confirmed mathematical model. The Journal of Neuroscience, 5, 1688-1703.
    • (1985) The Journal of Neuroscience , vol.5 , pp. 1688-1703
    • Flash, T.1    Hogan, N.2
  • 7
    • 0033760864 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Grasping visual illusions: No evidence for a dissociation between perception and action
    • doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00209
    • Franz, V. H., Gegenfurtner, K. R., Bülthoff, H. H., & Fahle, M. (2000). Grasping visual illusions: No evidence for a dissociation between perception and action. Psychological Science, 11, 20-25. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00209
    • (2000) Psychological Science , vol.11 , pp. 20-25
    • Franz, V.H.1    Gegenfurtner, K.R.2    Bülthoff, H.H.3    Fahle, M.4
  • 8
    • 43049163807 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Processing of peripersonal and extrapersonal space using tools: Evidence from visual line bisection in real and virtual environments
    • doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.016
    • Gamberini, L., Seraglia, B., & Priftis, K. (2008). Processing of peripersonal and extrapersonal space using tools: Evidence from visual line bisection in real and virtual environments. Neuropsychologia, 46, 1298-1304. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.016
    • (2008) Neuropsychologia , vol.46 , pp. 1298-1304
    • Gamberini, L.1    Seraglia, B.2    Priftis, K.3
  • 9
    • 0029926566 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Generalization to local remappings of the visuomotor coordinate transformation
    • Ghahramani, Z., Wolpert, D. M., & Jordan, M. I. (1996). Generalization to local remappings of the visuomotor coordinate transformation. The Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 7085-7096.
    • (1996) The Journal of Neuroscience , vol.16 , pp. 7085-7096
    • Ghahramani, Z.1    Wolpert, D.M.2    Jordan, M.I.3
  • 10
    • 79952390923 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Grasping preparation enhances orientation change detection
    • doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0017675
    • Gutteling, T. P., Kenemans, J. L., & Neggers, S. F. W. (2011). Grasping preparation enhances orientation change detection. PLoS ONE, 6, e17675. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0017675
    • (2011) PLoS ONE , vol.6
    • Gutteling, T.P.1    Kenemans, J.L.2    Neggers, S.F.W.3
  • 11
    • 0031929764 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The effect of pictorial illusion on prehension and perception
    • doi:10.1162/089892998563824
    • Haffenden, A. M., & Goodale, M. A. (1998). The effect of pictorial illusion on prehension and perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 122-136. doi:10.1162/089892998563824
    • (1998) Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , vol.10 , pp. 122-136
    • Haffenden, A.M.1    Goodale, M.A.2
  • 12
    • 77955303070 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Analysis of individual variations in the classical horizontal-vertical illusion
    • doi:10.3758/APP.72.4.1045
    • Hamburger, K., & Hansen, T. (2010). Analysis of individual variations in the classical horizontal-vertical illusion. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 1045-1052. doi:10.3758/APP.72.4.1045
    • (2010) Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics , vol.72 , pp. 1045-1052
    • Hamburger, K.1    Hansen, T.2
  • 13
    • 0032552114 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning
    • doi:10.1038/29528
    • Harris, C. M., & Wolpert, D. M. (1998). Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature, 394, 780-784. doi:10.1038/29528
    • (1998) Nature , vol.394 , pp. 780-784
    • Harris, C.M.1    Wolpert, D.M.2
  • 15
    • 8644222172 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Extending or projecting peripersonal space with tools? Multisensory interactions highlight only the distal and proximal ends of tools
    • doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2004.09.024
    • Holmes, N. P., Calvert, G. A., & Spence, C. (2004). Extending or projecting peripersonal space with tools? Multisensory interactions highlight only the distal and proximal ends of tools. Neuroscience Letters, 372, 62-67. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2004.09.024
    • (2004) Neuroscience Letters , vol.372 , pp. 62-67
    • Holmes, N.P.1    Calvert, G.A.2    Spence, C.3
  • 16
    • 77952293151 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Does dorsal processing require central capacity? More evidence from the PRP paradigm
    • doi:10.1007/s00221-010-2211-9
    • Janczyk, M., & Kunde, W. (2010). Does dorsal processing require central capacity? More evidence from the PRP paradigm. Experimental Brain Research, 203, 89-100. doi:10.1007/s00221-010-2211-9
    • (2010) Experimental Brain Research , vol.203 , pp. 89-100
    • Janczyk, M.1    Kunde, W.2
  • 17
    • 0022744902 scopus 로고
    • Perception of force and weight: Theory and research
    • doi:10.1037/0033-2909.100.1.29
    • Jones, L. A. (1986). Perception of force and weight: Theory and research. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 29-42. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.100.1.29
    • (1986) Psychological Bulletin , vol.100 , pp. 29-42
    • Jones, L.A.1
  • 18
    • 84860113579 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Influence of motor planning on distance perception within the peripersonal space
    • doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0034880
    • Kirsch, W., Herbort, O., Butz, M. V., & Kunde, W. (2012). Influence of motor planning on distance perception within the peripersonal space. PLoS ONE, 7, e34880. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0034880
    • (2012) PLoS ONE , vol.7
    • Kirsch, W.1    Herbort, O.2    Butz, M.V.3    Kunde, W.4
  • 19
    • 56549117714 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Action-dependent plasticity in peripersonal space representations
    • doi:10.1080/02643290802359113
    • Làdavas, E., & Serino, A. (2008). Action-dependent plasticity in peripersonal space representations. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 1099-1113. doi:10.1080/02643290802359113
    • (2008) Cognitive Neuropsychology , vol.25 , pp. 1099-1113
    • Làdavas, E.1    Serino, A.2
  • 21
    • 33644960889 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • On the nature of near space: Effects of tool use and the transition to far space
    • doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.09.003
    • Longo, M. R., & Lourenco, S. F. (2006). On the nature of near space: Effects of tool use and the transition to far space. Neuropsychologia, 44, 977-981. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.09.003
    • (2006) Neuropsychologia , vol.44 , pp. 977-981
    • Longo, M.R.1    Lourenco, S.F.2
  • 22
    • 68049129546 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The plasticity of near space: Evidence for contraction
    • doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.011
    • Lourenco, S. F., & Longo, M. R. (2009). The plasticity of near space: Evidence for contraction. Cognition, 112, 451-456. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.011
    • (2009) Cognition , vol.112 , pp. 451-456
    • Lourenco, S.F.1    Longo, M.R.2
  • 23
    • 0035795127 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Reaching with a tool extends visual-tactile interactions into far space: Evidence from cross-modal extinction
    • doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00150-0
    • Maravita, A., Husain, M., Clarke, K., & Driver, J. (2001). Reaching with a tool extends visual-tactile interactions into far space: Evidence from cross-modal extinction. Neuropsychologia, 39, 580-585. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00150-0
    • (2001) Neuropsychologia , vol.39 , pp. 580-585
    • Maravita, A.1    Husain, M.2    Clarke, K.3    Driver, J.4
  • 24
    • 0036181317 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Tool-use changes multimodal spatial interactions between vision and touch in normal humans
    • doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00003-3
    • Maravita, A., Spence, C., Kennett, S., & Driver, J. (2002). Tool-use changes multimodal spatial interactions between vision and touch in normal humans. Cognition, 83, B25-B34. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00003-3
    • (2002) Cognition , vol.83
    • Maravita, A.1    Spence, C.2    Kennett, S.3    Driver, J.4
  • 26
    • 84993729290 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Embodied perception and the economy of action
    • doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00008.x
    • Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 110-122. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00008.x
    • (2006) Perspectives on Psychological Science , vol.1 , pp. 110-122
    • Proffitt, D.R.1
  • 27
    • 73649102327 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • An action specific approach to spatial perception
    • In R. L. Klatzky, B. MacWhinney, & M. Behrmann (Eds.) Embodiment, ego-space, and action, New York, NY: Psychology Press
    • Proffitt, D. R. (2008). An action specific approach to spatial perception. In R. L. Klatzky, B. MacWhinney, & M. Behrmann (Eds.), Embodiment, ego-space, and action (pp. 177-200). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
    • (2008) , pp. 177-200
    • Proffitt, D.R.1
  • 28
    • 0038050785 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The role of effort in perceiving distance
    • doi:10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01427
    • Proffitt, D. R., Stefanucci, J., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2003). The role of effort in perceiving distance. Psychological Science, 14, 106-112. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01427
    • (2003) Psychological Science , vol.14 , pp. 106-112
    • Proffitt, D.R.1    Stefanucci, J.2    Banton, T.3    Epstein, W.4
  • 29
    • 0019247319 scopus 로고
    • Human movement initiation: Specification of arm, direction, and extent
    • doi:10.1037/0096-3445.109.4.444
    • Rosenbaum, D. A. (1980). Human movement initiation: Specification of arm, direction, and extent. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 444-474. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.109.4.444
    • (1980) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , vol.109 , pp. 444-474
    • Rosenbaum, D.A.1
  • 30
    • 59049101150 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Reaching while walking: Reaching distance costs more than walking distance
    • doi:10.3758/PBR.15.6.1100
    • Rosenbaum, D. A. (2008). Reaching while walking: Reaching distance costs more than walking distance. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 1100-1104. doi:10.3758/PBR.15.6.1100
    • (2008) Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , vol.15 , pp. 1100-1104
    • Rosenbaum, D.A.1
  • 31
    • 0002341310 scopus 로고
    • Motor theories of cognitive structure: A historical review
    • In W. Prinz & A. F. Sanders (Eds.) Cognition and motor processes, Berlin, Germany: Springer-Verlag. doi:10.1007/978-3-642-69382-3_6
    • Scheerer, E. (1984). Motor theories of cognitive structure: A historical review. In W. Prinz & A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Cognition and motor processes (pp. 77-98). Berlin, Germany: Springer-Verlag. doi:10.1007/978-3-642-69382-3_6
    • (1984) , pp. 77-98
    • Scheerer, E.1
  • 32
    • 79952063025 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Escalating slant: Increasing physiological potential does not reduce slant overestimation
    • doi:10.1177/0956797610393744
    • Shaffer, D. M., & Flint, M. (2011). Escalating slant: Increasing physiological potential does not reduce slant overestimation. Psychological Science, 22, 209-211. doi:10.1177/0956797610393744
    • (2011) Psychological Science , vol.22 , pp. 209-211
    • Shaffer, D.M.1    Flint, M.2
  • 33
    • 84864443097 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Memory for target height is scaled to observer height
    • doi:10.3758/s13421-011-0166-0
    • Twedt, E., Crawford, L. E., & Proffitt, D. R. (2012). Memory for target height is scaled to observer height. Memory & Cognition, 40, 339-351. doi:10.3758/s13421-011-0166-0
    • (2012) Memory & Cognition , vol.40 , pp. 339-351
    • Twedt, E.1    Crawford, L.E.2    Proffitt, D.R.3
  • 34
    • 0024314287 scopus 로고
    • Formation and control of optimal trajectory in human multijoint arm movement
    • doi:10.1007/BF00204593
    • Uno, Y., Kawato, M., & Suzuki, R. (1989). Formation and control of optimal trajectory in human multijoint arm movement. Biological Cybernetics, 61, 89-101. doi:10.1007/BF00204593
    • (1989) Biological Cybernetics , vol.61 , pp. 89-101
    • Uno, Y.1    Kawato, M.2    Suzuki, R.3
  • 35
    • 77956722469 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • On the perception of positions
    • In G. Aschersleben, T. Bachmann, & J. Müsseler (J. Eds.) Cognitive contributions to the perception of spatial and temporal events, Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Elsevier
    • Van der Heijden, A. H. C., Müsseler, J., & Bridgeman, B. (1999). On the perception of positions. In G. Aschersleben, T. Bachmann, & J. Müsseler (J. Eds.), Cognitive contributions to the perception of spatial and temporal events (pp. 19-37). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Elsevier.
    • (1999) , pp. 19-37
    • Van der Heijden, A.H.C.1    Müsseler, J.2    Bridgeman, B.3
  • 36
    • 34547566590 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Planning to reach for an object changes how the reacher perceives it
    • doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01965.x
    • Vishton, P. M., Stephens, N. J., Nelson, L. A., Morra, S. E., Brunick, K. L., & Stevens, J. A. (2007). Planning to reach for an object changes how the reacher perceives it. Psychological Science, 18, 713-719. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01965.x
    • (2007) Psychological Science , vol.18 , pp. 713-719
    • Vishton, P.M.1    Stephens, N.J.2    Nelson, L.A.3    Morra, S.E.4    Brunick, K.L.5    Stevens, J.A.6
  • 37
    • 0012643882 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Motor competence in the perception of dynamic events: A tutorial
    • In W. Prinz, & B. Hommel (Eds.) Common mechanisms in perception and action, Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press
    • Viviani, P. (2002). Motor competence in the perception of dynamic events: A tutorial. In W. Prinz, & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action (pp. 406-442). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
    • (2002) , pp. 406-442
    • Viviani, P.1
  • 38
    • 79957806138 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Tool use influences perceived shape and perceived parallelism, which serve as indirect measure of perceived distance
    • doi:10.1037/a0021933
    • Witt, J. K. (2011a). Tool use influences perceived shape and perceived parallelism, which serve as indirect measure of perceived distance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1148-1156. doi:10.1037/a0021933
    • (2011) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.37 , pp. 1148-1156
    • Witt, J.K.1
  • 39
    • 79957795355 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Action's effect on perception
    • doi:10.1177/0963721411408770
    • Witt, J. K. (2011b). Action's effect on perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 201-206. doi:10.1177/0963721411408770
    • (2011) Current Directions in Psychological Science , vol.20 , pp. 201-206
    • Witt, J.K.1
  • 40
    • 76549106060 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Kicking to bigger uprights: Field goal kicking performance influences perceived size
    • doi:10.1068/p6325
    • Witt, J. K., & Dorsch, T. E. (2009). Kicking to bigger uprights: Field goal kicking performance influences perceived size. Perception, 38, 1328-1340. doi:10.1068/p6325
    • (2009) Perception , vol.38 , pp. 1328-1340
    • Witt, J.K.1    Dorsch, T.E.2
  • 41
    • 50949133085 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Putting to a bigger hole: Golf performance relates to perceived size
    • doi:10.3758/PBR.15.3.581
    • Witt, J. K., Linkenauger, S. A., Bakdash, J. Z., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). Putting to a bigger hole: Golf performance relates to perceived size. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 581-585. doi:10.3758/PBR.15.3.581
    • (2008) Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , vol.15 , pp. 581-585
    • Witt, J.K.1    Linkenauger, S.A.2    Bakdash, J.Z.3    Proffitt, D.R.4
  • 42
    • 30344487556 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • See the ball, hit the ball: Apparent ball size is correlated with batting average
    • doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01640.x
    • Witt, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2005). See the ball, hit the ball: Apparent ball size is correlated with batting average. Psychological Science, 16, 937-938. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01640.x
    • (2005) Psychological Science , vol.16 , pp. 937-938
    • Witt, J.K.1    Proffitt, D.R.2
  • 43
    • 57749192754 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Action-specific influences on distance perception: A role for motor simulation
    • doi: 10.1037/a0010781
    • Witt, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). Action-specific influences on distance perception: A role for motor simulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1479-1492. doi: 10.1037/a0010781
    • (2008) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.34 , pp. 1479-1492
    • Witt, J.K.1    Proffitt, D.R.2
  • 44
    • 3042736094 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Perceiving distance: A role of effort and intent
    • doi:10.1068/p5090
    • Witt, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., & Epstein, W. (2004). Perceiving distance: A role of effort and intent. Perception, 33, 577-590. doi:10.1068/p5090
    • (2004) Perception , vol.33 , pp. 577-590
    • Witt, J.K.1    Proffitt, D.R.2    Epstein, W.3
  • 47
    • 69049083419 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The various perceptions of distance: An alternative view of how effort affects distance judgments
    • doi:10.1037/a0013622
    • Woods, A. J., Philbeck, J. W., Danoff, J. V. (2009). The various perceptions of distance: An alternative view of how effort affects distance judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1104-1117. doi:10.1037/a0013622
    • (2009) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.35 , pp. 1104-1117
    • Woods, A.J.1    Philbeck, J.W.2    Danoff, J.V.3


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.