메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 39, Issue 1, 2013, Pages 85-95

Do stimulus-action associations contribute to repetition priming?

Author keywords

Event files; Instances; Repetition priming; Response learning; Stimulus specificity

Indexed keywords

ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE; ARTICLE; ASSOCIATION; ATTENTION; FEMALE; HEMISPHERIC DOMINANCE; HUMAN; LINGUISTICS; MALE; PHOTOSTIMULATION; PHYSIOLOGY; PSYCHOMOTOR PERFORMANCE; REACTION TIME; REPETITION PRIMING; STUDENT; UNIVERSITY;

EID: 84875500373     PISSN: 02787393     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1037/a0028479     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (19)

References (32)
  • 1
    • 0036516982 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Subliminal words activate semantic categories (not automated motor responses)
    • Abrams, R. L., Klinger, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Subliminal words activate semantic categories (not automated motor responses). Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 100-106.
    • (2002) Psychonomic Bulletin and Review , vol.9 , pp. 100-106
    • Abrams, R.L.1    Klinger, M.R.2    Greenwald, A.G.3
  • 3
    • 85047681794 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Congruity effects evoked by subliminally presented primes: Automaticity rather than semantic processing
    • Damian, M. F. (2001). Congruity effects evoked by subliminally presented primes: Automaticity rather than semantic processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 154- 165.
    • (2001) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.27 , pp. 154-165
    • Damian, M.F.1
  • 4
    • 65749104080 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Perceive-decide-act, perceive-decide-act: How abstract is repetition-related decision learning?
    • doi:10.1037/a0015263
    • Denkinger, B., & Koutstaal, W. (2009). Perceive-decide-act, perceive-decide-act: How abstract is repetition-related decision learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 742-756. doi:10.1037/a0015263
    • (2009) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , vol.35 , pp. 742-756
    • Denkinger, B.1    Koutstaal, W.2
  • 5
    • 72149106199 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Sizing up the associative account of repetition priming
    • doi: 10.1007/s00426-008-0224-9
    • Dennis, I., Carder, H., & Perfect, T. J. (2010). Sizing up the associative account of repetition priming. Psychological Research, 74, 35-49. doi: 10.1007/s00426-008-0224-9
    • (2010) Psychological Research , vol.74 , pp. 35-49
    • Dennis, I.1    Carder, H.2    Perfect, T.J.3
  • 7
    • 1642633104 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Cortical activity reductions during repetition priming can result from rapid response learning
    • March 18, doi:10.1038/ nature02400
    • Dobbins, I. G., Schnyer, D. M., Verfaellie, M., & Schacter, D. L. (2004, March 18). Cortical activity reductions during repetition priming can result from rapid response learning. Nature, 428, 316-319. doi:10.1038/nature02400
    • (2004) Nature , vol.428 , pp. 316-319
    • Dobbins, I.G.1    Schnyer, D.M.2    Verfaellie, M.3    Schacter, D.L.4
  • 8
    • 44149128191 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • When do motor behaviors (mis-)match affective stimuli? An evaluative coding view of approach and avoidance reactions
    • doi:10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.262
    • Eder, A. B., & Rothermund, K. (2008). When do motor behaviors (mis-)match affective stimuli? An evaluative coding view of approach and avoidance reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 262-281. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.262
    • (2008) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , vol.137 , pp. 262-281
    • Eder, A.B.1    Rothermund, K.2
  • 9
    • 0036582250 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Task switching: A PDP model
    • doi:10.1006/cogp.2001.0770
    • Gilbert, S. J., & Shallice, T. (2002). Task switching: A PDP model. Cognitive Psychology, 44, 297-337. doi:10.1006/cogp.2001.0770
    • (2002) Cognitive Psychology , vol.44 , pp. 297-337
    • Gilbert, S.J.1    Shallice, T.2
  • 10
    • 0000240437 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Event files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus-response episodes
    • doi:10.1080/713756773
    • Hommel, B. (1998). Event files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus-response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5, 183-216. doi:10.1080/ 713756773
    • (1998) Visual Cognition , vol.5 , pp. 183-216
    • Hommel, B.1
  • 11
    • 6344251612 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action
    • doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.08.007
    • Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 494-500. doi:10.1016/ j.tics.2004.08.007
    • (2004) Trends in Cognitive Sciences , vol.8 , pp. 494-500
    • Hommel, B.1
  • 12
    • 65749083359 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Bindings between stimuli and multiple response codes dominate long-lag repetition priming in speeded classification tasks
    • doi:10.1037/a0015262
    • Horner, A. J., & Henson, R. N. (2009). Bindings between stimuli and multiple response codes dominate long-lag repetition priming in speeded classification tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 757-779. doi:10.1037/a0015262
    • (2009) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , vol.35 , pp. 757-779
    • Horner, A.J.1    Henson, R.N.2
  • 13
    • 33846964556 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Task switching: On the origins of response congruency effects
    • doi:10.1007/s00426-005-0004-8
    • Kiesel, A., Wendt, M., & Peters, A. (2007). Task switching: On the origins of response congruency effects. Psychological Research, 71, 117-125. doi:10.1007/s00426-005-0004-8
    • (2007) Psychological Research , vol.71 , pp. 117-125
    • Kiesel, A.1    Wendt, M.2    Peters, A.3
  • 14
    • 0001296920 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Putting frequencies in the dictionary
    • doi:10.1093/ijl/10.2.135
    • Kilgarriff, A. (1997). Putting frequencies in the dictionary. International Journal of Lexicography, 10, 135-155. doi:10.1093/ijl/10.2.135
    • (1997) International Journal of Lexicography , vol.10 , pp. 135-155
    • Kilgarriff, A.1
  • 15
    • 33745648800 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Cue-based preparation and stimulus-based priming of tasks in task switching
    • doi:10.3758/BF03193420
    • Koch, I., & Allport, A. (2006). Cue-based preparation and stimulus-based priming of tasks in task switching. Memory & Cognition, 34, 433-444. doi:10.3758/BF03193420
    • (2006) Memory & Cognition , vol.34 , pp. 433-444
    • Koch, I.1    Allport, A.2
  • 16
    • 79955864438 scopus 로고
    • Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs
    • doi:10.3758/BF03210951
    • Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. J. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 476-490. doi:10.3758/BF03210951
    • (1994) Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , vol.1 , pp. 476-490
    • Loftus, G.R.1    Masson, M.E.J.2
  • 17
    • 1842646010 scopus 로고
    • Towards an instance theory of automatization
    • doi:10.1037/0033-295X.95.4.492
    • Logan, G. D. (1988). Towards an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492-527. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.95.4.492
    • (1988) Psychological Review , vol.95 , pp. 492-527
    • Logan, G.D.1
  • 18
    • 38249019407 scopus 로고
    • Repetition priming and automaticity: Common underlying mechanisms
    • doi:10.1016/ 0010-0285(90)90002-L
    • Logan, G. D. (1990). Repetition priming and automaticity: Common underlying mechanisms. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 1-35. doi:10.1016/ 0010-0285(90)90002-L
    • (1990) Cognitive Psychology , vol.22 , pp. 1-35
    • Logan, G.D.1
  • 19
    • 39449094580 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The task rule congruency effect in task switching reflects activated long-term memory
    • doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.1.137
    • Meiran, N., & Kessler, Y. (2008). The task rule congruency effect in task switching reflects activated long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 137-157. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.1.137
    • (2008) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.34 , pp. 137-157
    • Meiran, N.1    Kessler, Y.2
  • 20
    • 33748743195 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Neural response suppression predicts repetition priming of spoken words and pseudowords
    • doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.8.1237
    • Orfanidou, E., Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Davis, M. H. (2006). Neural response suppression predicts repetition priming of spoken words and pseudowords. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1237-1252. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.8.1237
    • (2006) Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , vol.18 , pp. 1237-1252
    • Orfanidou, E.1    Marslen-Wilson, W.D.2    Davis, M.H.3
  • 21
    • 0029301378 scopus 로고
    • On the use of counterbalanced designs in cognitive research: A suggestion for better and more powerful analysis
    • doi:10.1037/0278-7393.21.3.785
    • Pollatsek, A., & Well, A. D. (1995). On the use of counterbalanced designs in cognitive research: A suggestion for better and more powerful analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 785-794. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.21.3.785
    • (1995) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , vol.21 , pp. 785-794
    • Pollatsek, A.1    Well, A.D.2
  • 22
    • 21844510093 scopus 로고
    • Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks
    • doi:10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.207
    • Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207-231. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.207
    • (1995) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , vol.124 , pp. 207-231
    • Rogers, R.D.1    Monsell, S.2
  • 24
    • 75549087894 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Behavioral, neuroimaging, and computational evidence for perceptual caching in repetition priming
    • doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2009.11.074
    • Saggar, M., Miikkulainen, R.,&Schnyer, D. S. (2010). Behavioral, neuroimaging, and computational evidence for perceptual caching in repetition priming. Brain Research, 1315, 75-91. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2009.11.074
    • (2010) Brain Research , vol.1315 , pp. 75-91
    • Saggar, M.1    Miikkulainen, R.2    Schnyer, D.S.3
  • 25
  • 26
    • 28044437167 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Rapid response learning in amnesia: Delineating associative learning components in repetition priming
    • doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.03.027
    • Schnyer, D. M., Dobbins, I. G., Nicholls, L., Schacter, D. L., & Verfaellie, M. (2006). Rapid response learning in amnesia: Delineating associative learning components in repetition priming. Neuropsychologia, 44, 140- 149. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.03.027
    • (2006) Neuropsychologia , vol.44 , pp. 140-149
    • Schnyer, D.M.1    Dobbins, I.G.2    Nicholls, L.3    Schacter, D.L.4    Verfaellie, M.5
  • 30
    • 0038457034 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Task-switching and longterm priming: Role of episodic stimulus-task bindings in task-shift costs
    • doi:10.1016/S0010- 0285(02)00520-0
    • Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, A. (2003). Task-switching and longterm priming: Role of episodic stimulus-task bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 361-413. doi:10.1016/S0010- 0285(02)00520-0
    • (2003) Cognitive Psychology , vol.46 , pp. 361-413
    • Waszak, F.1    Hommel, B.2    Allport, A.3
  • 31
    • 43649093830 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The impact of stimulus-specific practice and task instructions on response congruency effects between tasks
    • doi:10.1007/s00426-007-0117-3
    • Wendt, M., & Kiesel, A. (2008). The impact of stimulus-specific practice and task instructions on response congruency effects between tasks. Psychological Research, 72, 425-432. doi:10.1007/s00426-007-0117-3
    • (2008) Psychological Research , vol.72 , pp. 425-432
    • Wendt, M.1    Kiesel, A.2
  • 32


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.