메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 142, Issue 4, 2013, Pages 1028-1046

The cost and benefit of implicit spatial cues for visual attention

Author keywords

Conceptual metaphor; Embodied semantics; Theory of event coding; Visual attention

Indexed keywords

ADULT; ARTICLE; ASSOCIATION; ATTENTION; DEPTH PERCEPTION; HUMAN; PHYSIOLOGY; REACTION TIME; VISION;

EID: 84874520124     PISSN: 00963445     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1037/a0030362     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (45)

References (71)
  • 2
    • 41549109070 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Grounded cognition
    • doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639
    • Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617-645. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639
    • (2008) Annual Review of Psychology , vol.59 , pp. 617-645
    • Barsalou, L.W.1
  • 3
    • 0037312096 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems
    • doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00029-3
    • Barsalou, L. W., Simmons, W. K., Barbey, A. K., & Wilson, C. D. (2003). Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 84-91. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00029-3
    • (2003) Trends in Cognitive Sciences , vol.7 , pp. 84-91
    • Barsalou, L.W.1    Simmons, W.K.2    Barbey, A.K.3    Wilson, C.D.4
  • 4
    • 84887963808 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Mental simulation in spatial language processing
    • In Proceedings of the 27th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Retrieved from
    • Bergen, B. (2005). Mental simulation in spatial language processing. In Proceedings of the 27th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Retrieved from http://csjarchive.cogsci.rpi.edu/Proceedings/2005/docs/p232.pdf
    • (2005)
    • Bergen, B.1
  • 5
    • 85190103142 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Experimental methods for simulation semantics
    • In M. Gonzalez-Marquez, I. Mittleberg, S. Coulson, & M. Spivey (Eds.), Methods in cognitive linguistics, Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Benjamins
    • Bergen, B. (2007). Experimental methods for simulation semantics. In M. Gonzalez-Marquez, I. Mittleberg, S. Coulson, & M. Spivey (Eds.), Methods in cognitive linguistics (pp. 277-301). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Benjamins.
    • (2007) , pp. 277-301
    • Bergen, B.1
  • 6
    • 35649025828 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Spatial and linguistic aspects of visual imagery in sentence comprehension
    • doi:10.1080/03640210701530748
    • Bergen, B. K., Lindsay, S., Matlock, T., & Narayanan, S. (2007). Spatial and linguistic aspects of visual imagery in sentence comprehension. Cognitive Science, 31, 733-764. doi:10.1080/03640210701530748
    • (2007) Cognitive Science , vol.31 , pp. 733-764
    • Bergen, B.K.1    Lindsay, S.2    Matlock, T.3    Narayanan, S.4
  • 7
    • 33749432299 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Cross-talk between language processes and overt motor behavior in the first 200 msec of processing
    • doi:10.1162/jocn.2006.18.10.1607
    • Boulenger, V., Roy, A. C., Paulignan, Y., Deprez, V., Jeannerod, M., & Nazir, T. A. (2006). Cross-talk between language processes and overt motor behavior in the first 200 msec of processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1607-1615. doi:10.1162/jocn.2006.18.10.1607
    • (2006) Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , vol.18 , pp. 1607-1615
    • Boulenger, V.1    Roy, A.C.2    Paulignan, Y.3    Deprez, V.4    Jeannerod, M.5    Nazir, T.A.6
  • 8
    • 0030612822 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The Psychophysics Toolbox
    • Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433-436.
    • (1997) Spatial Vision , vol.10 , pp. 433-436
    • Brainard, D.H.1
  • 9
    • 72549097633 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Thinking of god moves attention
    • doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.029
    • Chasteen, A. L., Burdzy, D. C., & Pratt, J. (2010). Thinking of god moves attention. Neuropsychologia, 48, 627-630. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.029
    • (2010) Neuropsychologia , vol.48 , pp. 627-630
    • Chasteen, A.L.1    Burdzy, D.C.2    Pratt, J.3
  • 10
    • 0035253393 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Language and space: Some interactions
    • doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01598-9
    • Chatterjee, A. (2001). Language and space: Some interactions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 55-61. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01598-9
    • (2001) Trends in Cognitive Sciences , vol.5 , pp. 55-61
    • Chatterjee, A.1
  • 11
    • 77956652193 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Sentence processing: Linking language to motor chains
    • doi:10.3389/fnbot.2010.00004
    • Chersi, F., Thill, S., Ziemke, T., & Borghi, A. M. (2010). Sentence processing: Linking language to motor chains. Frontiers in Neurorobotics, 4, 1-8. doi:10.3389/fnbot.2010.00004
    • (2010) Frontiers in Neurorobotics , vol.4 , pp. 1-8
    • Chersi, F.1    Thill, S.2    Ziemke, T.3    Borghi, A.M.4
  • 12
    • 0043280846 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Stimulus and response representations underlying orthogonal stimulus-response compatibility effects
    • doi:10.3758/BF03196468
    • Cho, Y. S., & Proctor, R. W. (2003). Stimulus and response representations underlying orthogonal stimulus-response compatibility effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 45-73. doi:10.3758/BF03196468
    • (2003) Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , vol.10 , pp. 45-73
    • Cho, Y.S.1    Proctor, R.W.2
  • 13
    • 33746635923 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A common representation for semantic and physical properties: A cognitive-anatomical approach
    • doi:10.1027/1618-3169.53.2.87
    • Cohen Kadosh, R., & Henik, A. (2006). A common representation for semantic and physical properties: A cognitive-anatomical approach. Experimental Psychology, 53, 87-94. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.53.2.87
    • (2006) Experimental Psychology , vol.53 , pp. 87-94
    • Cohen Kadosh, R.1    Henik, A.2
  • 14
    • 0032980044 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Spatial influences on motor and language function
    • doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00116-X
    • Coslett, H. B. (1999). Spatial influences on motor and language function. Neuropsychologia, 37, 695-706. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00116-X
    • (1999) Neuropsychologia , vol.37 , pp. 695-706
    • Coslett, H.B.1
  • 15
    • 33746890027 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson's method
    • Cousineau, D. (2007). Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson's method. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 1, 42-45.
    • (2007) Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology , vol.1 , pp. 42-45
    • Cousineau, D.1
  • 16
    • 79951527548 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Visual object detection, categorization, and identification tasks are associated with different time courses and sensitivities
    • doi:10.1037/a0020553
    • de la Rosa, S., Choudhery, R. N., & Chatziastros, A. (2011). Visual object detection, categorization, and identification tasks are associated with different time courses and sensitivities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 38-47. doi:10.1037/a0020553
    • (2011) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.37 , pp. 38-47
    • De La Rosa, S.1    Choudhery, R.N.2    Chatziastros, A.3
  • 19
    • 34548313699 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Common valence coding in action and evaluation: Affective blindness towards response-compatible stimuli
    • doi:10.1080/02699930701438277
    • Eder, A. B., & Klauer, K. C. (2007). Common valence coding in action and evaluation: Affective blindness towards response-compatible stimuli. Cognition & Emotion, 21, 1297-1322. doi:10.1080/02699930701438277
    • (2007) Cognition & Emotion , vol.21 , pp. 1297-1322
    • Eder, A.B.1    Klauer, K.C.2
  • 20
    • 38949155877 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Head up, foot down: Object words orient attention to the objects' typical location
    • doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02051.x
    • Estes, Z., Verges, M., & Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Head up, foot down: Object words orient attention to the objects' typical location. Psychological Science, 19, 93-97. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02051.x
    • (2008) Psychological Science , vol.19 , pp. 93-97
    • Estes, Z.1    Verges, M.2    Barsalou, L.W.3
  • 21
    • 0038022572 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention
    • doi:10.1038/nn1066
    • Fischer, M. H., Castel, A. D., Dodd, M. D., & Pratt, J. (2003). Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 555-556. doi:10.1038/nn1066
    • (2003) Nature Neuroscience , vol.6 , pp. 555-556
    • Fischer, M.H.1    Castel, A.D.2    Dodd, M.D.3    Pratt, J.4
  • 22
    • 33646787130 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Automatic response activation of implicit spatial information: Evidence from the SNARC effect
    • doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.11.004
    • Gevers, W., Lammertyn, J., Notebaert, W., Verguts, T., & Fias, W. (2006). Automatic response activation of implicit spatial information: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Acta Psychologica, 122, 221-233. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.11.004
    • (2006) Acta Psychologica , vol.122 , pp. 221-233
    • Gevers, W.1    Lammertyn, J.2    Notebaert, W.3    Verguts, T.4    Fias, W.5
  • 23
    • 81755161429 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Seeing while acting: Hand movements can modulate attentional capture by motion onset
    • doi:10.3758/s13414-011-0203-x
    • Gozli, D. G., & Pratt, J. (2011). Seeing while acting: Hand movements can modulate attentional capture by motion onset. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 2448-2456. doi:10.3758/s13414-011-0203-x
    • (2011) Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics , vol.73 , pp. 2448-2456
    • Gozli, D.G.1    Pratt, J.2
  • 24
    • 6344251612 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action
    • doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.08.007
    • Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: Feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 494-500. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.08.007
    • (2004) Trends in Cognitive Sciences , vol.8 , pp. 494-500
    • Hommel, B.1
  • 25
    • 0035495020 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning
    • doi:10.1017/S0140525X01000103
    • Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849-878. doi:10.1017/S0140525X01000103
    • (2001) Behavioral and Brain Sciences , vol.24 , pp. 849-878
    • Hommel, B.1    Müsseler, J.2    Aschersleben, G.3    Prinz, W.4
  • 26
    • 0035459708 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Symbolic control of visual attention
    • doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00367
    • Hommel, B., Pratt, J., Colzato, L., & Godijn, R. (2001). Symbolic control of visual attention. Psychological Science, 12, 360-365. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00367
    • (2001) Psychological Science , vol.12 , pp. 360-365
    • Hommel, B.1    Pratt, J.2    Colzato, L.3    Godijn, R.4
  • 27
    • 20044363089 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex
    • doi:10.1038/nrn1684
    • Hubbard, E. M., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Dehaene, S. (2005). Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 435-448. doi:10.1038/nrn1684
    • (2005) Nature Reviews Neuroscience , vol.6 , pp. 435-448
    • Hubbard, E.M.1    Piazza, M.2    Pinel, P.3    Dehaene, S.4
  • 28
    • 84970382083 scopus 로고
    • Chronometric evidence for two types of attention
    • doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00527.x
    • Johnston, J. C., McCann, R. S., & Remington, R. W. (1995). Chronometric evidence for two types of attention. Psychological Science, 6, 365-369. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00527.x
    • (1995) Psychological Science , vol.6 , pp. 365-369
    • Johnston, J.C.1    McCann, R.S.2    Remington, R.W.3
  • 29
    • 0025410809 scopus 로고
    • Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility-A model and taxonomy
    • Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility-A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253-270.
    • (1990) Psychological Review , vol.97 , pp. 253-270
    • Kornblum, S.1    Hasbroucq, T.2    Osman, A.3
  • 30
    • 0034485060 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • On the locus of the semantic satiation effect: Evidence from event-related brain potentials
    • Kounios, J., Kotz, S. A., & Holocomb, P. J. (2000). On the locus of the semantic satiation effect: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Memory & Cognition, 28, 1366-1377.
    • (2000) Memory & Cognition , vol.28 , pp. 1366-1377
    • Kounios, J.1    Kotz, S.A.2    Holocomb, P.J.3
  • 32
    • 16544381078 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Actions blind to conceptually overlapping stimuli
    • Kunde, W., & Wühr, P. (2004). Actions blind to conceptually overlapping stimuli. Psychological Research, 68, 199-207.
    • (2004) Psychological Research , vol.68 , pp. 199-207
    • Kunde, W.1    Wühr, P.2
  • 33
    • 84867120061 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • High skies and oceans deep: Polarity benefits or mental simulation?
    • Article 21. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00021
    • Lakens, D. (2011). High skies and oceans deep: Polarity benefits or mental simulation? Frontiers in Cognition, 2, Article 21. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00021
    • (2011) Frontiers in Cognition , vol.2
    • Lakens, D.1
  • 34
    • 84865694065 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Polarity correspondence in metaphor congruency effects: Structural overlap predicts categorization times for bipolar concepts presented in vertical space
    • doi:10.1037/a0024955
    • Lakens, D. (2012). Polarity correspondence in metaphor congruency effects: Structural overlap predicts categorization times for bipolar concepts presented in vertical space. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 726-736. doi:10.1037/a0024955
    • (2012) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , vol.38 , pp. 726-736
    • Lakens, D.1
  • 35
    • 84872142027 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • But for the bad, there would not be good: Grounding valence in brightness through shared relational structure
    • doi:10.1037/a0026468
    • Lakens, D., Semin, G. R., & Foroni, F. (2012). But for the bad, there would not be good: Grounding valence in brightness through shared relational structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 584-594. doi:10.1037/a0026468
    • (2012) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , vol.141 , pp. 584-594
    • Lakens, D.1    Semin, G.R.2    Foroni, F.3
  • 36
    • 67349156473 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The neural theory of metaphor
    • In J. R. W. Gibbs (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought, Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press
    • Lakoff, G. (2008). The neural theory of metaphor. In J. R. W. Gibbs (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought (pp. 17-38). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
    • (2008) , pp. 17-38
    • Lakoff, G.1
  • 37
    • 84867119914 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Stormy seas and cloudy skies: Conceptual processing is (still) linguistic and perceptual
    • Louwerse, M. M. (2011). Stormy seas and cloudy skies: Conceptual processing is (still) linguistic and perceptual. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 150.
    • (2011) Frontiers in Psychology , vol.2 , pp. 150
    • Louwerse, M.M.1
  • 38
    • 2042506346 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Why the sunny side is up: Associations between affect and vertical position
    • doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00659.x
    • Meier, B. P., & Robinson, M. D. (2004). Why the sunny side is up: Associations between affect and vertical position. Psychological Science, 15, 243-247. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00659.x
    • (2004) Psychological Science , vol.15 , pp. 243-247
    • Meier, B.P.1    Robinson, M.D.2
  • 39
    • 85050839351 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The metaphorical representation of affect
    • doi:10.1207/s15327868ms2004_1
    • Meier, B. P., & Robinson, M. D. (2005). The metaphorical representation of affect. Metaphor and Symbol, 20, 239-257. doi:10.1207/s15327868ms2004_1
    • (2005) Metaphor and Symbol , vol.20 , pp. 239-257
    • Meier, B.P.1    Robinson, M.D.2
  • 40
    • 77956724705 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • How independent from action control is perception? An event-coding account for more equally-ranked crosstalks
    • In G. Aschersleben, T. Bachmann, & J. Müsseler (Eds.), Cognitive contributions to the perception of spatial and temporal events, Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Elsevier
    • Müsseler, J. (1999). How independent from action control is perception? An event-coding account for more equally-ranked crosstalks. In G. Aschersleben, T. Bachmann, & J. Müsseler (Eds.), Cognitive contributions to the perception of spatial and temporal events (pp. 121-147). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Elsevier.
    • (1999) , pp. 121-147
    • Müsseler, J.1
  • 42
    • 82455174950 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Numbers in space: Differences between concrete and abstract situations
    • Article 121. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00121
    • Pecher, D., & Boot, I. (2011). Numbers in space: Differences between concrete and abstract situations. Frontiers in Cognition, 2, Article 121. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00121
    • (2011) Frontiers in Cognition , vol.2
    • Pecher, D.1    Boot, I.2
  • 43
    • 79951503872 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Abstract concepts: Sensorymotor grounding, metaphors, and beyond
    • In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, Burlington, VT: Academic Press
    • Pecher, D., Boot, I., & Van Dantzig, S. (2011). Abstract concepts: Sensorymotor grounding, metaphors, and beyond. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 217-248). Burlington, VT: Academic Press.
    • (2011) , pp. 217-248
    • Pecher, D.1    Boot, I.2    Van Dantzig, S.3
  • 44
    • 84866765377 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Congruency between word position and meaning is caused by taskinduced spatial attention
    • Article 30. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00030
    • Pecher, D., Van Dantzig, S., Boot, I., Zanolie, K., & Huber, D. E. (2010). Congruency between word position and meaning is caused by taskinduced spatial attention. Frontiers in Cognition, 1, Article 30. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00030
    • (2010) Frontiers in Cognition , pp. 1
    • Pecher, D.1    Van Dantzig, S.2    Boot, I.3    Zanolie, K.4    Huber, D.E.5
  • 45
    • 0030612832 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The Video Toolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies
    • Pelli, D. G. (1997). The Video Toolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437-442.
    • (1997) Spatial Vision , vol.10 , pp. 437-442
    • Pelli, D.G.1
  • 47
    • 0002442636 scopus 로고
    • Components of visual orienting
    • In H., Bouma & D. G. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and performance 10: Control of language processes, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
    • Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H., Bouma & D. G. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and performance 10: Control of language processes (pp. 531-556). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
    • (1984) , pp. 531-556
    • Posner, M.I.1    Cohen, Y.2
  • 48
    • 33744947076 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Polarity correspondence: A general principle for performance of speeded binary classification tasks
    • doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.416
    • Proctor, R. W., & Cho, Y. S. (2006). Polarity correspondence: A general principle for performance of speeded binary classification tasks. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 416-442. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.416
    • (2006) Psychological Bulletin , vol.132 , pp. 416-442
    • Proctor, R.W.1    Cho, Y.S.2
  • 49
    • 84860390183 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Puddles, parties, and professors: Linking word categorization to neural patterns of visuospatial coding
    • doi:10.1162/jocn .2011.21628
    • Quadflieg, S., Etzel, J. A., Gazzola, V., Keysers, C., Schubert, T. W., Waiter, G. D., & Macrae, C. N. (2011). Puddles, parties, and professors: Linking word categorization to neural patterns of visuospatial coding. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 2636-2649. doi:10.1162/jocn .2011.21628
    • (2011) Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , vol.23 , pp. 2636-2649
    • Quadflieg, S.1    Etzel, J.A.2    Gazzola, V.3    Keysers, C.4    Schubert, T.W.5    Waiter, G.D.6    Macrae, C.N.7
  • 50
    • 0035514668 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A dissociation between attention and selection
    • doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00394
    • Remington, R. W., & Folk, C. L. (2001). A dissociation between attention and selection. Psychological Science, 12, 511-515. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00394
    • (2001) Psychological Science , vol.12 , pp. 511-515
    • Remington, R.W.1    Folk, C.L.2
  • 51
    • 0141532966 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Spatial representations activated during real-time comprehension of verbs
    • doi:10.1207/s15516709cog2705_4
    • Richardson, D. C., Spivey, M. J., Barsalou, L. W., & McRae, K. (2003). Spatial representations activated during real-time comprehension of verbs. Cognitive Science, 27, 767-780. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog2705_4
    • (2003) Cognitive Science , vol.27 , pp. 767-780
    • Richardson, D.C.1    Spivey, M.J.2    Barsalou, L.W.3    McRae, K.4
  • 52
    • 44249115320 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The SNARC effect does not imply a mental number line
    • doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.01.002
    • Santens, S., & Gevers, W. (2008). The SNARC effect does not imply a mental number line. Cognition, 108, 263-270. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.01.002
    • (2008) Cognition , vol.108 , pp. 263-270
    • Santens, S.1    Gevers, W.2
  • 53
    • 84864347534 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Attentional factors in conceptual congruency
    • doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2012.01240.x
    • Santiago, J., Ouellet, M., Román, A., & Valenzuela, J. (2012). Attentional factors in conceptual congruency. Cognitive Science, 36, 1051-1077. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2012.01240.x
    • (2012) Cognitive Science , vol.36 , pp. 1051-1077
    • Santiago, J.1    Ouellet, M.2    Román, A.3    Valenzuela, J.4
  • 54
    • 42249099995 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Task related modulation of the motor system during language processing
    • doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2007.10.001
    • Sato, M., Mengarelli, M., Riggio, L., Gallese, V., & Buccino, G. (2008). Task related modulation of the motor system during language processing. Brain and Language, 105, 83-90. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2007.10.001
    • (2008) Brain and Language , vol.105 , pp. 83-90
    • Sato, M.1    Mengarelli, M.2    Riggio, L.3    Gallese, V.4    Buccino, G.5
  • 55
    • 23844475419 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Your highness: Vertical positions as perceptual symbols of power
    • doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.1.1
    • Schubert, T. W. (2005). Your highness: Vertical positions as perceptual symbols of power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 1-21. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.1.1
    • (2005) Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , vol.89 , pp. 1-21
    • Schubert, T.W.1
  • 56
    • 4544278533 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Moving the eyes along the mental number line: Comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses
    • doi:10.3758/BF03194909
    • Schwarz, W., & Keus, I. M. (2004). Moving the eyes along the mental number line: Comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 651-664. doi:10.3758/BF03194909
    • (2004) Perception & Psychophysics , vol.66 , pp. 651-664
    • Schwarz, W.1    Keus, I.M.2
  • 57
    • 33847659976 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The influence of vertical spatial orientation on property verification
    • doi:10.1080/01690960600732430
    • Šetić, M., & Domijan, D. (2007). The influence of vertical spatial orientation on property verification. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22, 297-312. doi:10.1080/01690960600732430
    • (2007) Language and Cognitive Processes , vol.22 , pp. 297-312
    • Šetić, M.1    Domijan, D.2
  • 60
    • 0036615237 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Eye gaze is not unique: Automatic orienting in response to uninformative arrows
    • doi:10.3758/BF03196287
    • Tipples, J. (2002). Eye gaze is not unique: Automatic orienting in response to uninformative arrows. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 314-318. doi:10.3758/BF03196287
    • (2002) Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , vol.9 , pp. 314-318
    • Tipples, J.1
  • 61
    • 33748868660 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Flexible conceptual projection of time onto spatial frames of reference
    • doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0000_67
    • Torralbo, A., Santiago, J., & Lupiáñez, J. (2006). Flexible conceptual projection of time onto spatial frames of reference. Cognitive Science, 30, 745-757. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0000_67
    • (2006) Cognitive Science , vol.30 , pp. 745-757
    • Torralbo, A.1    Santiago, J.2    Lupiáñez, J.3
  • 62
    • 84867130969 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Spatial attention is driven by mental simulations
    • Van Dantzig, S., & Pecher, D. (2011). Spatial attention is driven by mental simulations. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 40.
    • (2011) Frontiers in Psychology , vol.2 , pp. 40
    • Van Dantzig, S.1    Pecher, D.2
  • 63
    • 0027719331 scopus 로고
    • The role of position in object selection in vision
    • doi:10.1007/BF00572132
    • van der Heijden, A. H. C. (1993). The role of position in object selection in vision. Psychological Research, 56, 44-58. doi:10.1007/BF00572132
    • (1993) Psychological Research , vol.56 , pp. 44-58
    • Van Der Heijden, A.H.C.1
  • 64
    • 0142216244 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity
    • doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.09.002
    • Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 483-488. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.09.002
    • (2003) Trends in Cognitive Sciences , vol.7 , pp. 483-488
    • Walsh, V.1
  • 65
    • 78349244097 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Neural representation of concrete concepts: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies
    • doi:10.1002/hbm .20950
    • Wang, J., Conder, J. A., Blitzer, D. N., & Shinkareva, S. V. (2010). Neural representation of concrete concepts: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 31, 1459-1468. doi:10.1002/hbm .20950
    • (2010) Human Brain Mapping , vol.31 , pp. 1459-1468
    • Wang, J.1    Conder, J.A.2    Blitzer, D.N.3    Shinkareva, S.V.4
  • 66
    • 27844461549 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Content differences for abstract and concrete concepts
    • doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0000_33
    • Wiemer-Hastings, K., & Xu, X. (2005). Content differences for abstract and concrete concepts. Cognitive Science, 29, 719-736. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0000_33
    • (2005) Cognitive Science , vol.29 , pp. 719-736
    • Wiemer-Hastings, K.1    Xu, X.2
  • 67
    • 82455164105 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Mighty metaphors: Behavioral and ERP evidence that power shifts attention on a vertical dimension
    • doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2011.10.006
    • Zanolie, K., van Dantzig, S., Boot, I., Wijnen, J., Schubert, T. W., Giessner, S. R., & Pecher, D. (2012). Mighty metaphors: Behavioral and ERP evidence that power shifts attention on a vertical dimension. Brain and Cognition, 78, 50-58. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2011.10.006
    • (2012) Brain and Cognition , vol.78 , pp. 50-58
    • Zanolie, K.1    Van Dantzig, S.2    Boot, I.3    Wijnen, J.4    Schubert, T.W.5    Giessner, S.R.6    Pecher, D.7
  • 68
    • 71549128726 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Experiential traces and mental simulations in language comprehension
    • In M. DeVega, A. M. Glenberg, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Symbols and embodiment, Oxford, England: Oxford University Press
    • Zwaan, R. A. (2008). Experiential traces and mental simulations in language comprehension. In M. DeVega, A. M. Glenberg, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Symbols and embodiment (pp. 165-180). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
    • (2008) , pp. 165-180
    • Zwaan, R.A.1
  • 69
    • 1842787557 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Spatial iconicity affects semanticrelatedness judgments
    • doi:10.3758/BF03196557
    • Zwaan, R. A., & Yaxley, R. H. (2003). Spatial iconicity affects semanticrelatedness judgments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 954-958. doi:10.3758/BF03196557
    • (2003) Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , vol.10 , pp. 954-958
    • Zwaan, R.A.1    Yaxley, R.H.2
  • 70
    • 77950357063 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • On interference effects in concurrent perception and action
    • doi:10.1007/s00426-009-0226-2
    • Zwickel, J., Grosjean, M., & Prinz, W. (2010). On interference effects in concurrent perception and action. Psychological Research, 74, 152-171. doi:10.1007/s00426-009-0226-2
    • (2010) Psychological Research , vol.74 , pp. 152-171
    • Zwickel, J.1    Grosjean, M.2    Prinz, W.3
  • 71
    • 84857053221 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Assimilation and contrast: The two sides of specific interference between action and perception
    • doi:10.1007/s00426-011-0338-3
    • Zwickel, J., & Prinz, W. (2012). Assimilation and contrast: The two sides of specific interference between action and perception. Psychological Research, 76, 171-182. doi:10.1007/s00426-011-0338-3
    • (2012) Psychological Research , vol.76 , pp. 171-182
    • Zwickel, J.1    Prinz, W.2


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.