메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 138, Issue 4, 2012, Pages 589-615

The experience of force: The role of haptic experience of forces in visual perception of object motion and interactions, mental simulation, and motion-related judgments

Author keywords

Force perception; Haptics; Mental simulation; Perceived object motion; Representational momentum

Indexed keywords

BIOMECHANICS; CONCEPT FORMATION; DECISION MAKING; GRAVITY; HUMAN; MECHANORECEPTOR; MEMORY; MOTOR PERFORMANCE; MOVEMENT PERCEPTION; PATTERN RECOGNITION; PHYSIOLOGY; PSYCHOLOGICAL MODEL; PSYCHOPHYSICS; REVIEW; TOUCH; VISION;

EID: 84867307398     PISSN: 00332909     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1037/a0025587     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (60)

References (200)
  • 1
    • 0003927905 scopus 로고
    • Art and visual perception: A psychology of the creative eye
    • London, England: Faber & Faber
    • Arnheim, R. (1956). Art and visual perception: A psychology of the creative eye. London, England: Faber & Faber.
    • (1956)
    • Arnheim, R.1
  • 2
    • 0037322859 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Importance of cutaneous feedback in maintaining a secure grip during manipulation of hand-held objects
    • doi:10.1152/jn.00249.2002
    • Augurelle, A.-S., Smith, A. M., Lejeune, T., & Thonnard, J.-L. (2003). Importance of cutaneous feedback in maintaining a secure grip during manipulation of hand-held objects. Journal of Neurophysiology, 89, 665-671. doi:10.1152/jn.00249.2002
    • (2003) Journal of Neurophysiology , vol.89 , pp. 665-671
    • Augurelle, A.-S.1    Smith, A.M.2    Lejeune, T.3    Thonnard, J.-L.4
  • 3
    • 41549109070 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Grounded cognition
    • doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639
    • Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617-645. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639
    • (2008) Annual Review of Psychology , vol.59 , pp. 617-645
    • Barsalou, L.W.1
  • 4
    • 34247365389 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Infants' perception of goal-directed actions: Development through cue-based bootstrapping
    • doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00544.x
    • Biro, S., & Leslie, A. M. (2007). Infants' perception of goal-directed actions: Development through cue-based bootstrapping. Developmental Science, 10, 379-398. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00544.x
    • (2007) Developmental Science , vol.10 , pp. 379-398
    • Biro, S.1    Leslie, A.M.2
  • 6
    • 0035434054 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • From the perception of action to the understanding of intention
    • Blakemore, S.-J., & Decety, J. (2001). From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 561-567.
    • (2001) Nature Reviews Neuroscience , vol.2 , pp. 561-567
    • Blakemore, S.-J.1    Decety, J.2
  • 7
    • 0032736316 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Spatio-temporal prediction modulates the perception of self-produced stimuli
    • doi:10.1162/089892999563607
    • Blakemore, S.-J., Frith, C. D., & Wolpert, D. M. (1999). Spatio-temporal prediction modulates the perception of self-produced stimuli. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 551-559. doi:10.1162/089892999563607
    • (1999) Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , vol.11 , pp. 551-559
    • Blakemore, S.-J.1    Frith, C.D.2    Wolpert, D.M.3
  • 8
    • 0036606476 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Abnormalities in the awareness of action
    • doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01907-1
    • Blakemore, S.-J., Wolpert, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2002). Abnormalities in the awareness of action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 237-242. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01907-1
    • (2002) Trends in Cognitive Sciences , vol.6 , pp. 237-242
    • Blakemore, S.-J.1    Wolpert, D.M.2    Frith, C.D.3
  • 9
    • 70350780450 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Predictive mechanisms control grip force after impact in self-triggered perturbations
    • doi:10.3200/35-08-084
    • Bleyenheuft, Y., Lefevre, P., & Thonnard, J. L. (2009). Predictive mechanisms control grip force after impact in self-triggered perturbations. Journal of Motor Behavior, 41, 411-417. doi:10.3200/35-08-084
    • (2009) Journal of Motor Behavior , vol.41 , pp. 411-417
    • Bleyenheuft, Y.1    Lefevre, P.2    Thonnard, J.L.3
  • 10
    • 27744522410 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Inferring another's expectation from action: The role of peripheral sensation
    • doi:10.1038/nn1535
    • Bosbach, S., Cole, J., Prinz, W., & Knoblich, G. (2005). Inferring another's expectation from action: The role of peripheral sensation. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1295-1297. doi:10.1038/nn1535
    • (2005) Nature Neuroscience , vol.8 , pp. 1295-1297
    • Bosbach, S.1    Cole, J.2    Prinz, W.3    Knoblich, G.4
  • 11
    • 0027648811 scopus 로고
    • Motor development and the mind: The potential role of motor abilities as a determinant of aspects of perceptual development
    • doi: 10.2307/1131323
    • Bushnell, E. W., & Boudreau, J. P. (1993). Motor development and the mind: The potential role of motor abilities as a determinant of aspects of perceptual development. Child Development, 64, 1005-1021. doi: 10.2307/1131323
    • (1993) Child Development , vol.64 , pp. 1005-1021
    • Bushnell, E.W.1    Boudreau, J.P.2
  • 12
    • 0026872013 scopus 로고
    • Haptic probing: Perceiving the length of a probe and the distance of the surface probed
    • doi:10.3758/ BF03211655
    • Carello, C., Fitzpatrick, P., & Turvey, M. T. (1992). Haptic probing: Perceiving the length of a probe and the distance of the surface probed. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 51, 580-598. doi:10.3758/ BF03211655
    • (1992) Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics , vol.51 , pp. 580-598
    • Carello, C.1    Fitzpatrick, P.2    Turvey, M.T.3
  • 13
    • 1642281555 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Physics and psychology of the muscle sense
    • doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01301007.x
    • Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (2004). Physics and psychology of the muscle sense. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 25-28. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01301007.x
    • (2004) Current Directions in Psychological Science , vol.13 , pp. 25-28
    • Carello, C.1    Turvey, M.T.2
  • 14
    • 0037088854 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Central and peripheral mediation of human force sensation following eccentric or concentric contractions
    • doi:10.1113/ jphysiol.2001.013385
    • Carson, R. G., Riek, S., & Shahbazpour, N. (2002). Central and peripheral mediation of human force sensation following eccentric or concentric contractions. Journal of Physiology, 539, 913-925. doi:10.1113/ jphysiol.2001.013385
    • (2002) Journal of Physiology , vol.539 , pp. 913-925
    • Carson, R.G.1    Riek, S.2    Shahbazpour, N.3
  • 15
    • 30044434143 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Nonvisual motor training influences biological motion perception
    • doi:10.1016/ j.cub.2005.10.071
    • Casile, A., & Gliese, M. A. (2006). Nonvisual motor training influences biological motion perception. Current Biology, 16, 69-74. doi:10.1016/ j.cub.2005.10.071
    • (2006) Current Biology , vol.16 , pp. 69-74
    • Casile, A.1    Gliese, M.A.2
  • 16
    • 54849409678 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Producing and processing self-propelled motion in infancy
    • doi:10.1037/a0012619
    • Cicchino, J. B., & Rakison, D. H. (2008). Producing and processing self-propelled motion in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1232-1241. doi:10.1037/a0012619
    • (2008) Developmental Psychology , vol.44 , pp. 1232-1241
    • Cicchino, J.B.1    Rakison, D.H.2
  • 17
    • 33746934142 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Contribution of invariants, heuristics, and exemplars to the perception of relative mass
    • doi:10.1037/0096-1523.32.3.574
    • Cohen, A. L. (2006). Contribution of invariants, heuristics, and exemplars to the perception of relative mass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 574-598. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.32.3.574
    • (2006) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.32 , pp. 574-598
    • Cohen, A.L.1
  • 18
    • 0026504014 scopus 로고
    • The perceptions of force and of movement in a man without large myelinated sensory afferents below the neck
    • Cole, J. D., & Sedgwick, E. M. (1992). The perceptions of force and of movement in a man without large myelinated sensory afferents below the neck. Journal of Physiology, 449, 503-515.
    • (1992) Journal of Physiology , vol.449 , pp. 503-515
    • Cole, J.D.1    Sedgwick, E.M.2
  • 19
    • 84874301283 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Energy, the subtle concept: The discovery of Feynman's blocks from Leibniz to Einstein
    • Oxford, England: Oxford University Press
    • Coopersmith, J. (2010). Energy, the subtle concept: The discovery of Feynman's blocks from Leibniz to Einstein. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
    • (2010)
    • Coopersmith, J.1
  • 20
    • 0032616597 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Somesthesis
    • doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.305
    • Craig, J. C. (1999). Somesthesis. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 305-331. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.305
    • (1999) Annual Review of Psychology , vol.50 , pp. 305-331
    • Craig, J.C.1
  • 21
    • 41349101303 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Goal attribution to inanimate agents by 6.5-month-old infants
    • doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.08.001
    • Csibra, G. (2008). Goal attribution to inanimate agents by 6.5-month-old infants. Cognition, 107, 705-717. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.08.001
    • (2008) Cognition , vol.107 , pp. 705-717
    • Csibra, G.1
  • 22
    • 0002664199 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Is there such a thing as functional equivalence between imagined, observed, and executed action?
    • In A Meltzoff & W. Prinz (Eds.), (pp ). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511489969.017
    • Decety, J. (2002). Is there such a thing as functional equivalence between imagined, observed, and executed action? In A. Meltzoff & W. Prinz (Eds.), The imitative mind: Development, evolution, and brain bases (pp. 291-310). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511489969.017
    • (2002) The imitative mind: Development, evolution, and brain bases , pp. 291-310
    • Decety, J.1
  • 23
    • 33645069410 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The power of simulation: Imagining one's own and other's behavior
    • doi:10.1016/ j.brainres.2005.12.115
    • Decety, J., & Grezès, J. (2006). The power of simulation: Imagining one's own and other's behavior. Brain Research, 1079, 4-14. doi:10.1016/ j.brainres.2005.12.115
    • (2006) Brain Research , vol.1079 , pp. 4-14
    • Decety, J.1    Grezès, J.2
  • 24
    • 1842557578 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Awareness of muscular force during movement production: An fMRI study
    • doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.009
    • de Graaf, J. B., Galléa, C., Pailhous, J., Anton, J.-L., Roth, M., & Bonnard, M. (2004). Awareness of muscular force during movement production: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 21, 1357-1367. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.009
    • (2004) NeuroImage , vol.21 , pp. 1357-1367
    • de Graaf, J.B.1    Galléa, C.2    Pailhous, J.3    Anton, J.-L.4    Roth, M.5    Bonnard, M.6
  • 25
    • 0242381877 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Efficiency of grip force adjustments for impulsive loading during imposed and actively produced collisions
    • doi:10.1080/02724980245000025
    • Delevoye-Turrell, Y. N., Li, F.-X., & Wing, A. M. (2003). Efficiency of grip force adjustments for impulsive loading during imposed and actively produced collisions. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 56, 1113-1128. doi:10.1080/02724980245000025
    • (2003) The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A , vol.56 , pp. 1113-1128
    • Delevoye-Turrell, Y.N.1    Li, F.-X.2    Wing, A.M.3
  • 26
    • 70349404208 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A parietal-premotor network for movement intention and motor awareness
    • doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.08.001
    • Desmurget, M., & Sirigu, A. (2009). A parietal-premotor network for movement intention and motor awareness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 411-419. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.08.001
    • (2009) Trends in Cognitive Sciences , vol.13 , pp. 411-419
    • Desmurget, M.1    Sirigu, A.2
  • 27
    • 0031008196 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The relationship between curvature and velocity in two-dimensional smooth pursuit eye movements
    • de'Sperati, C., & Viviani, P. (1997). The relationship between curvature and velocity in two-dimensional smooth pursuit eye movements. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 3932-3945.
    • (1997) The Journal of Neuroscience , vol.17 , pp. 3932-3945
    • de'Sperati, C.1    Viviani, P.2
  • 28
    • 34548156255 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Somatosensory processes subserving perception and action
    • doi:10.1017/S0140525X07001392
    • Dijkerman, H. C., & de Haan, E. H. F. (2007). Somatosensory processes subserving perception and action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 189-201. doi:10.1017/S0140525X07001392
    • (2007) Behavioral and Brain Sciences , vol.30 , pp. 189-201
    • Dijkerman, H.C.1    de Haan, E.H.F.2
  • 29
    • 84455175167 scopus 로고
    • Toward an epistemology of physics [Special issue]
    • diSessa, A. A. (1993). Toward an epistemology of physics [Special issue]. Cognition and Instruction, 10, 105-225.
    • (1993) Cognition and Instruction , vol.10 , pp. 105-225
    • diSessa, A.A.1
  • 30
    • 0028249589 scopus 로고
    • Visual perception of intentional motion
    • doi:10.1068/p230253
    • Dittrich, W. H., & Lea, S. E. G. (1994). Visual perception of intentional motion. Perception, 23, 253-268. doi:10.1068/p230253
    • (1994) Perception , vol.23 , pp. 253-268
    • Dittrich, W.H.1    Lea, S.E.G.2
  • 31
    • 65549091538 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Amelioration of axis-aligned motion bias for active versus stationary judgments of bilaterally symmetric moving shapes' final destinations
    • doi: 10.3758/APP.71.3.523
    • Dolgov, I., Birchfield, D. A., McBeath, M. K., Thornburg, H., & Todd, C. G. (2009). Amelioration of axis-aligned motion bias for active versus stationary judgments of bilaterally symmetric moving shapes' final destinations. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 523-529. doi: 10.3758/APP.71.3.523
    • (2009) Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics , vol.71 , pp. 523-529
    • Dolgov, I.1    Birchfield, D.A.2    McBeath, M.K.3    Thornburg, H.4    Todd, C.G.5
  • 32
    • 74949092644 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Evidence for axis-aligned motion bias: Football axis-trajectory misalignment causes systematic error in projected final destinations of thrown American footballs
    • doi:10.1068/p6114
    • Dolgov, I., McBeath, M. K., & Sugar, T. (2009). Evidence for axis-aligned motion bias: Football axis-trajectory misalignment causes systematic error in projected final destinations of thrown American footballs. Perception, 38, 399-410. doi:10.1068/p6114
    • (2009) Perception , vol.38 , pp. 399-410
    • Dolgov, I.1    McBeath, M.K.2    Sugar, T.3
  • 33
    • 0022740156 scopus 로고
    • Implied velocity and acceleration induce transformations of visual memory
    • doi:10.1037/0096-3445.115.2.175
    • Finke, R. A., Freyd, J. J., & Shyi, G. C.-W. (1986). Implied velocity and acceleration induce transformations of visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 175-188. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.115.2.175
    • (1986) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , vol.115 , pp. 175-188
    • Finke, R.A.1    Freyd, J.J.2    Shyi, G.C.-W.3
  • 34
    • 2342499959 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The two-thirds power law in motion perception
    • doi:10.1080/ 13506280344000392
    • Flach, R., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2004). The two-thirds power law in motion perception. Visual Cognition, 11, 461-481. doi:10.1080/ 13506280344000392
    • (2004) Visual Cognition , vol.11 , pp. 461-481
    • Flach, R.1    Knoblich, G.2    Prinz, W.3
  • 35
    • 0031017178 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The role of internal models in planning and control: Evidence from grip force adjustments during movements of hand-held loads
    • Flanagan, J. R., & Wing, A. M. (1997). The role of internal models in planning and control: Evidence from grip force adjustments during movements of hand-held loads. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 1519-1528.
    • (1997) Journal of Neuroscience , vol.17 , pp. 1519-1528
    • Flanagan, J.R.1    Wing, A.M.2
  • 36
    • 84918922935 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • June. [Reply to question posted on The Last Word web page]
    • Follows, M. (2010, June). [Reply to question posted on The Last Word web page]. New Scientist. Retrieved from http://www.last-word.com/content_handling/show_tree/tree_id/1962.html
    • (2010) New Scientist
    • Follows, M.1
  • 37
    • 0031769761 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Limited conscious monitoring of motor performance in normal subjects
    • doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00006-2
    • Fourneret, P., & Jeannerod, M. (1998). Limited conscious monitoring of motor performance in normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 36, 1133-1140. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00006-2
    • (1998) Neuropsychologia , vol.36 , pp. 1133-1140
    • Fourneret, P.1    Jeannerod, M.2
  • 38
    • 0023433056 scopus 로고
    • Dynamic mental representations
    • doi:10.1037/0033-295X.94.4.427
    • Freyd, J. J. (1987). Dynamic mental representations. Psychological Review, 94, 427-438. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.94.4.427
    • (1987) Psychological Review , vol.94 , pp. 427-438
    • Freyd, J.J.1
  • 41
  • 42
    • 0036909538 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Attention to action and awareness of other minds
    • doi:10.1016/S1053-8100(02) 00022-3
    • Frith, C. (2002). Attention to action and awareness of other minds. Consciousness and Cognition, 11, 481-487. doi:10.1016/S1053-8100(02) 00022-3
    • (2002) Consciousness and Cognition , vol.11 , pp. 481-487
    • Frith, C.1
  • 43
    • 0028418736 scopus 로고
    • Recognition memory for movement in photographs: A developmental study
    • doi:10.1006/jecp.1994.1008
    • Futterweit, L. R., & Beilin, H. (1994). Recognition memory for movement in photographs: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 57, 163-179. doi:10.1006/jecp.1994.1008
    • (1994) Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , vol.57 , pp. 163-179
    • Futterweit, L.R.1    Beilin, H.2
  • 44
    • 0034417023 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Counterfactuals as behavioral primes: Priming the simulation heuristic and consideration of alternatives
    • doi: 10.1006/jesp.1999.1409
    • Galinsky, A. D., & Moskowitz, G. B. (2000). Counterfactuals as behavioral primes: Priming the simulation heuristic and consideration of alternatives. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 384-409. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1999.1409
    • (2000) Journal of Experimental Social Psychology , vol.36 , pp. 384-409
    • Galinsky, A.D.1    Moskowitz, G.B.2
  • 45
    • 0001133019 scopus 로고
    • Body image and body schema in a deafferented subject
    • Gallagher, S., & Cole, J. D. (1995). Body image and body schema in a deafferented subject. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 16, 369-390.
    • (1995) Journal of Mind and Behavior , vol.16 , pp. 369-390
    • Gallagher, S.1    Cole, J.D.2
  • 46
    • 0000878294 scopus 로고
    • Does the nervous system depend on kinesthetic information to control natural limb movements?
    • Gandevia, S. C., & Burke, D. (1992). Does the nervous system depend on kinesthetic information to control natural limb movements? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15, 614-632.
    • (1992) Behavioral and Brain Sciences , vol.15 , pp. 614-632
    • Gandevia, S.C.1    Burke, D.2
  • 47
    • 0014117353 scopus 로고
    • Muscle sense in man
    • doi:10.1016/0014-4886(67)90064-7
    • Gelfan, S., & Carter, S. (1967). Muscle sense in man. Experimental Neurology, 18, 469-473. doi:10.1016/0014-4886(67)90064-7
    • (1967) Experimental Neurology , vol.18 , pp. 469-473
    • Gelfan, S.1    Carter, S.2
  • 48
    • 25844519812 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Force estimation processing as a function of the input-output relationship of the corticospinal pathway in humans
    • doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04287.x
    • Gelli, F., Del Santo, R., Mazzocchio, R., & Rossi, A. (2005). Force estimation processing as a function of the input-output relationship of the corticospinal pathway in humans. European Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 1127-1134. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04287.x
    • (2005) European Journal of Neuroscience , vol.22 , pp. 1127-1134
    • Gelli, F.1    Del Santo, R.2    Mazzocchio, R.3    Rossi, A.4
  • 49
    • 0002450105 scopus 로고
    • First principles organize attention to and learning about relevant data: Number and the animate-inanimate distinction as examples
    • doi:10.1207/s15516709cog1401_5
    • Gelman, R. (1990). First principles organize attention to and learning about relevant data: Number and the animate-inanimate distinction as examples. Cognitive Science, 14, 79-106. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog1401_5
    • (1990) Cognitive Science , vol.14 , pp. 79-106
    • Gelman, R.1
  • 50
    • 0002295857 scopus 로고
    • Distinguishing between animates and inanimates: Not by motion alone
    • D. Sperber, D. Premack, & A. J. Premack (Eds.), Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.
    • Gelman, R., Durgin, F., & Kaufman, L. (1995). Distinguishing between animates and inanimates: Not by motion alone. In D. Sperber, D. Premack, & A. J. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate (pp. 150-184). Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.
    • (1995) Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate , pp. 150-184
    • Gelman, R.1    Durgin, F.2    Kaufman, L.3
  • 51
    • 58149447475 scopus 로고
    • Observations on active touch
    • doi:10.1037/h0046962
    • Gibson, J. J. (1962). Observations on active touch. Psychological Review, 69, 477-491. doi:10.1037/h0046962
    • (1962) Psychological Review , vol.69 , pp. 477-491
    • Gibson, J.J.1
  • 52
    • 0003896940 scopus 로고
    • The senses considered as perceptual systems
    • Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin
    • Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
    • (1966)
    • Gibson, J.J.1
  • 53
    • 0026231259 scopus 로고
    • On the origins of dynamical awareness
    • Gilden, D. L. (1991). On the origins of dynamical awareness. Psychological Review, 98, 554-568.
    • (1991) Psychological Review , vol.98 , pp. 554-568
    • Gilden, D.L.1
  • 55
    • 0015453948 scopus 로고
    • The contribution of muscle afferents to kinesthesia shown by vibration induced illusions of movement and by the effects of paralyzing joint afferents
    • doi:10.1093/brain/95.4.705
    • Goodwin, G. M., McCloskey, D. I., & Matthews, P. B. (1972). The contribution of muscle afferents to kinesthesia shown by vibration induced illusions of movement and by the effects of paralyzing joint afferents. Brain, 95, 705-748. doi:10.1093/brain/95.4.705
    • (1972) Brain , vol.95 , pp. 705-748
    • Goodwin, G.M.1    McCloskey, D.I.2    Matthews, P.B.3
  • 56
    • 0025793306 scopus 로고
    • Integration of sensory information during the programming of precision grip: Comments on the contribution of size cues
    • doi:10.1007/BF00230004
    • Gordon, A. M., Forssberg, H., Johansson, R. S., & Westling, G. (1991). Integration of sensory information during the programming of precision grip: Comments on the contribution of size cues. Experimental Brain Research, 85, 226-229. doi:10.1007/BF00230004
    • (1991) Experimental Brain Research , vol.85 , pp. 226-229
    • Gordon, A.M.1    Forssberg, H.2    Johansson, R.S.3    Westling, G.4
  • 57
    • 0025309714 scopus 로고
    • Perceived causality occurs with stroboscopic movement of one or both stimulus elements
    • doi:10.1068/p190017
    • Gordon, I. E., Day, R. H., & Stecher, E. J. (1990). Perceived causality occurs with stroboscopic movement of one or both stimulus elements. Perception, 19, 17-20. doi:10.1068/p190017
    • (1990) Perception , vol.19 , pp. 17-20
    • Gordon, I.E.1    Day, R.H.2    Stecher, E.J.3
  • 59
    • 77249122422 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Motor adaptation and manual transfer: Insight into the persistent nature of sensorimotor representations
    • doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2009.11.006
    • Green, S., Grierson, L. E. M., Dubrowski, A., & Carnahan, H. (2010). Motor adaptation and manual transfer: Insight into the persistent nature of sensorimotor representations. Brain and Cognition, 72, 385-393. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2009.11.006
    • (2010) Brain and Cognition , vol.72 , pp. 385-393
    • Green, S.1    Grierson, L.E.M.2    Dubrowski, A.3    Carnahan, H.4
  • 61
    • 19544371934 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Conscious intention and motor cognition
    • doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.04.012
    • Haggard, P. (2005). Conscious intention and motor cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 290-295. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.04.012
    • (2005) Trends in Cognitive Sciences , vol.9 , pp. 290-295
    • Haggard, P.1
  • 62
    • 71549157761 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Exemplar similarity and rule application
    • doi:10.1016/ j.cognition.2009.08.011
    • Hahn, U., Prat-Sala, M., Pothos, E. M., & Brumby, D. P. (2010). Exemplar similarity and rule application. Cognition, 114, 1-18. doi:10.1016/ j.cognition.2009.08.011
    • (2010) Cognition , vol.114 , pp. 1-18
    • Hahn, U.1    Prat-Sala, M.2    Pothos, E.M.3    Brumby, D.P.4
  • 63
    • 1842535187 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Your own action influences how you perceive another person's action
    • doi:10.1016/j.cub.2004.03.007
    • Hamilton, A., Wolpert, D., & Frith, U. (2004). Your own action influences how you perceive another person's action. Current Biology, 14, 493-498. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2004.03.007
    • (2004) Current Biology , vol.14 , pp. 493-498
    • Hamilton, A.1    Wolpert, D.2    Frith, U.3
  • 64
    • 0002005030 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Heuristics and invariants in dynamic event perception: Immunized concepts or nonstatements?
    • Hecht, H. (1996). Heuristics and invariants in dynamic event perception: Immunized concepts or nonstatements? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3, 61-70.
    • (1996) Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , vol.3 , pp. 61-70
    • Hecht, H.1
  • 65
    • 0035736213 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Regularities of the physical world and the absence of their internalization
    • doi: 10.1017/S0140525X01000036
    • Hecht, H. (2001a). Regularities of the physical world and the absence of their internalization. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 608-617. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X01000036
    • (2001) Behavioral and Brain Sciences , vol.24 , pp. 608-617
    • Hecht, H.1
  • 66
    • 0035736110 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Universal internalization or pluralistic micro-theories?
    • doi:10.1017/ S0140525X0175008X
    • Hecht, H. (2001b). Universal internalization or pluralistic micro-theories? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 749-755. doi:10.1017/ S0140525X0175008X
    • (2001) Behavioral and Brain Sciences , vol.24 , pp. 749-755
    • Hecht, H.1
  • 68
    • 0035232644 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Motor learning enhances perceptual judgment: A case for action-perception transfer
    • doi:10.1007/s004260000043
    • Hecht, H., Vogt, S., & Prinz, W. (2001). Motor learning enhances perceptual judgment: A case for action-perception transfer. Psychological Research, 65, 3-14. doi:10.1007/s004260000043
    • (2001) Psychological Research , vol.65 , pp. 3-14
    • Hecht, H.1    Vogt, S.2    Prinz, W.3
  • 69
    • 0000942804 scopus 로고
    • An experimental study of apparent behavior
    • doi: 10.2307/1416950
    • Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. The American Journal of Psychology, 57, 243-249. doi: 10.2307/1416950
    • (1944) The American Journal of Psychology , vol.57 , pp. 243-249
    • Heider, F.1    Simmel, M.2
  • 71
    • 43149090309 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Grip force control of predictable external loads
    • doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1195-6
    • Hermsdörfer, J., & Blankenfeld, H. (2008). Grip force control of predictable external loads. Experimental Brain Research, 185, 719-728. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1195-6
    • (2008) Experimental Brain Research , vol.185 , pp. 719-728
    • Hermsdörfer, J.1    Blankenfeld, H.2
  • 72
    • 0042406666 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Weight perception by newborns of depressed versus non-depressed mothers
    • doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(01)00081-9
    • Hernandez-Reif, M., Field, T., Diego, M., & Largie, S. (2001). Weight perception by newborns of depressed versus non-depressed mothers. Infant Behavior and Development, 24, 305-316. doi:10.1016/S0163-6383(01)00081-9
    • (2001) Infant Behavior and Development , vol.24 , pp. 305-316
    • Hernandez-Reif, M.1    Field, T.2    Diego, M.3    Largie, S.4
  • 73
    • 77955579037 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Selecting explanations from causal chains: Do statistical principles explain preferences for voluntary causes?
    • doi:10.1002/ejsp.623
    • Hilton, D. J., McClure, J., & Sutton, R. M. (2010). Selecting explanations from causal chains: Do statistical principles explain preferences for voluntary causes? European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 383-400. doi:10.1002/ejsp.623
    • (2010) European Journal of Social Psychology , vol.40 , pp. 383-400
    • Hilton, D.J.1    McClure, J.2    Sutton, R.M.3
  • 74
    • 0343408338 scopus 로고
    • "Schema abstraction" in a multiple-trace memory model
    • doi:10.1037/0033-295X.93.4.411
    • Hintzman, D. L. (1986). "Schema abstraction" in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychological Review, 93, 411-428. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.93.4.411
    • (1986) Psychological Review , vol.93 , pp. 411-428
    • Hintzman, D.L.1
  • 75
    • 0035495020 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning
    • doi:10.1017/ S0140525X01000103
    • Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849-878. doi:10.1017/ S0140525X01000103
    • (2001) Behavioral and Brain Sciences , vol.24 , pp. 849-878
    • Hommel, B.1    Müsseler, J.2    Aschersleben, G.3    Prinz, W.4
  • 76
    • 33644760382 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Representational momentum and related displacements in spatial memory: A review of the findings
    • doi:10.3758/BF03196775
    • Hubbard, T. L. (2005). Representational momentum and related displacements in spatial memory: A review of the findings. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 822-851. doi:10.3758/BF03196775
    • (2005) Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , vol.12 , pp. 822-851
    • Hubbard, T.L.1
  • 77
    • 33645689400 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Bridging the gap: Possible roles and contributions of representational momentum
    • Hubbard, T. L. (2006). Bridging the gap: Possible roles and contributions of representational momentum. Psicológica, 27, 1-34.
    • (2006) Psicológica , vol.27 , pp. 1-34
    • Hubbard, T.L.1
  • 79
    • 0036160881 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A possible role of naïve impetus in Michotte's "launching effect" evidence from representational momentum
    • doi:10.1080/13506280143000377
    • Hubbard, T. L., & Ruppel, S. E. (2002). A possible role of naïve impetus in Michotte's "launching effect" evidence from representational momentum. Visual Cognition, 9, 153-176. doi:10.1080/13506280143000377
    • (2002) Visual Cognition , vol.9 , pp. 153-176
    • Hubbard, T.L.1    Ruppel, S.E.2
  • 80
    • 2642528414 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • When is the ball going to hit the ground? Duration estimates, eye movements, and mental imagery of object motion
    • doi:10.1037/0096-1523.30.3.431
    • Huber, S., & Krist, H. (2004). When is the ball going to hit the ground? Duration estimates, eye movements, and mental imagery of object motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 431-444. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.30.3.431
    • (2004) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.30 , pp. 431-444
    • Huber, S.1    Krist, H.2
  • 81
    • 77950521222 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Multiple grasp-specific representations of tool dynamics mediate skillful manipulation
    • doi:10.1016/ j.cub.2010.01.054
    • Ingram, J. N., Howard, I. S., Flanagan, J. R., & Wolpert, D. M. (2010). Multiple grasp-specific representations of tool dynamics mediate skillful manipulation. Current Biology, 20, 618-623. doi:10.1016/ j.cub.2010.01.054
    • (2010) Current Biology , vol.20 , pp. 618-623
    • Ingram, J.N.1    Howard, I.S.2    Flanagan, J.R.3    Wolpert, D.M.4
  • 82
    • 0037007999 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Two-thirds power law in human locomotion: Role of ground contact forces
    • doi:10.1097/00001756-200207020-00020
    • Ivanenko, Y. P., Grasso, R., Macellari, V., & Lacquaniti, F. (2002). Two-thirds power law in human locomotion: Role of ground contact forces. NeuroReport, 13, 1171-1174. doi:10.1097/00001756-200207020-00020
    • (2002) NeuroReport , vol.13 , pp. 1171-1174
    • Ivanenko, Y.P.1    Grasso, R.2    Macellari, V.3    Lacquaniti, F.4
  • 83
    • 67349146792 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Coding and use of tactile signals from the fingertips in object manipulation tasks
    • doi:10.1038/nrn2621
    • Johansson, R. S., & Flanagan, J. R. (2009). Coding and use of tactile signals from the fingertips in object manipulation tasks. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 345-359. doi:10.1038/nrn2621
    • (2009) Nature Reviews Neuroscience , vol.10 , pp. 345-359
    • Johansson, R.S.1    Flanagan, J.R.2
  • 85
    • 28644438586 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Contribution of tactile feedback from the hand to the perception of force
    • doi:10.1007/s00221-005-0259-8
    • Jones, L. A., & Piateski, E. (2006). Contribution of tactile feedback from the hand to the perception of force. Experimental Brain Research, 168, 298-302. doi:10.1007/s00221-005-0259-8
    • (2006) Experimental Brain Research , vol.168 , pp. 298-302
    • Jones, L.A.1    Piateski, E.2
  • 86
    • 39449086171 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Learned patterns of action-effect anticipation contribute to the spatial displacement of continuously moving stimuli
    • doi:10.1037/0096-1523.34.1.113
    • Jordan, J. S., & Hunsinger, M. (2008). Learned patterns of action-effect anticipation contribute to the spatial displacement of continuously moving stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 113-124. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.34.1.113
    • (2008) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.34 , pp. 113-124
    • Jordan, J.S.1    Hunsinger, M.2
  • 87
    • 22644446873 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Exploration of the newborn's manual activity: A window into early cognitive processes
    • doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2005.05.001
    • Jouen, F., & Molina, M. (2005). Exploration of the newborn's manual activity: A window into early cognitive processes. Infant Behavior and Development, 28, 227-239. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2005.05.001
    • (2005) Infant Behavior and Development , vol.28 , pp. 227-239
    • Jouen, F.1    Molina, M.2
  • 88
    • 0002554835 scopus 로고
    • The simulation heuristic
    • D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.), Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
    • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982). The simulation heuristic. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 201-208). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
    • (1982) Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases , pp. 201-208
    • Kahneman, D.1    Tversky, A.2
  • 89
    • 0039765601 scopus 로고
    • La percezione della reazione intenzionale [The perception of intentional reaction]
    • G. Kanizsa & G. Vicario (Eds.), Trieste, Italy: University of Trieste Press.
    • Kanizsa, G., & Vicario, G. (1968). La percezione della reazione intenzionale [The perception of intentional reaction]. In G. Kanizsa & G. Vicario (Eds.), Ricerche Sperimentali sulla Percezione (pp. 71-126). Trieste, Italy: University of Trieste Press.
    • (1968) Ricerche Sperimentali sulla Percezione , pp. 71-126
    • Kanizsa, G.1    Vicario, G.2
  • 90
    • 1842484841 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Which mechanical invariants are associated with the perception of length and heaviness of a nonvisible handheld rod? Testing the inertia tensor hypothesis
    • doi:10.1037/0096-1523.30.2.346
    • Kingma, I., van de Langenberg, R., & Beek, P. J. (2004). Which mechanical invariants are associated with the perception of length and heaviness of a nonvisible handheld rod? Testing the inertia tensor hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 346-354. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.30.2.346
    • (2004) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.30 , pp. 346-354
    • Kingma, I.1    van de Langenberg, R.2    Beek, P.J.3
  • 91
    • 0036637005 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Authorship effects in the prediction of handwriting strokes: Evidence for action simulation during action perception
    • doi:10.1080/ 02724980143000631
    • Knoblich, G., Seigerschmidt, E., Flach, R., & Prinz, W. (2002). Authorship effects in the prediction of handwriting strokes: Evidence for action simulation during action perception. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 55, 1027-1046. doi:10.1080/ 02724980143000631
    • (2002) The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A , vol.55 , pp. 1027-1046
    • Knoblich, G.1    Seigerschmidt, E.2    Flach, R.3    Prinz, W.4
  • 92
    • 0035460628 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Impetus beliefs as default heuristics: Dissociation between explicit and implicit knowledge about motion
    • doi:10.3758/ BF03196179
    • Kozhevnikov, M., & Hegarty, M. (2001). Impetus beliefs as default heuristics: Dissociation between explicit and implicit knowledge about motion. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 439-453. doi:10.3758/ BF03196179
    • (2001) Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , vol.8 , pp. 439-453
    • Kozhevnikov, M.1    Hegarty, M.2
  • 93
    • 0024581848 scopus 로고
    • The role of preparation in timing anticipatory and reflex responses during catching
    • Lacquaniti, F., & Maioli, C. (1989). The role of preparation in timing anticipatory and reflex responses during catching. Journal of Neuroscience, 9, 134-148.
    • (1989) Journal of Neuroscience , vol.9 , pp. 134-148
    • Lacquaniti, F.1    Maioli, C.2
  • 94
    • 0020838812 scopus 로고
    • The law relating kinematic and figural aspects of drawing movements
    • doi:10.1016/0001-6918(83)90027-6
    • Lacquaniti, F., Terzuolo, C., & Viviani, P. (1983). The law relating kinematic and figural aspects of drawing movements. Acta Psychologica, 54, 115-130. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(83)90027-6
    • (1983) Acta Psychologica , vol.54 , pp. 115-130
    • Lacquaniti, F.1    Terzuolo, C.2    Viviani, P.3
  • 95
    • 0038345636 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Production and perception of grip force without proprioception: Is there a sense of effort in deafferented subjects?
    • doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02700.x
    • Lafargue, G., Paillard, J., Lamarre, Y., & Sirigu, A. (2003). Production and perception of grip force without proprioception: Is there a sense of effort in deafferented subjects? European Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 2741-2749. doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02700.x
    • (2003) European Journal of Neuroscience , vol.17 , pp. 2741-2749
    • Lafargue, G.1    Paillard, J.2    Lamarre, Y.3    Sirigu, A.4
  • 96
    • 0027685938 scopus 로고
    • Extracting object properties through haptic exploration
    • doi:10.1016/ 0001-6918(93)90070-8
    • Lederman, S. J., & Klatzky, R. L. (1993). Extracting object properties through haptic exploration. Acta Psychologica, 84, 29-40. doi:10.1016/ 0001-6918(93)90070-8
    • (1993) Acta Psychologica , vol.84 , pp. 29-40
    • Lederman, S.J.1    Klatzky, R.L.2
  • 97
    • 33947669777 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Haptic recognition of static and dynamic expressions of emotion in the live face
    • doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01866.x
    • Lederman, S. J., Klatzky, R. L., Abramowicz, R., Salsman, K., Kitada, R., & Hamilton, C. (2007). Haptic recognition of static and dynamic expressions of emotion in the live face. Psychological Science, 18, 158-164. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01866.x
    • (2007) Psychological Science , vol.18 , pp. 158-164
    • Lederman, S.J.1    Klatzky, R.L.2    Abramowicz, R.3    Salsman, K.4    Kitada, R.5    Hamilton, C.6
  • 98
    • 0017201970 scopus 로고
    • A theory of the visual control of braking based on information about time-to-collision
    • doi: 10.1068/p050437
    • Lee, D. N. (1976). A theory of the visual control of braking based on information about time-to-collision. Perception, 5, 437-459. doi: 10.1068/p050437
    • (1976) Perception , vol.5 , pp. 437-459
    • Lee, D.N.1
  • 99
    • 0032262015 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Guiding movement by coupling taus
    • doi:10.1207/s15326969eco103&4_4
    • Lee, D. N. (1998). Guiding movement by coupling taus. Ecological Psychology, 10, 221-250. doi:10.1207/s15326969eco103&4_4
    • (1998) Ecological Psychology , vol.10 , pp. 221-250
    • Lee, D.N.1
  • 100
    • 0002491205 scopus 로고
    • 5-ToMM, ToBy, and agency: Core architecture and domain specificity
    • L. Hirschfield & S. Gelman (Eds.), New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/ CBO9780511752902.006
    • Leslie, A. M. (1994). 5-ToMM, ToBy, and agency: Core architecture and domain specificity. In L. Hirschfield & S. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 119-148). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/ CBO9780511752902.006
    • (1994) Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture , pp. 119-148
    • Leslie, A.M.1
  • 101
    • 0002685572 scopus 로고
    • A theory of agency
    • D. Sperber, D. Premack, & A. J. Premack (Eds.), Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.
    • Leslie, A. M. (1995). A theory of agency. In D. Sperber, D. Premack, & A. J. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate (pp. 121-149). Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.
    • (1995) Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate , pp. 121-149
    • Leslie, A.M.1
  • 102
    • 0023319590 scopus 로고
    • Do six-month-old infants perceive causality?
    • doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(87) 80006-9
    • Leslie, A. M., & Keeble, S. (1987). Do six-month-old infants perceive causality? Cognition, 25, 265-288. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(87) 80006-9
    • (1987) Cognition , vol.25 , pp. 265-288
    • Leslie, A.M.1    Keeble, S.2
  • 103
    • 0004245950 scopus 로고
    • Symmetry, causality, mind
    • Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
    • Leyton, M. (1992). Symmetry, causality, mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    • (1992)
    • Leyton, M.1
  • 104
    • 69249154719 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Object manipulation and motion perception: Evidence of an influence of action planning on visual processing
    • doi:10.1037/a0015023
    • Lindemann, O., & Bekkering, H. (2009). Object manipulation and motion perception: Evidence of an influence of action planning on visual processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1062-1071. doi:10.1037/a0015023
    • (2009) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.35 , pp. 1062-1071
    • Lindemann, O.1    Bekkering, H.2
  • 105
    • 64649099071 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Young infants' reasoning about physical events involving inert and self-propelled objects
    • doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2008.11.001
    • Luo, Y., Kaufman, L., & Baillargeon, R. (2009). Young infants' reasoning about physical events involving inert and self-propelled objects. Cognitive Psychology, 58, 441-486. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2008.11.001
    • (2009) Cognitive Psychology , vol.58 , pp. 441-486
    • Luo, Y.1    Kaufman, L.2    Baillargeon, R.3
  • 107
    • 84965400261 scopus 로고
    • Perceptual bias for forward-facing motion
    • doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1992.tb00048.x
    • McBeath, M. K., Morikawa, K., & Kaiser, M. K. (1992). Perceptual bias for forward-facing motion. Psychological Science, 3, 362-367. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1992.tb00048.x
    • (1992) Psychological Science , vol.3 , pp. 362-367
    • McBeath, M.K.1    Morikawa, K.2    Kaiser, M.K.3
  • 108
    • 0015987515 scopus 로고
    • Estimation of weights and tensions and apparent involvement of a "sense of effort"
    • McCloskey, D. L., Ebeling, P., & Goodwin, G. M. (1974). Estimation of weights and tensions and apparent involvement of a "sense of effort." Experimental Neurology, 42, 220-232.
    • (1974) Experimental Neurology , vol.42 , pp. 220-232
    • McCloskey, D.L.1    Ebeling, P.2    Goodwin, G.M.3
  • 109
    • 0001596874 scopus 로고
    • Intuitive physics
    • April, doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0483-122
    • McCloskey, M. (1983, April). Intuitive physics. Scientific American, 248, 122-130. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0483-122
    • (1983) Scientific American , vol.248 , pp. 122-130
    • McCloskey, M.1
  • 110
    • 34748862996 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Judgments of voluntary and physical causes in causal chains: Probabilistic and social functionalist criteria for attributions
    • doi:10.1002/ejsp.394
    • McClure, J., Hilton, D. J., & Sutton, R. M. (2007). Judgments of voluntary and physical causes in causal chains: Probabilistic and social functionalist criteria for attributions. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 879-901. doi:10.1002/ejsp.394
    • (2007) European Journal of Social Psychology , vol.37 , pp. 879-901
    • McClure, J.1    Hilton, D.J.2    Sutton, R.M.3
  • 111
    • 0032044434 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Higher and lower order variables in the visual perception of relative pulling force
    • doi:10.1037/0096-1523.24.2.526
    • Michaels, C. F., & De Vries, M. M. (1998). Higher and lower order variables in the visual perception of relative pulling force. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 526-546. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.24.2.526
    • (1998) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.24 , pp. 526-546
    • Michaels, C.F.1    De Vries, M.M.2
  • 112
    • 0004029862 scopus 로고
    • The perception of causality
    • New York, NY: Basic Books
    • Michotte, A. (1963). The perception of causality. New York, NY: Basic Books.
    • (1963)
    • Michotte, A.1
  • 113
    • 0742268022 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Manual cyclical activity as an exploratory tool in neonates
    • doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2003.05.003
    • Molina, M., & Jouen, F. (2004). Manual cyclical activity as an exploratory tool in neonates. Infant Behavior and Development, 27, 42-53. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2003.05.003
    • (2004) Infant Behavior and Development , vol.27 , pp. 42-53
    • Molina, M.1    Jouen, F.2
  • 114
    • 0037319320 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The effects of digital anesthesia on force control using a precision grip
    • doi:10.1152/jn.00434.2001
    • Monzée, J., Lamarre, Y., & Smith, A. M. (2003). The effects of digital anesthesia on force control using a precision grip. Journal of Neurophysiology, 89, 672-683. doi:10.1152/jn.00434.2001
    • (2003) Journal of Neurophysiology , vol.89 , pp. 672-683
    • Monzée, J.1    Lamarre, Y.2    Smith, A.M.3
  • 116
    • 0032609384 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Symmetry and elongation of objects influence perceived direction of translational motion
    • doi:10.3758/BF03211954
    • Morikawa, K. (1999). Symmetry and elongation of objects influence perceived direction of translational motion. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 61, 134-143. doi:10.3758/BF03211954
    • (1999) Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics , vol.61 , pp. 134-143
    • Morikawa, K.1
  • 118
    • 0038985161 scopus 로고
    • Principles of momentum and kinetic energy in the perception of causality
    • doi:10.2307/1419745
    • Natsoulas, T. (1961). Principles of momentum and kinetic energy in the perception of causality. The American Journal of Psychology, 74, 394-402. doi:10.2307/1419745
    • (1961) The American Journal of Psychology , vol.74 , pp. 394-402
    • Natsoulas, T.1
  • 119
    • 52749087180 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The origins of causal perception: Evidence from postdictive processing in infancy
    • doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2008 .02.003
    • Newman, G. E., Choi, H., Wynn, K., & Scholl, B. J. (2008). The origins of causal perception: Evidence from postdictive processing in infancy. Cognitive Psychology, 57, 262-291. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2008 .02.003
    • (2008) Cognitive Psychology , vol.57 , pp. 262-291
    • Newman, G.E.1    Choi, H.2    Wynn, K.3    Scholl, B.J.4
  • 121
    • 0347989168 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • How predictive is grip force control in the complete absence of somatosensory feedback?
    • doi:10.1093/brain/awh016
    • Nowak, D. A., Glasauer, S., & Hermsdörfer, J. (2004). How predictive is grip force control in the complete absence of somatosensory feedback? Brain, 127, 182-192. doi:10.1093/brain/awh016
    • (2004) Brain , vol.127 , pp. 182-192
    • Nowak, D.A.1    Glasauer, S.2    Hermsdörfer, J.3
  • 122
    • 23844436271 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The Galileo bias: A naive conceptual belief that influences people's perceptions and performance in a ball-dropping task
    • doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.4.643
    • Oberle, C. D., McBeath, M. K., Madigan, S. C., & Sugar, T. G. (2005). The Galileo bias: A naive conceptual belief that influences people's perceptions and performance in a ball-dropping task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 643-653. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.4.643
    • (2005) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , vol.31 , pp. 643-653
    • Oberle, C.D.1    McBeath, M.K.2    Madigan, S.C.3    Sugar, T.G.4
  • 123
    • 0031310162 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Phenomenal causality: Impressions of pulling in the visual perception of objects in motion
    • doi:10.2307/1423411
    • P. A., & Milne, A. (1997). Phenomenal causality: Impressions of pulling in the visual perception of objects in motion. The American Journal of Psychology, 110, 573-602. doi:10.2307/1423411
    • (1997) The American Journal of Psychology , vol.110 , pp. 573-602
    • Milne, A.1
  • 124
    • 0032483423 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Effects of movement direction upon kinematic characteristics of vertical arm pointing movements in man
    • doi:10.1016/S0304-3940(98)00604-1
    • Papaxanthis, C., Pozzo, T., & Stapley, P. (1998). Effects of movement direction upon kinematic characteristics of vertical arm pointing movements in man. Neuroscience Letters, 253, 103-106. doi:10.1016/S0304-3940(98)00604-1
    • (1998) Neuroscience Letters , vol.253 , pp. 103-106
    • Papaxanthis, C.1    Pozzo, T.2    Stapley, P.3
  • 125
    • 0028488382 scopus 로고
    • Temporal and kinetic properties of motor behavior reflected in mentally simulated action
    • doi:10.1037/ 0096-1523.20.4.709
    • Parsons, L. M. (1994). Temporal and kinetic properties of motor behavior reflected in mentally simulated action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 709-730. doi:10.1037/ 0096-1523.20.4.709
    • (1994) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.20 , pp. 709-730
    • Parsons, L.M.1
  • 126
    • 33244487224 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Kinematic features of movement tunes perception and action coupling
    • doi:10.1016/ j.bbr.2005.12.005
    • Pozzo, T., Papaxanthis, C., Petit, J. L., Schweighofer, N., & Stucchi, N. (2006). Kinematic features of movement tunes perception and action coupling. Behavioural Brain Research, 169, 75-82. doi:10.1016/ j.bbr.2005.12.005
    • (2006) Behavioural Brain Research , vol.169 , pp. 75-82
    • Pozzo, T.1    Papaxanthis, C.2    Petit, J.L.3    Schweighofer, N.4    Stucchi, N.5
  • 127
    • 84993729290 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Embodied perception and the economy of action
    • doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00008.x
    • Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 110-122. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00008.x
    • (2006) Perspectives on Psychological Science , vol.1 , pp. 110-122
    • Proffitt, D.R.1
  • 129
    • 0035462952 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Seeing mountains in molehills: Geographical-slant perception
    • doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00377
    • Proffitt, D. R., Creem, S. H., & Zosh, W. D. (2001). Seeing mountains in molehills: Geographical-slant perception. Psychological Science, 12, 418-423. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00377
    • (2001) Psychological Science , vol.12 , pp. 418-423
    • Proffitt, D.R.1    Creem, S.H.2    Zosh, W.D.3
  • 130
    • 22644437863 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A secret agent? How infants learn about the identity of objects in a causal scene
    • doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2005.03.005
    • Rakison, D. H. (2005). A secret agent? How infants learn about the identity of objects in a causal scene. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 91, 271-296. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2005.03.005
    • (2005) Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , vol.91 , pp. 271-296
    • Rakison, D.H.1
  • 131
    • 33748787685 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Making the first move: How infants learn about self-propelled objects
    • doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.5.900
    • Rakison, D. H. (2006). Making the first move: How infants learn about self-propelled objects. Developmental Psychology, 42, 900-912. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.5.900
    • (2006) Developmental Psychology , vol.42 , pp. 900-912
    • Rakison, D.H.1
  • 132
    • 0035290281 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Developmental origin of the animate-inanimate distinction
    • doi:10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.209
    • Rakison, D. H., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2001). Developmental origin of the animate-inanimate distinction. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 209-228. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.209
    • (2001) Psychological Bulletin , vol.127 , pp. 209-228
    • Rakison, D.H.1    Poulin-Dubois, D.2
  • 133
    • 0036583464 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • You go this way and I'll go that way: Developmental changes in infants' detection of correlations among static and dynamic features in motion events
    • doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00432
    • Rakison, D. H., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2002). You go this way and I'll go that way: Developmental changes in infants' detection of correlations among static and dynamic features in motion events. Child Development, 73, 682-699. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00432
    • (2002) Child Development , vol.73 , pp. 682-699
    • Rakison, D.H.1    Poulin-Dubois, D.2
  • 134
    • 38249034691 scopus 로고
    • Mouthing and grasping in neonates: Evidence for the early detection of what hard or soft substances afford for action
    • doi:10.1016/0163-6383(87)90041-5
    • Rochat, P. (1987). Mouthing and grasping in neonates: Evidence for the early detection of what hard or soft substances afford for action. Infant Behavior and Development, 10, 435-449. doi:10.1016/0163-6383(87)90041-5
    • (1987) Infant Behavior and Development , vol.10 , pp. 435-449
    • Rochat, P.1
  • 135
    • 0001417795 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Young infants' sensitivity to movement information specifying social causality
    • doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(97)90022-8
    • Rochat, P., Morgan, R., & Carpenter, M. (1997). Young infants' sensitivity to movement information specifying social causality. Cognitive Development, 12, 537-561. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(97)90022-8
    • (1997) Cognitive Development , vol.12 , pp. 537-561
    • Rochat, P.1    Morgan, R.2    Carpenter, M.3
  • 136
    • 0017610143 scopus 로고
    • A quantitative analysis of sensations of tension and of kinaesthesia in man: Evidence for a peripherally operating muscular sense and for a sense of effort
    • doi:10.1093/brain/100.4.671
    • Roland, P. E., & Ladegaard-Pederson, H. (1977). A quantitative analysis of sensations of tension and of kinaesthesia in man: Evidence for a peripherally operating muscular sense and for a sense of effort. Brain, 100, 671-692. doi:10.1093/brain/100.4.671
    • (1977) Brain , vol.100 , pp. 671-692
    • Roland, P.E.1    Ladegaard-Pederson, H.2
  • 137
    • 0019920672 scopus 로고
    • Manual motor performance in a deafferented man
    • doi:10.1093/brain/105.3.515
    • Rothwell, J. C., Traub, M. M., Day, B. L., Obeso, J. L., Thomas, P. K., & Marsden, C. D. (1982). Manual motor performance in a deafferented man. Brain, 105, 515-542. doi:10.1093/brain/105.3.515
    • (1982) Brain , vol.105 , pp. 515-542
    • Rothwell, J.C.1    Traub, M.M.2    Day, B.L.3    Obeso, J.L.4    Thomas, P.K.5    Marsden, C.D.6
  • 138
    • 0025534756 scopus 로고
    • The "memory system" of prelinguistic infants
    • doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1990.tb48908.x
    • Rovee-Collier, C. (1990). The "memory system" of prelinguistic infants. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 608, 517-542. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1990.tb48908.x
    • (1990) Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences , vol.608 , pp. 517-542
    • Rovee-Collier, C.1
  • 139
    • 0016143678 scopus 로고
    • Constant velocity-Not perceived as such
    • doi:10.1007/BF00309076
    • Runeson, S. (1974). Constant velocity-Not perceived as such. Psychological Research, 37, 3-23. doi:10.1007/BF00309076
    • (1974) Psychological Research , vol.37 , pp. 3-23
    • Runeson, S.1
  • 140
    • 0016840393 scopus 로고
    • Visual prediction of collision with natural and nonnatural motion functions
    • doi:10.3758/BF03199372
    • Runeson, S. (1975). Visual prediction of collision with natural and nonnatural motion functions. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 18, 261-266. doi:10.3758/BF03199372
    • (1975) Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics , vol.18 , pp. 261-266
    • Runeson, S.1
  • 142
    • 0038497010 scopus 로고
    • Kinematic specification of dynamics as an informational basis for person-and-action perception: Expectation, gender recognition, and deceptive intention
    • doi:10.1037/0096-3445.112.4.585
    • Runeson, S., & Frykholm, G. (1983). Kinematic specification of dynamics as an informational basis for person-and-action perception: Expectation, gender recognition, and deceptive intention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 585-615. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.112.4.585
    • (1983) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , vol.112 , pp. 585-615
    • Runeson, S.1    Frykholm, G.2
  • 143
    • 0034220069 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Visual perception of dynamic properties: Cue heuristics versus direct-perceptual competence
    • doi:10.1037/0033-295X.107.3.525
    • Runeson, S., Juslin, P., & Olsson, H. (2000). Visual perception of dynamic properties: Cue heuristics versus direct-perceptual competence. Psychological Review, 107, 525-555. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.107.3.525
    • (2000) Psychological Review , vol.107 , pp. 525-555
    • Runeson, S.1    Juslin, P.2    Olsson, H.3
  • 144
    • 0027621151 scopus 로고
    • The indispensability of precollision kinematics in the visual perception of relative mass
    • doi:10.3758/BF03211738
    • Runeson, S., & Vedeler, D. (1993). The indispensability of precollision kinematics in the visual perception of relative mass. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 53, 617-632. doi:10.3758/BF03211738
    • (1993) Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics , vol.53 , pp. 617-632
    • Runeson, S.1    Vedeler, D.2
  • 145
    • 36749079057 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Objectand directionspecific interference between manual and mental rotation
    • doi:10.3758/BF03192958
    • Sack, A. T., Lindner, M., & Linden, D. E. J. (2007). Objectand directionspecific interference between manual and mental rotation. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 69, 1435-1449. doi:10.3758/BF03192958
    • (2007) Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics , vol.69 , pp. 1435-1449
    • Sack, A.T.1    Lindner, M.2    Linden, D.E.J.3
  • 146
    • 48049095497 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The limits of newborns' grasping to detect texture in a cross-modal transfer task
    • doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.03.001
    • Sann, C., & Streri, A. (2008). The limits of newborns' grasping to detect texture in a cross-modal transfer task. Infant Behavior and Development, 31, 523-531. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.03.001
    • (2008) Infant Behavior and Development , vol.31 , pp. 523-531
    • Sann, C.1    Streri, A.2
  • 147
    • 30344473607 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Secret agents: Inferences about hidden causes by 10 and 12-month-old infants
    • doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01649.x
    • Saxe, R., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Carey, S. (2005). Secret agents: Inferences about hidden causes by 10 and 12-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 16, 995-1001. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01649.x
    • (2005) Psychological Science , vol.16 , pp. 995-1001
    • Saxe, R.1    Tenenbaum, J.B.2    Carey, S.3
  • 148
    • 33846051045 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Knowing who dunnit: Infants identify the causal agent in an unseen causal interaction
    • doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.1.149
    • Saxe, R., Tzelnic, T., & Carey, S. (2007). Knowing who dunnit: Infants identify the causal agent in an unseen causal interaction. Developmental Psychology, 43, 149-158. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.1.149
    • (2007) Developmental Psychology , vol.43 , pp. 149-158
    • Saxe, R.1    Tzelnic, T.2    Carey, S.3
  • 149
    • 33748050116 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Perceived social and physical causality in animated motions: Spontaneous reports and ratings
    • doi:10.1016/ j.actpsy.2006.05.006
    • Schlottmann, A., Ray, E., Mitchell, A., & Demetriou, N. (2006). Perceived social and physical causality in animated motions: Spontaneous reports and ratings. Acta Psychologica, 123, 112-143. doi:10.1016/ j.actpsy.2006.05.006
    • (2006) Acta Psychologica , vol.123 , pp. 112-143
    • Schlottmann, A.1    Ray, E.2    Mitchell, A.3    Demetriou, N.4
  • 150
    • 0034255945 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Perceptual causality and animacy
    • doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01506-0
    • Scholl, B. J., & Tremoulet, P. D. (2000). Perceptual causality and animacy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 299-309. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01506-0
    • (2000) Trends in Cognitive Sciences , vol.4 , pp. 299-309
    • Scholl, B.J.1    Tremoulet, P.D.2
  • 151
    • 34447568210 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Perceptual resonance: Action induced modulation of perception
    • doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.06.005
    • Schütz-Bosbach, S., & Prinz, W. (2007). Perceptual resonance: Action induced modulation of perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 349-355. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.06.005
    • (2007) Trends in Cognitive Sciences , vol.11 , pp. 349-355
    • Schütz-Bosbach, S.1    Prinz, W.2
  • 152
    • 0033126880 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Physical imagery: Kinematic versus dynamic models
    • doi:10.1006/ cogp.1998.0702
    • Schwartz, D. L. (1999). Physical imagery: Kinematic versus dynamic models. Cognitive Psychology, 38, 433-464. doi:10.1006/ cogp.1998.0702
    • (1999) Cognitive Psychology , vol.38 , pp. 433-464
    • Schwartz, D.L.1
  • 154
    • 0021507049 scopus 로고
    • Ecological constraints on internal representation: Resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming
    • doi:10.1037/0033-295X.91.4.417
    • Shepard, R. N. (1984). Ecological constraints on internal representation: Resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming. Psychological Review, 91, 417-447. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.91.4.417
    • (1984) Psychological Review , vol.91 , pp. 417-447
    • Shepard, R.N.1
  • 155
    • 0012336764 scopus 로고
    • Perceptual-cognitive universals as reflections of the world
    • doi:10.3758/ BF03200759
    • Shepard, R. N. (1994). Perceptual-cognitive universals as reflections of the world. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 2-28. doi:10.3758/ BF03200759
    • (1994) Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , vol.1 , pp. 2-28
    • Shepard, R.N.1
  • 156
    • 0035735599 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Perceptual-cognitive universals as reflections of the world
    • doi:10.1017/ S0140525X01000012
    • Shepard, R. N. (2001). Perceptual-cognitive universals as reflections of the world. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 581-601. doi:10.1017/ S0140525X01000012
    • (2001) Behavioral and Brain Sciences , vol.24 , pp. 581-601
    • Shepard, R.N.1
  • 157
    • 84970207040 scopus 로고
    • Apparent motion of the human body
    • doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990 .tb00210.x
    • Shiffrar, M., & Freyd, J. (1990). Apparent motion of the human body. Psychological Science, 1, 257-264. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990 .tb00210.x
    • (1990) Psychological Science , vol.1 , pp. 257-264
    • Shiffrar, M.1    Freyd, J.2
  • 158
    • 2942590318 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Estimation of lifted weight and produced effort through perception of point-light display
    • doi:10.1068/p3434
    • Shim, J., Carlton, L. G., & Kim, J. (2004). Estimation of lifted weight and produced effort through perception of point-light display. Perception, 33, 277-291. doi:10.1068/p3434
    • (2004) Perception , vol.33 , pp. 277-291
    • Shim, J.1    Carlton, L.G.2    Kim, J.3
  • 160
    • 42449117549 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Lower limb force production and bilateral force asymmetries are based on sense of effort
    • doi:10.1007/s00221-008-1288-x
    • Simon, A. M., & Ferris, D. P. (2008). Lower limb force production and bilateral force asymmetries are based on sense of effort. Experimental Brain Research, 187, 129-138. doi:10.1007/s00221-008-1288-x
    • (2008) Experimental Brain Research , vol.187 , pp. 129-138
    • Simon, A.M.1    Ferris, D.P.2
  • 162
    • 0024617684 scopus 로고
    • Perceiving extents of rods by wielding: Haptic diagonalization and decomposition of the inertia tensor
    • doi:10.1037/0096-1523.15.1.58
    • Solomon, H. Y., Turvey, M. T., & Burton, G. (1989). Perceiving extents of rods by wielding: Haptic diagonalization and decomposition of the inertia tensor. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 58-68. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.15.1.58
    • (1989) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.15 , pp. 58-68
    • Solomon, H.Y.1    Turvey, M.T.2    Burton, G.3
  • 163
    • 17044380638 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Action experience alters 3-month-old infants' perception of others' actions
    • doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.07.004
    • Sommerville, J. A., Woodward, A. L., & Needham, A. (2005). Action experience alters 3-month-old infants' perception of others' actions. Cognition, 96, B1-B11. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2004.07.004
    • (2005) Cognition , vol.96
    • Sommerville, J.A.1    Woodward, A.L.2    Needham, A.3
  • 164
    • 42249095070 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Infants attribute goals even to biomechanically impossible actions
    • doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.10.002
    • Southgate, V., Johnson, M. H., & Csibra, G. (2008). Infants attribute goals even to biomechanically impossible actions. Cognition, 107, 1059-1069. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.10.002
    • (2008) Cognition , vol.107 , pp. 1059-1069
    • Southgate, V.1    Johnson, M.H.2    Csibra, G.3
  • 165
    • 0028414286 scopus 로고
    • Initial knowledge: Six suggestions
    • doi:10.1016/0010-0277(94)90039-6
    • Spelke, E. (1994). Initial knowledge: Six suggestions. Cognition, 50, 431-445. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(94)90039-6
    • (1994) Cognition , vol.50 , pp. 431-445
    • Spelke, E.1
  • 167
    • 0001926748 scopus 로고
    • Infants' knowledge of object motion and human action
    • D. Sperber, D. Premack, & A. J. Premack (Eds.), Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.
    • Spelke, E. S., Phillips, A., & Woodward, A. L. (1995). Infants' knowledge of object motion and human action. In D. Sperber, D. Premack, & A. J. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate (pp. 44-78). Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.
    • (1995) Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate , pp. 44-78
    • Spelke, E.S.1    Phillips, A.2    Woodward, A.L.3
  • 168
    • 22744454133 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The development of haptic abilities in very young infants: From perception to cognition
    • doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2005.05.004
    • Streri, A., & Féron, J. (2005). The development of haptic abilities in very young infants: From perception to cognition. Infant Behavior and Development, 28, 290-304. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2005.05.004
    • (2005) Infant Behavior and Development , vol.28 , pp. 290-304
    • Streri, A.1    Féron, J.2
  • 169
    • 0037262901 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Cross-modal recognition of shape from hand to eyes in human newborns
    • doi:10.1080/0899022031000083799
    • Streri, A., & Gentaz, E. (2003). Cross-modal recognition of shape from hand to eyes in human newborns. Somatosensory & Motor Research, 20, 13-18. doi:10.1080/0899022031000083799
    • (2003) Somatosensory & Motor Research , vol.20 , pp. 13-18
    • Streri, A.1    Gentaz, E.2
  • 170
    • 2942544452 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Cross-modal recognition of shape from hand to eyes and handedness in human newborns
    • doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.02.012
    • Streri, A., & Gentaz, E. (2004). Cross-modal recognition of shape from hand to eyes and handedness in human newborns. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1365-1369. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.02.012
    • (2004) Neuropsychologia , vol.42 , pp. 1365-1369
    • Streri, A.1    Gentaz, E.2
  • 171
    • 0027619478 scopus 로고
    • Modality-specific and amodal aspects of object perception in infancy: The case of active touch
    • doi:10.1016/0010-0277(93)90051-V
    • Streri, A., Spelke, E., & Rameix, E. (1993). Modality-specific and amodal aspects of object perception in infancy: The case of active touch. Cognition, 47, 251-279. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(93)90051-V
    • (1993) Cognition , vol.47 , pp. 251-279
    • Streri, A.1    Spelke, E.2    Rameix, E.3
  • 172
    • 34248875622 scopus 로고
    • Force dynamics in language and cognition
    • doi:10.1207/s15516709cog1201_2
    • Talmy, L. (1988). Force dynamics in language and cognition. Cognitive Science, 12, 49-100. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog1201_2
    • (1988) Cognitive Science , vol.12 , pp. 49-100
    • Talmy, L.1
  • 173
    • 0029249908 scopus 로고
    • Motor development: A new synthesis
    • doi:10.1037/0003-066X.50.2.79
    • Thelen, E. (1995). Motor development: A new synthesis. American Psychologist, 50, 79-95. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.50.2.79
    • (1995) American Psychologist , vol.50 , pp. 79-95
    • Thelen, E.1
  • 174
    • 0000758048 scopus 로고
    • Newborn stepping: An explanation for a "disappearing reflex"
    • doi: 101037/0012-1649.18.5.760
    • Thelen, E., & Fisher, D. M. (1982). Newborn stepping: An explanation for a "disappearing reflex." Developmental Psychology, 18, 760-775. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.18.5.760
    • (1982) Developmental Psychology , vol.18 , pp. 760-775
    • Thelen, E.1    Fisher, D.M.2
  • 175
    • 0021076796 scopus 로고
    • The organization of spontaneous leg movements in newborn infants
    • Thelen, E., & Fisher, D. M. (1983). The organization of spontaneous leg movements in newborn infants. Journal of Motor Behavior, 15, 353-377.
    • (1983) Journal of Motor Behavior , vol.15 , pp. 353-377
    • Thelen, E.1    Fisher, D.M.2
  • 176
    • 12044249440 scopus 로고
    • Hidden skills: A dynamic systems analysis of treadmill stepping during the first year
    • Thelen, E., & Ulrich, B. D. (1991). Hidden skills: A dynamic systems analysis of treadmill stepping during the first year. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 56(1, Serial No. 223).
    • (1991) Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development , vol.56 , Issue.1 SERIAL NO. 223
    • Thelen, E.1    Ulrich, B.D.2
  • 177
    • 0033654174 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Perception of animacy from the motion of a single object
    • doi:10.1068/p3101
    • Tremoulet, P. D., & Feldman, J. (2000). Perception of animacy from the motion of a single object. Perception, 29, 943-951. doi:10.1068/p3101
    • (2000) Perception , vol.29 , pp. 943-951
    • Tremoulet, P.D.1    Feldman, J.2
  • 178
    • 33845704326 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The influence of spatial context and the role of intentionality in the interpretation of animacy from motion
    • doi: 10.3758/BF03193364
    • Tremoulet, P. D., & Feldman, J. (2006). The influence of spatial context and the role of intentionality in the interpretation of animacy from motion. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 68, 1047-1058. doi: 10.3758/BF03193364
    • (2006) Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics , vol.68 , pp. 1047-1058
    • Tremoulet, P.D.1    Feldman, J.2
  • 179
    • 0032873623 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Grip force dynamics in the approach to a collision
    • doi:10.1007/s002210050822
    • Turrell, Y. N., Li, F.-X., & Wing, A. M. (1999). Grip force dynamics in the approach to a collision. Experimental Brain Research, 128, 86-91. doi:10.1007/s002210050822
    • (1999) Experimental Brain Research , vol.128 , pp. 86-91
    • Turrell, Y.N.1    Li, F.-X.2    Wing, A.M.3
  • 180
    • 0030279245 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Dynamic touch
    • doi:10.1037/0003-066X.51.11.1134
    • Turvey, M. T. (1996). Dynamic touch. American Psychologist, 51, 1134-1152. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.51.11.1134
    • (1996) American Psychologist , vol.51 , pp. 1134-1152
    • Turvey, M.T.1
  • 181
    • 0031839483 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Differential effects of load stiffness on matching pinch force, finger span, and effort
    • doi:10.1007/s002210050422
    • Van Doren, C. L. (1998). Differential effects of load stiffness on matching pinch force, finger span, and effort. Experimental Brain Research, 120, 487-495. doi:10.1007/s002210050422
    • (1998) Experimental Brain Research , vol.120 , pp. 487-495
    • Van Doren, C.L.1
  • 182
    • 0035827465 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Relationahip between velocity and curvature of a human locomotor trajectory
    • doi:10.1016/S0304-3940(01)01798-0
    • Vieilledent, S., Kerlirzin, Y., Dalbera, S., & Berthoz, A. (2001). Relationahip between velocity and curvature of a human locomotor trajectory. Neuroscience Letters, 305, 65-69. doi:10.1016/S0304-3940(01)01798-0
    • (2001) Neuroscience Letters , vol.305 , pp. 65-69
    • Vieilledent, S.1    Kerlirzin, Y.2    Dalbera, S.3    Berthoz, A.4
  • 183
    • 0031204178 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Perceiving and tracking kinesthetic stimuli: Further evidence of motor-perceptual interactions
    • doi:10.1037/0096-1523.23.4.1232
    • Viviani, P., Baud-Bovy, G., & Redolfi, M. (1997). Perceiving and tracking kinesthetic stimuli: Further evidence of motor-perceptual interactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1232-1252. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.23.4.1232
    • (1997) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.23 , pp. 1232-1252
    • Viviani, P.1    Baud-Bovy, G.2    Redolfi, M.3
  • 184
    • 0024351758 scopus 로고
    • The effect of movement velocity on form perception: Geometric illusions in dynamic displays
    • doi:10.3758/BF03208089
    • Viviani, P., & Stucchi, N. (1989). The effect of movement velocity on form perception: Geometric illusions in dynamic displays. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 46, 266-274. doi:10.3758/BF03208089
    • (1989) Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics , vol.46 , pp. 266-274
    • Viviani, P.1    Stucchi, N.2
  • 185
    • 0026905811 scopus 로고
    • Biological movements look constant: Evidence of motor-perceptual interactions
    • doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.18.3.603
    • Viviani, P., & Stucchi, N. (1992). Biological movements look constant: Evidence of motor-perceptual interactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 603-623. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.18.3.603
    • (1992) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.18 , pp. 603-623
    • Viviani, P.1    Stucchi, N.2
  • 186
    • 0020026261 scopus 로고
    • Trajectory determines movement dynamics
    • doi:10.1016/0306-4522(82) 90277-9
    • Viviani, P., & Terzuolo, C. (1982). Trajectory determines movement dynamics. Neuroscience, 7, 431-437. doi:10.1016/0306-4522(82) 90277-9
    • (1982) Neuroscience , vol.7 , pp. 431-437
    • Viviani, P.1    Terzuolo, C.2
  • 187
    • 33244484561 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The causal asymmetry
    • doi:10.1037/0033-295X.113.1.132
    • White, P. A. (2006). The causal asymmetry. Psychological Review, 113, 132-147. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.113.1.132
    • (2006) Psychological Review , vol.113 , pp. 132-147
    • White, P.A.1
  • 188
    • 35348899680 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Impressions of force in visual perception of collision events: A test of the causal asymmetry hypothesis
    • doi:10.3758/BF03196815
    • White, P. A. (2007). Impressions of force in visual perception of collision events: A test of the causal asymmetry hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 647-652. doi:10.3758/BF03196815
    • (2007) Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , vol.14 , pp. 647-652
    • White, P.A.1
  • 189
    • 67651061532 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Perception of forces exerted by objects in collision events
    • doi:10.1037/a0016337
    • White, P. A. (2009). Perception of forces exerted by objects in collision events. Psychological Review, 116, 580-601. doi:10.1037/a0016337
    • (2009) Psychological Review , vol.116 , pp. 580-601
    • White, P.A.1
  • 190
    • 79958762943 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Visual impressions of forces between objects: Entraining, enforced disintegration, and shattering
    • doi:10.1080/13506285.2011.575898
    • White, P. A. (2011a). Visual impressions of forces between objects: Entraining, enforced disintegration, and shattering. Visual Cognition, 19, 635-674. doi:10.1080/13506285.2011.575898
    • (2011) Visual Cognition , vol.19 , pp. 635-674
    • White, P.A.1
  • 191
    • 84874329376 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Visual impressions of pushing and pulling: The object perceived as causal is not always the one that moves first
    • Manuscript submitted for publication
    • White, P. A. (2011b). Visual impressions of pushing and pulling: The object perceived as causal is not always the one that moves first. Manuscript submitted for publication.
    • (2011)
    • White, P.A.1
  • 193
    • 0033259808 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Impressions of enforced disintegration and bursting in the visual perception of collision events
    • doi:10.1037/0096-3445.128.4.499
    • White, P. A., & Milne, A. (1999). Impressions of enforced disintegration and bursting in the visual perception of collision events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 499-516. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.128.4.499
    • (1999) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , vol.128 , pp. 499-516
    • White, P.A.1    Milne, A.2
  • 194
    • 0343776150 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Visual motion priming by invisible actions
    • doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00239-4
    • Wohlschläger, A. (2000). Visual motion priming by invisible actions. Vision Research, 40, 925-930. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00239-4
    • (2000) Vision Research , vol.40 , pp. 925-930
    • Wohlschläger, A.1
  • 196
    • 33847665921 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Representing causation
    • doi:10.1037/0096-3445.136.1.82
    • Wolff, P. (2007). Representing causation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 82-111. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.136.1.82
    • (2007) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , vol.136 , pp. 82-111
    • Wolff, P.1
  • 197
    • 0141987822 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Models of causation and the semantics of causal verbs
    • doi:10.1016/S0010-0285(03)00036-7
    • Wolff, P., & Song, G. (2003). Models of causation and the semantics of causal verbs. Cognitive Psychology, 47, 276-332. doi:10.1016/S0010-0285(03)00036-7
    • (2003) Cognitive Psychology , vol.47 , pp. 276-332
    • Wolff, P.1    Song, G.2
  • 198
    • 2142796047 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Motor prediction
    • doi:10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00432-8
    • Wolpert, D. M., & Flanagan, J. R. (2001). Motor prediction. Current Biology, 11, R729-R732. doi:10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00432-8
    • (2001) Current Biology , vol.11
    • Wolpert, D.M.1    Flanagan, J.R.2
  • 199
    • 0012894432 scopus 로고
    • Phenomenal causation at a distance
    • doi:10.1080/ 17470215208416612
    • Yela, M. (1952). Phenomenal causation at a distance. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 4, 139-154. doi:10.1080/ 17470215208416612
    • (1952) The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , vol.4 , pp. 139-154
    • Yela, M.1
  • 200
    • 13544273979 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Cognitive, perceptual and actionoriented representations of falling objects
    • doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.11.005
    • Zago, M., & Lacquaniti, F. (2005). Cognitive, perceptual and action oriented representations of falling objects. Neuropsychologia, 43, 178-188. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.11.005
    • (2005) Neuropsychologia , vol.43 , pp. 178-188
    • Zago, M.1    Lacquaniti, F.2


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.