메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 106, Issue 3, 2012, Pages 1451-1488

Liar, Liar, Jury's the Trier? The future of neuroscience-based credibility assessment in the court

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords


EID: 84866914874     PISSN: 00293571     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: None     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (14)

References (288)
  • 1
    • 84866894522 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • United States v. Semrau, No. 07-10074 Ml/P, 2010 WL 6845092, at *2 (W.D. Tenn. May 31, 2010).
  • 2
    • 84866942430 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Cephos is one of two companies in the United States that provides a neuroscience-based lie-detection service. Cephos, http://www.cephoscorp.com (last visited Aug. 4, 2012).
    • (2012)
  • 3
    • 84866924854 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • The other is No-Lie MRI. No Lie MRI, http://noliemri.com/ (last visited Aug. 4, 2012). Both of these companies use functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to attempt to identify brain activity that is associated with lying.
    • (2012)
  • 4
    • 84866918075 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • CEPHOS, Supra
    • Supra
  • 5
    • 84866951003 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • No Lie MRI, Supra. While Cephos lie-detection tests have been offered (and rejected) as evidence in two cases (Semrau, 2010 WL 6845092, at *3, and Wilson v. Corestaff Servs. L.P., 900 N.Y.S.2d 639, 640 (Sup. Ct. 2010)), there have been no recorded instances of parties attempting to introduce No Lie MRI tests as evidence.
    • Supra
  • 6
    • 84866894524 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Semrau, 2010 WL 6845092, at *4.
  • 7
    • 84866924853 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id. at *4-8. fMRI is a method used to determine the level of activity in various parts of the brain.
  • 9
    • 84866942428 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Semrau, 2010 WL 6845092, at *5.
  • 10
    • 84866938923 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id.
  • 11
    • 84866894523 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Id. at *4.
  • 12
    • 84866900742 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Id. at *6.
  • 13
    • 84866942432 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Id. at *1.
  • 14
    • 84866893436 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See Id. at *9-14. Additionally, because Dr. Semrau was retested after being originally identified as deceptive, Dr. Laken violated Cephos's standard protocol.
  • 15
    • 84866942431 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See Id. at *13. Under Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, Inc., the controlling standard for admissibility of expert testimony in federal cases, "the existence and maintenance of standards controlling [a] technique's operation" is a factor to consider in ruling on the admissibility of "a particular scientific technique." 509 U.S. 579, 594 (1993). Presumably, the lack of a followed standard by Dr. Laken in Semrau would cut against admitting the test.
  • 16
    • 84866938927 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Wilson v. Corestaff Servs. L.P., 900 N.Y.S.2d 639, 640 (Sup. Ct. 2010).
  • 17
    • 84866893435 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id.
  • 18
    • 84866938926 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id.
  • 19
    • 84866893434 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id. (internal quotation marks omitted).
  • 20
    • 84866942433 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • 2010 WL 6845092.
  • 21
    • 84866893437 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • 900 N.Y.S.2d 639.
  • 22
    • 84866942436 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id. at 642. It should be noted that the Wilson case was a New York state court case, and New York follows the Frye standard for admissibility of expert testimony.
  • 23
    • 84866938925 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See Id. at 640. The Frye test requires that evidence "must be sufficiently established to have gained general acceptance in the particular field in which it belongs." Frye v. United States, 293 F. 1013, 1014 (D.C. Cir. 1923).
  • 24
    • 84866893430 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • In contrast, federal courts follow the Daubert standard under the Federal Rules of Evidence, which asks whether the evidence is generally reliable. Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharm., Inc., 509 U.S. 579, 594-95 (1993).
  • 25
    • 84866942429 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Wilson, 900 N.Y.S.2d at 642. The court rejected the evidence solely on this basis, though it briefly mentioned that the evidence would also likely be disqualified because it was not derived from a method generally accepted in the scientific community, as required under Frye.
  • 26
    • 84866942435 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id.
  • 27
    • 0002905746 scopus 로고
    • The Polygraph in 1986: Scientific, Professional, and Legal Issues Surrounding Application and Acceptance of Polygraph Evidence
    • note
    • See David C. Raskin, The Polygraph in 1986: Scientific, Professional, and Legal Issues Surrounding Application and Acceptance of Polygraph Evidence, 1986 Utah L. Rev. 29+31. The polygraph measures "palmar sweating, blood pressure increases and respiratory changes" to determine the veracity of a statement or the presence of concealed information.
    • (1986) Utah L. Rev
    • Raskin, D.C.1
  • 28
    • 0002905746 scopus 로고
    • The Polygraph in 1986: Scientific, Professional, and Legal Issues Surrounding Application and Acceptance of Polygraph Evidence
    • Id.
    • (1986) Utah L. Rev
    • Raskin, D.C.1
  • 29
    • 84866900744 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The Case Against Polygraph Tests
    • note
    • See William G. Iacono & David T. Lykken, The Case Against Polygraph Tests, in 5 Modern Scientific Evidence: The Law And Science Of Expert Testimony § 40:47 (David L. Faigman et al. eds., 2011-2012). The "control" question is sometimes instead referred to as a "comparison question" because it is not a true control in the scientific sense of the word.
    • (2011) Modern Scientific Evidence: The Law and Science of Expert Testimony , vol.5 , Issue.40 , pp. 47
    • Iacono, W.G.1    Lykken, D.T.2
  • 35
    • 36549077126 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Detection of Deception
    • note
    • See William G. Iacono, Detection of Deception, in Handbook Of Psychophysiology 688+688 (John Cacioppo et al. eds., 3d ed. 2007).
    • (2007) Handbook of Psychophysiology
    • Iacono, W.G.1
  • 41
    • 84866900743 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See Nat'l Research Council, Supra note 28, at 92-95
    • Supra Note , vol.28 , pp. 92-95
  • 44
    • 78049421325 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Visions of Deception: Neuroimages and the Search for Truth
    • note
    • See Jane Campbell Moriarty, Visions of Deception: Neuroimages and the Search for Truth, 42 Akron L. Rev. 739+743-744 (2009).
    • (2009) Akron L. Rev , vol.42
    • Moriarty, J.C.1
  • 45
    • 84866893438 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • 293 F. 1013, 1014 (D.C. Cir. 1923). In Frye, the court rejected a "systolic blood pressure deception test" used to detect deception that was highly related to the modern polygraph-based CQT.
  • 46
    • 84866900746 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id. at 1013-1014. The defendant in the case offered the deception test as evidence of her innocence.
  • 47
    • 84866893442 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id. The court rejected the test because it "ha[d] not yet gained... scientific recognition."
  • 48
    • 84866894515 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Id. at 1014.
  • 49
    • 84866942477 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The Case for Polygraph Tests
    • note
    • The original CQT was developed by John Reid in 1947 in response to the shortcomings of a previous similar polygraph technique called the relevant-irrelevant test. See Charles R. Honts et al., The Case for Polygraph Tests, in 5 Modern Scientific Evidence.
    • Modern Scientific Evidence , vol.5
    • Honts, C.R.1
  • 50
    • 84866924845 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Supra note 20, § 40:22. This method was developed for the polygraph and continues to be used primarily with the polygraph.
    • Supra Note , vol.20 , Issue.40 , pp. 22
  • 51
    • 84866938963 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Though palmar sweating tends to be the primary measure used in polygraph tests, blood pressure increases and respiratory changes are also frequently considered. See, e.g., DC Raskin, Supra note 19, at 31.
    • Supra Note , vol.19 , pp. 31
    • Raskin, D.C.1
  • 52
    • 27644460709 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Detecting Deception Using Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging
    • note
    • See, e.g., F. Andrew Kozel et al., Detecting Deception Using Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 58 Biological Psychiatry 605+605 (2005)
    • (2005) Biological Psychiatry , vol.58
    • Kozel, F.A.1
  • 53
    • 28744458833 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Telling Truth from Lie in Individual Subjects with Fast Event-Related fMRI
    • Daniel D. Langleben et al., Telling Truth from Lie in Individual Subjects with Fast Event-Related fMRI, 26 Hum. Brain Mapping 262+262-263 (2005).
    • (2005) Hum. Brain Mapping , vol.26
    • Langleben, D.D.1
  • 54
    • 33845472695 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • True Lies: Delusions and Lie-Detection Technology
    • Daniel D. Langleben et al., True Lies: Delusions and Lie-Detection Technology, 34 J. Psychiatry & L. 351+359 (2006).
    • (2006) J. Psychiatry & L , vol.34
    • Langleben, D.D.1
  • 55
    • 84866942480 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See, e.g., NK Logothetis & BA Wandell, Supra note 4, at 760 ("The BOLD response provides us unprecedented visibility of the neural activity in the human brain"). The term "BOLD" stands for blood oxygen level dependent-it is a signal that measures blood flow to various areas of the brain.
    • Supra Note , vol.4 , pp. 760
    • Logothetis, N.K.1    Wandell, B.A.2
  • 58
    • 84866924846 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • The study involved the use of a "model building group" to identify approximately which neural networks were involved in deception for the experiment's particular paradigm. Id. at 605-606. This method could not likely be used in field scenarios, where individual questions must be unique for each person tested.
  • 60
    • 42249104737 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Flickering Admissibility: Neuroimaging Evidence in the U.S. Courts
    • note
    • See Jane Campbell Moriarty, Flickering Admissibility: Neuroimaging Evidence in the U.S. Courts, 26 Behav. Sci. & L. 29+39-40 (2008). For a case in which a PET scan was admitted to establish brain trauma, however, see Brown v. Allerton Assocs., No. 17917/03, 2006 WL 3102331 (N.Y. Sup. Ct. Oct. 5, 2006).
    • (2008) Behav. Sci. & L , vol.26
    • Moriarty, J.C.1
  • 61
    • 84929572361 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Law and Neuroscience in the United States
    • note
    • There are other contexts in which neuroimaging has been used in the courtroom. For a comprehensive review, see Owen D. Jones & Francis X. Shen, Law and Neuroscience in the United States, in International Neurolaw: A Comparative Analysis 349 (Tade Matthias Spranger ed., 2012).
    • (2012) International Neurolaw: A Comparative Analysis , pp. 349
    • Jones, O.D.1    Shen, F.X.2
  • 62
    • 84866924850 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • The CIT is also frequently referred to in the literature as the "Guilty Knowledge Test" or GKT. See CR Honts et al., Supra note 33, § 40:24.
    • Supra Note , vol.33 , Issue.40 , pp. 24
    • Honts, C.R.1
  • 64
    • 79951790939 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A Mock Terrorism Application of the P300-Based Concealed Information Test
    • John B. Meixner & J. Peter Rosenfeld, A Mock Terrorism Application of the P300-Based Concealed Information Test, 48 Psychophysiology 149+149 (2011).
    • (2011) Psychophysiology , vol.48
    • Meixner, J.B.1    Rosenfeld, J.P.2
  • 65
    • 0000200125 scopus 로고
    • The GSR in the Detection of Guilt
    • note
    • See David T. Lykken, The GSR in the Detection of Guilt, 43 J. Applied Psychol. 385+385 (1959) (using the polygraph to conduct the first published CIT).
    • (1959) J. Applied Psychol , vol.43
    • Lykken, D.T.1
  • 66
    • 0013854584 scopus 로고
    • Evoked-Potential Correlates of Stimulus Uncertainty
    • note
    • See Samuel Sutton et al., Evoked-Potential Correlates of Stimulus Uncertainty, 150 Science 1187+1187 (1965). The item must also be infrequently presented in order to generate the P300 component.
    • (1965) Science , vol.150
    • Sutton, S.1
  • 67
    • 0013854584 scopus 로고
    • Evoked-Potential Correlates of Stimulus Uncertainty
    • note
    • See Id. at 1188.
    • (1965) Science , vol.150 , pp. 1188
    • Sutton, S.1
  • 68
    • 0026353240 scopus 로고
    • The Truth Will Out: Interrogative Polygraphy ("Lie Detection") with Event-Related Brain Potentials
    • note
    • See, e.g., Lawrence A. Farwell & Emanuel Donchin, The Truth Will Out: Interrogative Polygraphy ("Lie Detection") with Event-Related Brain Potentials, 28 Psychophysiology 531+531-532 (1991).
    • (1991) Psychophysiology , vol.28
    • Farwell, L.A.1    Donchin, E.2
  • 69
    • 0024080772 scopus 로고
    • A Modified, Event-Related Potential-Based Guilty Knowledge Test
    • J. Peter Rosenfeld et al., A Modified, Event-Related Potential-Based Guilty Knowledge Test, 42 Int'l J. Neuroscience 157+157-158 (1988).
    • (1988) Int'l J. Neuroscience , vol.42
    • Rosenfeld, J.P.1
  • 70
    • 84866894520 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See, e.g., JB Meixner & JP Rosenfeld, Supra note 44, at 152-153 (reporting high levels of detection accuracy among twenty-four participants in a mock terrorist attack without any advance knowledge of the crime-related details)
    • Supra Note , vol.44 , pp. 152-153
    • Meixner, J.B.1    Rosenfeld, J.P.2
  • 71
    • 54049106592 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The Complex Trial Protocol (CTP): A New, Countermeasure-Resistant, Accurate, P300-Based Method for Detection of Concealed Information
    • note
    • J. Peter Rosenfeld et al., The Complex Trial Protocol (CTP): A New, Countermeasure-Resistant, Accurate, P300-Based Method for Detection of Concealed Information, 45 Psychophysiology 906+913 tbl.3b (2008) (reporting 92% detection accuracy with only 8% false positives).
    • (2008) Psychophysiology , vol.45
    • Rosenfeld, J.P.1
  • 72
    • 0036779629 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Trial by Polygraph: Reconsidering the Use of the Guilty Knowledge Technique in Court
    • Gershon Ben-Shakhar et al., Trial by Polygraph: Reconsidering the Use of the Guilty Knowledge Technique in Court, 26 Law & Hum. Behav. 527+528+536 (2002).
    • (2002) Law & Hum. Behav , vol.26
    • Ben-Shakhar, G.1
  • 74
    • 84866924848 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Fighting Crime by Reading Minds
    • note
    • Eben Harrell, Fighting Crime by Reading Minds, Time Science (Aug. 7, 2010), http://www.time.com/time/health/article/0,8599,2009131,00.html (covering Meixner & Rosenfeld, Supra note 44).
    • (2010) Time Science
    • Harrell, E.1
  • 75
    • 84866924851 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • United States v. Scheffer, 523 U.S. 303, 313 (1998) (quoting United States v. Barnard, 490 F.2d 907, 912 (9th Cir. 1973), cert. denied, 416 U.S. 959 (1974)).
  • 76
    • 84866938965 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Wilson v. Corestaff Servs. L.P., 900 N.Y.S.2d 639, 642 (Sup. Ct. 2010) (emphasis added).
  • 77
    • 84866894521 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See, e.g., Scheffer, 523 U.S. at 306-07, 313.
  • 78
    • 84866938960 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • State v. Porter, 698 A.2d 739, 742, 769 (Conn. 1997) (asserting that the principal problem in introducing polygraph-based lie-detection evidence is that it will "invade the fact-finding province of the jury"). It should be noted that the jury could still be the final determiner of credibility (and guilt) even if lie-detection evidence like the CQT were allowed into evidence. That is, the jury could still weigh the lie-detection evidence with all of the other evidence (e.g., demeanor, consistency of statements, etc.) and make a final decision as to whether they think the witness was being truthful. The argument made in Scheffer, however, is that the jury must be the only determiner of credibility, which would preclude lie-detection evidence, or any other evidence that directly speaks to the credibility of one of the witnesses. For extended discussion on this in the context of both the CQT and CIT, see infra Part II.D.
  • 79
    • 84866942479 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See, e.g., Barnard, 490 F.2d at 912 (excluding the testimony of a psychiatrist and psychologist called to testify to the likelihood that another witness was telling the truth).
  • 80
    • 84866924847 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Porter, 698 A.2d at 769.
  • 81
    • 84866942478 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Scheffer, 523 U.S. at 313.
  • 82
    • 84866894519 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • 523 U.S. 303.
  • 83
    • 84866942481 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Id. at 306.
  • 84
    • 84866894518 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • The polygraph test that the defendant underwent was a CQT. Id. at 306 & n.1.
  • 85
    • 84860837994 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Note, Daubert's Bipolar Treatment of Scientific Expert Testimony-From Frye's Polygraph to Farwell's Brain Fingerprinting
    • note
    • The Court did not elaborate on the differences between the CQT and the CIT in the opinion, leaving it ambiguous as to whether the holding applies to polygraph tests of all varieties or merely to the polygraph-based CQT. For a discussion of this, see Megan J. Erickson, Note, Daubert's Bipolar Treatment of Scientific Expert Testimony-From Frye's Polygraph to Farwell's Brain Fingerprinting, 55 Drake L. Rev. 763+784-786 (2007).
    • (2007) Drake L. Rev , vol.55
    • Erickson, M.J.1
  • 86
    • 84866924849 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Scheffer, 523 U.S. at 306.
  • 87
    • 84866942438 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Id. at 305+308.
  • 88
    • 84866938929 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id. at 312-314+318 (concurring Justices joining only in the judgment and in Parts I, II.A, and II.D of the opinion).
  • 89
    • 84866942440 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id. at 312-313. This was not the only consideration by which the Court reached its holding. The Court also considered the broad latitude of lawmakers in restricting the presentation of evidence without restricting due process.
  • 90
    • 84866893441 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id. at 308, as well as the lack of scientific consensus regarding the reliability of polygraph tests.
  • 91
    • 84866893440 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id. at 309-311. Though the Court never distinguished between the CQT and CIT, both of which may be conducted using the polygraph, one may assume that the Court was referring only to the CQT, as the literature cited is regarding the CQT.
  • 92
    • 84866942476 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id. at 313 (quoting Aetna Life Ins. Co. v. Ward, 140 U.S. 76, 88 (1891)).
  • 93
    • 84866942442 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id.
  • 94
    • 84866893444 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Id. at 318.
  • 95
    • 84866942439 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id. ("Given the ongoing debate about polygraphs, I agree the rule of exclusion is not so arbitrary or disproportionate that it is unconstitutional.").
  • 96
    • 84866942444 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id.
  • 97
    • 84866893443 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id. at 327-330 (Stevens, J., dissenting).
  • 98
    • 84866942441 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • 900 N.Y.S.2d 639, 642 (Sup. Ct. 2010).
  • 99
    • 84866894516 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • The Frye test outlines a standard where scientific evidence "must be sufficiently established to have gained general acceptance in the particular field in which it belongs." Frye v. United States, 293 F. 1013, 1014 (D.C. Cir. 1923). For more discussion of this test, see infra Part III.A.
  • 100
    • 84866938931 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See Wilson, 900 N.Y.S.2d at 641-42.
  • 101
    • 84866938932 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id. (quoting People v. Williams, 159 N.E.2d 549, 554 (N.Y. 1959)).
  • 102
    • 84866942443 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See Id. at 642. In rejecting the credibility-assessment evidence, the court stated that "it is well established that unless the jurors are unable or incompetent to evaluate the evidence and draw inferences and conclusions, the opinion of an expert, which intrudes on the province of the jury, is both unnecessary and improper".
  • 103
    • 84866942447 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id. (citing Kulak v. Nationwide Mut. Ins. Co., 351 N.E.2d 735 (1976)).
  • 104
    • 84866938930 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • United States v. Scheffer, 523 U.S. 303, 313 (1998).
  • 105
    • 84866924814 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Wilson, 900 N.Y.S.2d at 642.
  • 106
    • 0005495494 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Procedural Justice
    • note
    • There are other important potential goals of the trial system that may not favor outcome accuracy. For example, there may be a legitimate social goal of procedural justice in the trial system; that is, giving people the opportunity to be heard and maximizing the perceived legitimacy of the judicial system. See Tom R. Tyler & E. Allan Lind, Procedural Justice, in Handbook Of Justice Research In Law 65, 74-88 (Joseph Sanders & V. Lee Hamilton eds., 2001). Maximizing procedural justice could, in some cases, be in conflict with maximizing outcome accuracy. For example, if jurors make less accurate credibility assessments without the aid of expert testimony regarding credibility than they would with the aid of such testimony, their accuracy will be reduced by not admitting the expert testimony. However, the procedural justice may be increased by rejecting the testimony if perceived procedural fairness is strongly tied to receiving a judgment from peers regarding the truthfulness of testimony, rather than from an expert. While it is not clear how procedural justice may be influenced by experts testifying with regard to credibility, a complete discussion of this is beyond the scope of this Comment. The remainder of this Comment will focus on policies that maximize outcome accuracy.
    • Handbook of Justice Research In Law , vol.65 , pp. 74-88
    • Tyler, T.R.1    Allan Lind, E.2
  • 107
    • 84866893445 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The (Real!) Science Behind Fox's Lie to Me
    • note
    • See The (Real!) Science Behind Fox's Lie to Me, Popular Mechanics (Oct. 1, 2009, 12:00 AM), http://www.popularmechanics.com/science/4300722.
    • (2009) Popular Mechanics
  • 108
    • 84866924817 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Lie to Me, Paul Ekman Group, http://awstats.paulekman.com/drupal/?q=Lie-to-me (last visited Aug. 4, 2012) (summarizing the former television show's website).
    • Lie to Me
  • 109
    • 84866924816 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • 487 U.S. 1012, 1019 (1988).
  • 110
    • 84866942446 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See infra Part II.C.
  • 111
    • 84866938933 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See infra text accompanying notes 95-112.
  • 112
    • 77957036209 scopus 로고
    • Verbal and Nonverbal Communication of Deception
    • note
    • See Miron Zuckerman et al., Verbal and Nonverbal Communication of Deception, 14 Advances Experimental Soc. Psychol. 1, 11 (1981).
    • (1981) Advances Experimental Soc. Psychol , vol.14 , Issue.1 , pp. 11
    • Zuckerman, M.1
  • 113
    • 85047693720 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Cues to Deception
    • note
    • Extensive review of these early demeanor studies is beyond the scope of this Comment. For a review of these studies, see Bella M. DePaulo et al., Cues to Deception, 129 Psychol. Bull. 74 (2003),
    • (2003) Psychol. Bull , vol.129 , pp. 74
    • Depaulo, B.M.1
  • 119
    • 0026230791 scopus 로고
    • Who Can Catch a Liar?
    • note
    • Id. at 914-915. The different groups included college students, psychiatrists, judges, robbery investigators, federal polygraphers, and Secret Service agents.
    • (1991) Am. Psychologist , vol.46 , pp. 914-915
    • Ekman, P.1    O'Sullivan, M.2
  • 124
    • 0032852362 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • I'm Innocent!": Effects of Training on Judgments of Truth and Deception in the Interrogation Room
    • note
    • Saul M. Kassin & Christina T. Fong, "I'm Innocent!": Effects of Training on Judgments of Truth and Deception in the Interrogation Room, 23 Law & Hum. Behav. 499+501-503 (1999). Subjects committed one of four possible crimes, such as vandalizing the wall of a university building.
    • (1999) Law & Hum. Behav , vol.23
    • Kassin, S.M.1    Fong, C.T.2
  • 125
    • 84866942448 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id. Innocent acts involved going to the same location, but not committing any crime.
  • 126
    • 0032852362 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • I'm Innocent!": Effects of Training on Judgments of Truth and Deception in the Interrogation Room
    • Id. at 503-504.
    • (1999) Law & Hum. Behav , vol.23 , pp. 503-504
    • Kassin, S.M.1    Fong, C.T.2
  • 129
    • 0004280122 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • The Reid Technique is the leading method of training law enforcement officials to conduct interrogations, detect deception, and elicit confessions. The method is outlined in a book co-authored by John Reid. See Fred E. Inbau et al., Criminal Interrogation and Confessions 187-190 (5th ed. 2013).
    • (2013) Criminal Interrogation and Confessions , pp. 187-190
    • Inbau, F.E.1
  • 130
    • 84866942449 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • SM Kassin & CT Fong, Supra note 95, at 508. It should be noted that this accuracy is less than chance, indicating that the method is actually reducing lie-detection ability, rather than augmenting it.
    • Supra Note , vol.95 , pp. 508
    • Kassin, S.M.1    Fong, C.T.2
  • 131
    • 84866893446 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id. at 509. Confidence was measured by asking each receiver to self-rate his confidence, on a scale from one to ten, after viewing each interrogation.
    • Supra Note , vol.95 , pp. 509
    • Kassin, S.M.1    Fong, C.T.2
  • 132
    • 0036781294 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • He's Guilty!": Investigator Bias in Judgments of Truth and Deception
    • note
    • Christian A. Meissner & Saul M. Kassin, "He's Guilty!": Investigator Bias in Judgments of Truth and Deception, 26 Law & Hum. Behav. 469+474 (2002). The investigators had an average of nearly fourteen years of experience and 68% of the investigators had undergone formal training in the detection of deception.
    • (2002) Law & Hum. Behav , vol.26
    • Meissner, C.A.1    Kassin, S.M.2
  • 136
    • 0005843189 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Meissner and Kassin found this "more liberal response criterion" (i.e., a tendency to assume that others are lying more frequently than telling the truth) in a number of other studies of individuals trained in using demeanor evidence to detect deception. Id. at 472-473 & tbl.1 (citing Paul Ekman et al., A Few Can Catch a Liar, 10 PSYCHOL. SCI. 263 (1999)
  • 138
    • 0033763577 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Truth, Lies, and Videotape: An Investigation of the Ability of Federal Parole Officers to Detect Deception
    • Stephen Porter et al., Truth, Lies, and Videotape: An Investigation of the Ability of Federal Parole Officers to Detect Deception, 24 Law & Hum. Behav. 643 (2000).
    • (2000) Law & Hum. Behav , vol.24 , pp. 643
    • Porter, S.1
  • 139
    • 0002100348 scopus 로고
    • Primary, Secondary, and Meta-Analysis of Research
    • note
    • A meta-analysis is "the statistical analysis of a large collection of analysis results from individual studies for the purpose of integrating the findings." Gene V. Glass, Primary, Secondary, and Meta-Analysis of Research, 5 Educ. Researcher 3+3 (1976). Typically, a meta-analysis uses statistical methods to find the size of a particular effect that has been measured by an entire field of research.
    • (1976) Educ. Researcher , vol.5
    • Glass, G.V.1
  • 143
    • 84872536924 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See Supra note 106.
    • Supra Note , pp. 106
  • 144
    • 84866942450 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • One criticism of training paradigms like the one used by Kassin and Fong is that they are so short that they are virtually guaranteed to be ineffective. However, in the trial context, the thirty minute training session is likely very close to the maximum of what could be employed by the court, given the demands placed on a jury. Even if time constraints were not a factor, the cost of serious training would likely be prohibitive.
  • 145
    • 0344288309 scopus 로고
    • A Wipe of the Hands, a Lick of the Lips: The Validity of Demeanor Evidence in Assessing Witness Credibility
    • note
    • See, e.g., Jeremy A. Blumenthal, A Wipe of the Hands, a Lick of the Lips: The Validity of Demeanor Evidence in Assessing Witness Credibility, 72 Neb. L. Rev. 1157 (1993)
    • (1993) Neb. L. Rev , vol.72 , pp. 1157
    • Blumenthal, J.A.1
  • 146
    • 0006096670 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The Demeanor Gap: Race, Lie Detection, and the Jury
    • Joseph W. Rand, The Demeanor Gap: Race, Lie Detection, and the Jury, 33 Conn. L. Rev. 1 (2000)
    • (2000) Conn. L. Rev , vol.33 , pp. 1
    • Rand, J.W.1
  • 147
    • 77956972713 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • What Makes the Engine Go? Cognitive Limitations and Cross-Examination
    • Chris William Sanchirico, "What Makes the Engine Go?" Cognitive Limitations and Cross-Examination, 14 Widener L. Rev. 507 (2009).
    • (2009) Widener L. Rev , vol.14 , pp. 507
    • Sanchirico, C.W.1
  • 149
    • 77950646245 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Detecting Lies Using Demeanor, Bias, and Context
    • note
    • See Max Minzner, Detecting Lies Using Demeanor, Bias, and Context, 29 Cardozo L. Rev. 2557+2564 (2008) ("Today's findings... are far more complex than the then-current research and give us a much greater ability to identify those situations in which we need to worry about mistaken judgments about credibility and those in which we do not.").
    • (2008) Cardozo L. Rev , vol.29
    • Minzner, M.1
  • 150
    • 77950646245 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Detecting Lies Using Demeanor, Bias, and Context
    • Id. at 2567-2568.
    • (2008) Cardozo L. Rev , vol.29 , pp. 2567-2568
    • Minzner, M.1
  • 151
    • 0036332903 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • How People Really Detect Lies
    • Hee Sun Park et al., How People Really Detect Lies, 69 Comm. Monographs 144+149 (2002).
    • (2002) Comm. Monographs , vol.69
    • Park, H.S.1
  • 152
    • 77950646245 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Detecting Lies Using Demeanor, Bias, and Context
    • Id. at 149-150.
    • (2008) Cardozo L. Rev , vol.29 , pp. 149-150
    • Minzner, M.1
  • 153
    • 77950646245 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Detecting Lies Using Demeanor, Bias, and Context
    • note
    • The question read, "Now, think about how you found out that the person lied to you. Describe in as much detail as you can the events surrounding your discovery of the lie: how exactly did you find out that the person lied to you?" Id. at 150.
    • (2008) Cardozo L. Rev , vol.29 , pp. 150
    • Minzner, M.1
  • 155
    • 34247529903 scopus 로고
    • Availability: A Heuristic for Judging Frequency and Probability
    • note
    • This phenomenon is referred to in the psychological literature as the availability heuristic. For a more complete description, see Amos Tversky & Daniel Kahneman, Availability: A Heuristic for Judging Frequency and Probability, 5 Cognitive Psychol. 207 (1973).
    • (1973) Cognitive Psychol , vol.5 , pp. 207
    • Tversky, A.1    Kahneman, D.2
  • 157
    • 33847056973 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Juror Questions During Trial: A Window into Juror Thinking
    • note
    • Shari Seidman Diamond et al., Juror Questions During Trial: A Window into Juror Thinking, 59 Vand. L. Rev. 1927+1937 (2006). Jurors are allowed to present questions in Arizona state civil cases. The rule provides that "[j]urors shall be permitted to submit to the court written questions directed to witnesses or to the court." ARIZ. R. CIV. P. 39(b)(10) (2011).
    • (2006) Vand. L. Rev , vol.59
    • Diamond, S.S.1
  • 161
  • 163
    • 33644813001 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Detecting Deception via Strategic Disclosure of Evidence
    • Maria Hartwig et al., Detecting Deception via Strategic Disclosure of Evidence, 29 Law & Hum. Behav. 469+473-474 (2005).
    • (2005) Law & Hum. Behav , vol.29
    • Hartwig, M.1
  • 165
    • 33644813001 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Detecting Deception via Strategic Disclosure of Evidence
    • note
    • Id. at 475. The three pieces of evidence claimed against the student were that one witness had seen the student outside the video store, that one witness had seen the student inside the video store, and that the student's fingerprints were contained on the briefcase from which the wallet was stolen.
    • (2005) Law & Hum. Behav , vol.29 , pp. 475
    • Hartwig, M.1
  • 169
    • 84866938935 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See M Minzner, Supra note 115, at 2571 ("What do these results about demeanor, bias, and context indicate about lie detection in the legal system?... When a legal decision-maker possesses private information and uses it to probe the witness's story, lie detection accuracy will improve If the police and juries have multiple independent sources of information about an event, they are far more likely to accurately decide which witnesses to believe.").
    • Supra Note , vol.115 , pp. 2571
    • Minzner, M.1
  • 170
    • 84866942452 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Such knowledge could arise from several different sources. Police may reveal evidence to the suspect during interrogation. See Hartwig, Supra note 128, at 470. Additionally, certain types of evidence must be revealed to the defendant prior to trial, such as documents that the prosecution plans to use in its case-in-chief or the testimony of expert witnesses, which will typically include forensic experts. See FED. R. CRIM. PROC. 16(a)(1)(E)-(F).
  • 171
    • 0347739363 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • M Hartwig, Supra note 128, at 470 ("In an American study, this disclosure of evidence, often together with a suggestion of guilt, was the typical way to start the interrogation, and occurred in more than 80% of the cases." (citing Richard A. Leo, Inside the Interrogation Room, 86 J. CRIM. L. & CRIMINOLOGY 266 (1996))).
    • Supra Note , vol.128 , pp. 470
    • Hartwig, M.1
  • 173
    • 39049186604 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Strategic Use of Evidence During Police Interviews: When Training to Detect Deception Works
    • note
    • Maria Hartwig et al., Strategic Use of Evidence During Police Interviews: When Training to Detect Deception Works, 30 Law & Hum. Behav. 603+608 (2006) ("[The officers] were trained in planning and asking questions concerning the evidence without disclosing it to the suspect. For example, if the case-file included information that a suspect's car had been seen close to a crime scene on the day of the crime, they were taught to plan and ask questions about the suspect's car They then practiced the strategic use of [this] technique on each other several times").
    • (2006) Law & Hum. Behav , vol.30
    • Hartwig, M.1
  • 174
    • 85061111040 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Strategic Use of Evidence During Police Interviews: When Training to Detect Deception Works
    • Id. at 608-610.
    • (2006) Law & Hum. Behav , vol.30 , pp. 608-610
    • Hartwig, M.1
  • 175
    • 85061111066 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Strategic Use of Evidence During Police Interviews: When Training to Detect Deception Works
    • Id. at 610.
    • (2006) Law & Hum. Behav , vol.30 , pp. 610
    • Hartwig, M.1
  • 176
    • 85061111066 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Strategic Use of Evidence During Police Interviews: When Training to Detect Deception Works
    • Id. at 613.
    • (2006) Law & Hum. Behav , vol.30 , pp. 613
    • Hartwig, M.1
  • 177
    • 85061111040 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Strategic Use of Evidence During Police Interviews: When Training to Detect Deception Works
    • Id. at 608.
    • (2006) Law & Hum. Behav , vol.30 , pp. 608
    • Hartwig, M.1
  • 179
    • 84866924819 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • 523 U.S. 303, 318-19 (1998) (Kennedy, J., concurring in part and concurring in the judgment) ("[I]t seems the principal opinion overreaches when it rests its holding on the additional ground that the jury's role in making credibility determinations is diminished when it hears polygraph evidence [It] demeans and mistakes the role and competence of jurors").
  • 180
    • 84866924821 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See, e.g., McClesky v. Kemp, 481 U.S. 279, 287, 291 n.7 (1987) (accepting a social science study as demonstrating that "black defendants... who kill white victims have the greatest likelihood of receiving the death penalty" but reading the results narrowly and as insufficient to support a constitutional challenge).
  • 181
    • 84866924822 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Wilson v. Corestaff Servs. L.P., 900 N.Y.S.2d 639, 642 (Sup. Ct. 2010).
  • 182
    • 84866893450 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See Supra note 45 and accompanying text.
  • 183
    • 84866893448 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See Erikson, Supra note 61, at 785-786.
    • Supra Note , vol.61 , pp. 785-786
    • Erikson1
  • 184
    • 84866924820 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See, e.g., Scheffer, 523 U.S. at 309 ("[T]here is simply no consensus that polygraph evidence is reliable." (emphasis added)).
  • 185
    • 84866924823 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See W Iacono, Supra note 24, at 689 ("The [CIT]... has been developed and promoted by Lykken as a scientifically based alternative to the CQT.")
    • Supra Note , vol.24 , pp. 689
    • Iacono, W.1
  • 186
    • 84866938937 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • D Lykken, Supra note 45, at 385 ("Use of physiological measurements to detect not lying, but the presence of 'guilty knowledge,' requires... [a] more reasonable assumption [than those required by the CQT]."). Notably, the very first paragraph of the very first CIT study ever published focused on the shortcomings of more traditional "lie detector" tests.
    • Supra Note , vol.45 , pp. 385
    • Lykken, D.1
  • 188
    • 84866924824 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Compare W Iacono & D Lykken, Supra note 20, § 40:45-119 (writing a chapter titled "The Case Against Polygraph Tests," describing shortcomings of the CQT and related tests and instead favoring the CIT)
    • Supra Note , vol.20 , Issue.40 , pp. 45-119
    • Iacono, W.1    Lykken, D.2
  • 189
    • 84866938938 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • CR Honts et al., Supra note 33, § 40:20-44 (writing a countering chapter entitled "The Case for Polygraph Tests," arguing in favor of the CQT and related tests' validity).
    • Supra Note , vol.33 , Issue.40 , pp. 20-44
    • Honts, C.R.1
  • 190
    • 38949202261 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The Role of Psychophysiology in Forensic Assessments: Deception Detection, ERPs, and Virtual Reality Mock Crime Scenarios
    • note
    • See, e.g., Ralf Mertens & John J.B. Allen, The Role of Psychophysiology in Forensic Assessments: Deception Detection, ERPs, and Virtual Reality Mock Crime Scenarios, 45 Psychophysiology 286+286 (2008) ("Conventional field polygraph examinations, based on the control-question technique (CQT), suffer from many limitations and have been widely criticized in the scientific literature.").
    • (2008) Psychophysiology , vol.45
    • Mertens, R.1    Allen John, J.B.2
  • 193
    • 84866942454 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Scheffer, 523 U.S. at 313 (quoting United States v. Barnard, 490 F.2d 907, 912 (9th Cir. 1973), cert. denied, 416 U.S. 959 (1974)).
  • 194
    • 84866924826 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • 293 F. 1013, 1013-14 (D.C. Cir. 1923) (ruling that the "systolic blood pressure deception test," a precursor to the polygraph-based CQT, had not gained "general acceptance" among physiological and psychological authorities and therefore should not be admitted as evidence).
  • 195
    • 84866893452 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id. at 1014.
  • 196
    • 84866893451 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • 509 U.S. 579 (1993).
  • 197
    • 84866942457 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Id. at 586.
  • 198
    • 84866942474 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id. at 588 ("Nothing in the text of this Rule establishes 'general acceptance' as an absolute prerequisite to its admissibility... [nor] present[s] any clear indication that Rule 702 or the Rules as a whole were intended to incorporate a 'general acceptance' standard.").
  • 199
    • 84866924825 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • FED. R. EVID. 702 committee note (1975).
  • 200
    • 84866938940 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Preliminary Draft of Proposed Amendments to the Federal Rules of Civil Procedure and the Federal Rules of Evidence, 137 F.R.D. 53, 157 (1991) (describing the proposed amendment to FED. R. EVID. 702 and the accompanying committee notes).
  • 201
    • 84866938939 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Daubert, 509 U.S. at 587-88. Rule 401 is a highly lenient rule of relevancy, requiring only that evidence have "any tendency to make the existence of any fact that is of consequence to the determination of the action more probable or less probable than it would be without the evidence." FED. R. EVID. 401. This rule has been repeatedly determined to be a liberal standard.
  • 203
    • 84866942456 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Daubert, 509 U.S. at 589 ("That austere standard, absent from, and incompatible with, the Federal Rules of Evidence, should not be applied in federal trials.").
  • 204
    • 84866942460 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Id. at 593-594.
  • 205
    • 84866938943 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Id. at 593.
  • 206
    • 84866924842 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • This criterion was specifically mentioned by the Court as being "relevant, though not dispositive" because "some propositions... are... too new, or of too limited interest to be published." Id. at 593-594.
  • 207
    • 84866924827 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id. at 594. The court did not give any guidelines as to what would constitute an acceptable rate of error.
  • 208
    • 84866938942 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id. This factor is the very same test from Frye that the Court declared was no longer controlling. The Court explained the factor by stating that the Frye test was unnecessarily strict because a "reliability assessment does not require... an express determination of a particular degree of acceptance within the community."
  • 209
    • 84866942459 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id. (quoting United States v. Downing, 753 F.2d 1224, 1238 (3d Cir. 1985)). But this factor can have a bearing in the eventual calculus because a known technique that has not been accepted in its field should be viewed with skepticism.
  • 210
    • 84866942458 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • JB Weinstein & MA Berger, Supra note 162, § 13.02[4][a]. The rule also codifies the Supreme Court's holding in Kumho Tire Co. v. Carmichael, 526 U.S. 137 (1999), that Daubert's principles apply to all expert testimony admissible under Rule 702. FED. R. EVID. 702 committee note (2000). While the Daubert standard has been codified into the Federal Rules of Evidence and is therefore used in all federal courts, some state courts that have not adopted the rules still use the Frye test in determinations of the admissibility of scientific evidence.
    • Supra Note , vol.162
    • Weinstein, J.B.1    Berger, M.A.2
  • 211
    • 84866893454 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See, e.g., Grady v. Frito-Lay, Inc., 839 A.2d 1038, 1045 (Pa. 2003) (requiring general acceptance of an expert's methods).
  • 212
    • 84866893453 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • FED. R. EVID. 702.
  • 214
    • 84866942461 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • As of the writing of this Comment, no federal court has held a formal Daubert hearing regarding the admissibility of the CIT.
  • 215
    • 38749133696 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Detecting Deception: The Scope and Limits
    • note
    • Prior to 2000, there had been no published research using fMRI to detect deception. However, from 2000 to 2008, there were fifteen papers published on the subject. See Kamila E. Sip et al., Detecting Deception: The Scope and Limits, 12 Trends Cognitive Sci. 48+51 tbl.1 (2008).
    • (2008) Trends Cognitive Sci , vol.12
    • Sip, K.E.1
  • 216
    • 34548596279 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Lie Detection: Is a "Brainstorm" Heading Toward the "Gatekeeper"?
    • note
    • Many articles focusing on the admissibility of fMRI-based lie detection are pessimistic as to potential admissibility, but some offer countering views. Compare Archie Alexander, Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Lie Detection: Is a "Brainstorm" Heading Toward the "Gatekeeper"?, 7 Hous. J. Health L. & Pol'y 1, 49-55 (2007) (arguing that fMRI-based lie detection is not generally accepted within the field and that the "analytical gap" from experimental studies to practical application is too great)
    • (2007) Hous. J. Health L. & Pol'y , vol.7 , Issue.1 , pp. 49-55
    • Alexander, A.1
  • 217
    • 78049421931 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The Future of Neuroimaged Lie Detection and the Law
    • note
    • Joëlle Anne Moreno, The Future of Neuroimaged Lie Detection and the Law, 42 Akron L. Rev. 717+734-736 (2009) (describing the potential uses and limits of cognitive neuroscience in the courtroom)
    • (2009) Akron L. Rev , vol.42
    • Moreno, J.A.1
  • 218
    • 84866889920 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • J Moriarty, Supra note 31, at 758-761 (2009) (arguing that the number of fMRI lie-detection studies is too small to be considered reliable, that there has been too little replication of past research, and that these studies are not close enough to real-life situations to be applicable)
    • (2009) Supra Note , vol.31 , pp. 758-761
    • Moriarty, J.1
  • 219
    • 84866912722 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Note, Lie Detection: A Changing of the Guard in the Quest for Truth in Court?
    • note
    • Cooper Ellenberg, Note, Lie Detection: A Changing of the Guard in the Quest for Truth in Court?, 33 Law & Psychol. Rev. 139+147 (2009) (stating that fMRI-based lie detection does not currently have the necessary accuracy rates to reach admissibility but that there is potential for future admissibility)
    • (2009) Law & Psychol. Rev , vol.33
    • Ellenberg, C.1
  • 220
    • 81255194290 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Note, Admissibility of fMRI Lie Detection: The Cultural Bias Against "Mind Reading" Devices
    • note
    • Leo Kittay, Note, Admissibility of fMRI Lie Detection: The Cultural Bias Against "Mind Reading" Devices, 72 Brook. L. Rev. 1351+1376-1379 (2007) (stating that the general widespread acceptance of fMRI research meets the general acceptance test and that the error rate for fMRI-based lie detection is lower than that of other forensic evidence that is regularly admitted).
    • (2007) Brook. L. Rev , vol.72
    • Kittay, L.1
  • 221
    • 84866924830 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • I should also note that some of the studies described here do not follow the traditional CQT format, but are instead hybrids between the CQT and other types of lie-detection protocols. While a more nuanced approach would be necessary for a complete assessment of the admissibility of these tests, the critical distinction here is that these fMRI-based tests all seek to determine whether a person is being truthful, as the CQT does, instead of determining whether a person recognizes information related to a crime, as the CIT does. Thus, I loosely use the term CQT to refer to these fMRI-based CQT-like tests.
  • 222
    • 34548555340 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Neuroscience-Based Lie Detection: The Urgent Need for Regulation
    • note
    • Henry T. Greely & Judy Illes, Neuroscience-Based Lie Detection: The Urgent Need for Regulation, 33 Am. J.L. & Med. 377+413 (2007) ("The federal government-or, barring that, state governments-should ban any non-research use of new methods of lie detection, including specifically fMRI-based lie detection, unless or until the method has been proven safe and effective to the satisfaction of a regulatory agency and has been vetted through the peer-reviewed scientific literature.").
    • (2007) Am. J.L. & Med , vol.33
    • Greely, H.T.1    Illes, J.2
  • 223
    • 84866893456 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See sources cited Supra note 174.
    • Supra Note , vol.174
  • 224
    • 84866893455 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • A Google Scholar search for "fMRI" yields thousands of citations in scholarly journals, and fMRI is regularly used in neuroscience research.
  • 225
    • 84866924829 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See, e.g., Leo Kittay, Supra note 174, at 1383.
    • Supra Note , vol.174 , pp. 1383
    • Kittay, L.1
  • 226
    • 84866924828 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • For example, Frye held that a systolic blood pressure test to measure deception was not generally accepted despite the fact that the measurement of blood pressure was undoubtedly widely accepted in medicine at the time.
  • 227
    • 84866938946 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See Frye v. United States, 293 F. 1013, 1014 (D.C. Cir. 1923).
  • 228
    • 84866938944 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See FA Kozel et al., Supra note 35, at 610 (describing a 90% accuracy rate)
    • Supra Note , vol.35 , pp. 610
    • Kozel, F.A.1
  • 229
    • 84866942462 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • DD Langleben et al., Supra note 35, at 269 (reporting a 78% accuracy rate). Additionally, these studies have only occurred in lab settings and have never tested real-world deception.
    • Supra Note , vol.35 , pp. 269
    • Langleben, D.D.1
  • 230
    • 84866924831 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • The Daubert opinion only mentions that courts "should consider the known or potential rate of error." Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharm., Inc., 509 U.S. 579, 594 (1993). The case is silent, however, as to what rate of error is sufficient to meet the test.
  • 231
    • 84866942463 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See L Kittay, Supra note 174, at 1382. A number of types of commonly admitted forensic evidence have recently come under scrutiny for being inaccurate. A 2009 report from the National Research Council called for a major overhaul of the forensic sciences, stating that: "In a number of forensic science disciplines, forensic science professionals have yet to establish either the validity of their approach or the accuracy of their conclusions "Nat'l Research Council, Strengthening Forensic Science In The United States: A Path Forward 53 (2009).
    • Supra Note , vol.174 , pp. 1382
    • Kittay, L.1
  • 232
    • 84866894505 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Cf. United States v. Llera Plaza, 188 F. Supp. 2d 549, 571-72 (E.D. Pa. 2002) (recognizing the potential for inaccurate fingerprint evidence, but still allowing its use by an expert at trial).
  • 233
    • 84866938949 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • One related problem may be that the probative accuracy may not be enough to overcome the prejudicial influence of the test. Rule 403 provides that "[a]lthough relevant, evidence may be excluded if its probative value is substantially outweighed by the danger of unfair prejudice." FED. R. EVID. 403. Particularly in unilaterally obtained lie-detection tests, where the test is sought by one of the parties and will not be used if the outcome is not to that party's liking, the probative value is small because the party has nothing to lose by taking the test.
  • 235
    • 84866924833 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • A Westlaw search of the "All State and Federal Cases" database conducted on July 6, 2012, for "concealed information test" yielded only one result. A search in the same database for "guilty knowledge test" (a synonymous term for the CIT) yielded seven additional responses. A search for "polygraph" on the same database returned over 10,000 cases.
  • 236
    • 84866938948 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • No. PCCV 073247, at *5-10 (Iowa Dist. Ct. Mar. 5, 2001).
  • 237
    • 84866924832 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • 105 P.3d 832, 835-36 (Okla. Crim. App. 2005).
  • 238
    • 84866942464 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • The Slaughter court noted that "Dr. Farwell makes certain claims about the Brain Fingerprinting test that are not supported by anything other than his bare affidavit." Id. at 834-835.
  • 239
    • 84866938947 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • The court also opined that there was no evidence that Brain Fingerprinting had been tested, subjected to peer-review, or otherwise met the Daubert factors and thus would fail such an analysis. Id. at 836.
  • 240
    • 84866942465 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Brain Fingerprinting, http://www.governmentworks.com/bws/ (last visited Aug. 15, 2012). A brain fingerprinting test is similar to other P300-based CITs. A subject is shown probe items that relate to a particular crime or event and irrelevant items of the same class that do not relate to the crime or event. A larger P300 is expected to occur to probe items if the subject has knowledge of the crime or event in question.
    • (2012)
  • 241
    • 0035113257 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Using Brain MERMER Testing to Detect Knowledge Despite Efforts to Conceal
    • note
    • See Lawrence A. Farwell & Sharon S. Smith, Using Brain MERMER Testing to Detect Knowledge Despite Efforts to Conceal, 46 J. Forensic Sci. 135+136 (2001). However, in addition to the P300, brain fingerprinting uses a secondary brain response, termed by Farwell as the MERMER effect.
    • (2001) J. Forensic Sci , vol.46
    • Farwell, L.A.1    Smith, S.S.2
  • 242
    • 84866942469 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See Id. at 135. The basis for the MERMER effect is never explained by Farwell as it is the proprietary secret of the Brain Fingerprinting company.
  • 243
    • 33748976908 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • 'Brain Fingerprinting': A Critical Analysis
    • note
    • See J. Peter Rosenfeld, 'Brain Fingerprinting': A Critical Analysis, 4 Sci. Rev. Mental Health Prac. 20+23 (2005).
    • (2005) Sci. Rev. Mental Health Prac , vol.4
    • Rosenfeld, J.P.1
  • 244
    • 84866924835 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Some articles have discussed the admissibility of the P300-based CIT under Daubert, but have not discussed each factor in detail. See, e.g., Erikson, Supra note 61
    • Supra Note , vol.61
    • Erikson1
  • 245
    • 84866894504 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Waiting in the Wings? The Admissibility of Neuroimagery for Lie Detection
    • note
    • Eric K. Gerard, Waiting in the Wings? The Admissibility of Neuroimagery for Lie Detection, 27 Dev. Mental Health L., July 2008, at 1.
    • (2008) Dev. Mental Health L , vol.27
    • Gerard, E.K.1
  • 246
    • 84866942467 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharm., Inc., 509 U.S. 579, 593 (1993) ("[T]he criterion of the scientific status of a theory is its falsifiability, or refutability, or testability." (quoting KARL R. POPPER, CONJECTURES AND REFUTATIONS: THE GROWTH OF SCIENTIFIC KNOWLEDGE 37 (5th ed. 1989) (emphasis omitted))).
  • 247
    • 84866942466 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • A science is generally understood to be falsifiable when it is "capable of empirical test" such that the theories on which the method is based could be objectively proven wrong. See Id. (quoting CARL GUSTAV HEMPEL, PHILOSOPHY OF NATURAL SCIENCE 49 (1966)).
  • 248
  • 249
    • 84866938950 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Supra note 20, § 1:16.
    • Supra Note , vol.20 , Issue.1 , pp. 16
  • 250
    • 84866938952 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Ground truth is known in a CIT where the researchers know, a priori, whether a participant possesses the concealed information they are attempting to detect. Thus, the researchers can determine whether their test came to the correct result, unlike in cases where ground truth is not known.
  • 255
    • 84866894513 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Daubert, 509 U.S. at 593.
  • 256
    • 84866924843 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • A search on Google Scholar for "P300-based concealed information test" and "P300-based guilty knowledge test" conducted on July 6, 2012, revealed more than fifty peer-reviewed publications of empirical tests of the CIT on only the first five pages of results, and many more papers have undoubtedly been published. These publications have come from a number of labs both within the United States and abroad, indicating peer-reviewed replication of the validity of the CIT.
  • 259
    • 78951493092 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Mock Crime Application of the Complex Trial Protocol (CTP) P300-Based Concealed Information Test
    • note
    • Michael R. Winograd & J. Peter Rosenfeld, Mock Crime Application of the Complex Trial Protocol (CTP) P300-Based Concealed Information Test, 48 Psychophysiology 155 (2010). It should be noted that the research coming from this lab is, in part, my own. However, other labs have also tested the P300-based CIT.
    • (2010) Psychophysiology , vol.48 , pp. 155
    • Winograd, M.R.1    Rosenfeld, J.P.2
  • 260
    • 0026733780 scopus 로고
    • The Identification of Concealed Memories Using the Event-Related Potential and Implicit Behavioral Measures: A Methodology for Prediction in the Face of Individual Differences
    • note
    • See, e.g., John J. Allen et al., The Identification of Concealed Memories Using the Event-Related Potential and Implicit Behavioral Measures: A Methodology for Prediction in the Face of Individual Differences, 29 Psychophysiology 504 (1992)
    • (1992) Psychophysiology , vol.29
    • Allen, J.J.1
  • 262
    • 69049101033 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The Role of Intention to Conceal in the P300-Based Concealed Information Test
    • Kenta Kubo & Hiroshi Nittono, The Role of Intention to Conceal in the P300-Based Concealed Information Test, 34 Applied Psychophysiology & Biofeedback 227 (2009).
    • (2009) Applied Psychophysiology & Biofeedback , vol.34 , pp. 227
    • Kubo, K.1    Nittono, H.2
  • 263
    • 84866924836 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See Harrington v. State, No. PCCV 073247, at *8-9 (Iowa Dist. Ct. Mar. 5, 2001). A full explanation of the basis of the MERMER algorithm and the difference between Brain Fingerprinting and the more mainstream P300-based CIT is beyond the scope of this Comment. For an excellent discussion of this topic, see Rosenfeld, Supra note 190.
  • 264
    • 84866938951 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Harrington, No. PCCV 073247, at *9.
  • 265
    • 84866894508 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Id.
  • 267
    • 0031533424 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The Validity of the Lie Detector: Two Surveys of Scientific Opinion
    • note
    • See, e.g., W.G. Iacono & D.T. Lykken, The Validity of the Lie Detector: Two Surveys of Scientific Opinion, 82 J. Applied Psychol. 426+426-428 (1997).
    • (1997) J. Applied Psychol , vol.82
    • Iacono, W.G.1    Lykken, D.T.2
  • 280
    • 1942536610 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Simple, Effective Countermeasures to P300-Based Tests of Detection of Concealed Information
    • note
    • J. Peter Rosenfeld et al., Simple, Effective Countermeasures to P300-Based Tests of Detection of Concealed Information, 41 Psychophysiology 205+209 & tbl.1 (2004). One recent test reported an accuracy rate as low as 50%.
    • (2004) Psychophysiology , vol.41
    • Rosenfeld, J.P.1
  • 282
    • 84866924823 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • See WG Iacono, Supra note 24, at 689 ("A properly administered [CIT] with a sufficient number of items has almost no chance of producing a false positive outcome.").
    • Supra Note , vol.24 , pp. 689
    • Iacono, W.G.1
  • 283
    • 84866924838 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • JB Meixner & JP Rosenfeld, Supra note 44, at 151 tbl.1. There is no reason that other CITs could not follow the same approach; one could employ a block for every crime-related detail that a suspect is likely to recognize.
    • Supra Note , vol.44 , pp. 151
    • Meixner, J.B.1    Rosenfeld, J.P.2
  • 285
    • 0000329018 scopus 로고
    • Detection of Guilty Knowledge in Real-Life Criminal Investigations
    • note
    • Eitan Elaad, Detection of Guilty Knowledge in Real-Life Criminal Investigations, 75 J. Applied Psychol. 521+526 (1990) (correctly detecting only 50% of guilty individuals).
    • (1990) J. Applied Psychol , vol.75
    • Elaad, E.1
  • 286
    • 84866938956 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • United States v. Semrau, No. 07-10074 Ml/P, 2010 WL 6845092, at *11 (W.D. Tenn. June 1, 2010) ("[T]here are no known error rates for fMRI-based lie detection outside the laboratory setting, i.e. in the 'real-world' or 'real-life' setting.").
  • 288
    • 84866942471 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Supra note 79 (discussing potential goals of the trial system that are not related to outcome accuracy).
    • Supra Note , vol.79


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.