메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 38, Issue 1, 2012, Pages 80-89

Compressing perceived distance with remote tool-use: Real, imagined, and remembered

Author keywords

Affordances; Distance perception; Embodied cognition; Intention; Tool use

Indexed keywords

ADULT; ARTICLE; BEHAVIOR; COMPARATIVE STUDY; DISTANCE PERCEPTION; EPISODIC MEMORY; HUMAN; IMAGINATION; NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL TEST; PERCEPTION DISORDER; PHYSIOLOGY; RECALL;

EID: 84862860325     PISSN: 00961523     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1037/a0024981     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (55)

References (56)
  • 1
    • 42249091913 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Altered vision near the hands
    • doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.09.006
    • Abrams, R. A., Davoli, C. C., Du, F., Knapp, W. K., & Paull, D. (2008). Altered vision near the hands. Cognition, 107, 1035-1047. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.09.006
    • (2008) Cognition , vol.107 , pp. 1035-1047
    • Abrams, R.A.1    Davoli, C.C.2    Du, F.3    Knapp, W.K.4    Paull, D.5
  • 2
    • 77951265970 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Wishful seeing: More desirable objects are seen as closer
    • doi:10.1177/0956797609356283
    • Balcetis, E., & Dunning, D. (2010). Wishful seeing: More desirable objects are seen as closer. Psychological Science, 21, 147-152. doi:10.1177/0956797609356283
    • (2010) Psychological Science , vol.21 , pp. 147-152
    • Balcetis, E.1    Dunning, D.2
  • 3
    • 33947262101 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Embodied preference judgments: Can likeability be driven by the motor system?
    • doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01848.x
    • Beilock, S. L., & Holt, L. E. (2007). Embodied preference judgments: Can likeability be driven by the motor system? Psychological Science, 18, 51-57. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01848.x
    • (2007) Psychological Science , vol.18 , pp. 51-57
    • Beilock, S.L.1    Holt, L.E.2
  • 4
    • 0036635775 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Visual search is modulated by action intentions
    • doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2002.00466.x
    • Bekkering, H., & Neggers, S. F. W. (2002). Visual search is modulated by action intentions. Psychological Science, 13, 370-374. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2002.00466.x
    • (2002) Psychological Science , vol.13 , pp. 370-374
    • Bekkering, H.1    Neggers, S.F.W.2
  • 5
    • 0034044716 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • When far becomes near: Remapping of space by tool use
    • doi:10.1162/089892900562237
    • Berti, A., & Frassinetti, F. (2000). When far becomes near: Remapping of space by tool use. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 415-420. doi:10.1162/089892900562237
    • (2000) Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , vol.12 , pp. 415-420
    • Berti, A.1    Frassinetti, F.2
  • 7
    • 74849123010 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Action goals influence action-specific perception
    • doi:10.3758/PBR.16.6.1100
    • Cañal-Bruland, R., & van der Kamp, J. (2009). Action goals influence action-specific perception. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 1100-1105. doi:10.3758/PBR.16.6.1100
    • (2009) Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , vol.16 , pp. 1100-1105
    • Cañal-Bruland, R.1    van der Kamp, J.2
  • 8
    • 78649836150 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Attention affects visual perceptual processing near the hand
    • doi: 10.1177/0956797610380697
    • Cosman, J. D., & Vecera, S. P. (2010). Attention affects visual perceptual processing near the hand. Psychological Science, 21, 1254-1258. doi: 10.1177/0956797610380697
    • (2010) Psychological Science , vol.21 , pp. 1254-1258
    • Cosman, J.D.1    Vecera, S.P.2
  • 9
    • 13944253676 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Neural representations of graspable objects: Are tools special?
    • doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.10.006
    • Creem-Regehr, S. H., & Lee, J. N. (2005). Neural representations of graspable objects: Are tools special? Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 457-469. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.10.006
    • (2005) Cognitive Brain Research , vol.22 , pp. 457-469
    • Creem-Regehr, S.H.1    Lee, J.N.2
  • 10
    • 0034182973 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The role of the saccade target object in the perception of a visually stable world
    • doi: 10.3758/BF03206914
    • Currie, C. B., Mcconkie, G. W., Carlson-Radvansky, L. A., & Irwin, D. E. (2000). The role of the saccade target object in the perception of a visually stable world. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 673-683. doi: 10.3758/BF03206914
    • (2000) Perception & Psychophysics , vol.62 , pp. 673-683
    • Currie, C.B.1    Mcconkie, G.W.2    Carlson-Radvansky, L.A.3    Irwin, D.E.4
  • 11
    • 62349084294 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Reaching out with the imagination
    • doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02293.x
    • Davoli, C. C., & Abrams, R. A. (2009). Reaching out with the imagination. Psychological Science, 20, 293-295. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02293.x
    • (2009) Psychological Science , vol.20 , pp. 293-295
    • Davoli, C.C.1    Abrams, R.A.2
  • 12
    • 77956465168 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • When meaning matters, look but don't touch: The effects of posture on reading
    • doi:10.3758/MC.38.5.555
    • Davoli, C. C., Du, F., Montana, J., Garverick, S., & Abrams, R. A. (2010). When meaning matters, look but don't touch: The effects of posture on reading. Memory & Cognition, 38, 555-562. doi:10.3758/MC.38.5.555
    • (2010) Memory & Cognition , vol.38 , pp. 555-562
    • Davoli, C.C.1    Du, F.2    Montana, J.3    Garverick, S.4    Abrams, R.A.5
  • 13
    • 13544267577 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Shaping multisensory actionspace with tools: Evidence from patients with cross-modal extinction
    • doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.11.010
    • Farnè, A., Iriki, A., & Làdavas, E. (2005). Shaping multisensory actionspace with tools: Evidence from patients with cross-modal extinction. Neuropsychologia, 43, 238-248. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.11.010
    • (2005) Neuropsychologia , vol.43 , pp. 238-248
    • Farnè, A.1    Iriki, A.2    Làdavas, E.3
  • 14
    • 0034608569 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Dynamic size-change of hand peripersonal space following tool use
    • doi: 10.1097/00001756-200006050-00010
    • Farnè,A.,& Làdavas, E. (2000). Dynamic size-change of hand peripersonal space following tool use. NeuroReport, 11, 1645-1649. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200006050-00010
    • (2000) NeuroReport , vol.11 , pp. 1645-1649
    • Farnè, A.1    Làdavas, E.2
  • 15
    • 0018987536 scopus 로고
    • Levels of equivalence in imagery and perception
    • doi:10.1037/0033-295X.87.2.113
    • Finke, R. A. (1980). Levels of equivalence in imagery and perception. Psychological Review, 87, 113-132. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.87.2.113
    • (1980) Psychological Review , vol.87 , pp. 113-132
    • Finke, R.A.1
  • 16
    • 0022117832 scopus 로고
    • Theories relating mental imagery to perception
    • doi:10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.236
    • Finke, R. A. (1985). Theories relating mental imagery to perception. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 236-259. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.236
    • (1985) Psychological Bulletin , vol.98 , pp. 236-259
    • Finke, R.A.1
  • 17
    • 47549095866 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Embodied language: A review of the role of motor system in language comprehension
    • doi:10.1080/17470210701623605
    • Fischer, M. H., & Zwaan, R. A. (2008). Embodied language: A review of the role of motor system in language comprehension. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 825-850. doi:10.1080/17470210701623605
    • (2008) The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , vol.61 , pp. 825-850
    • Fischer, M.H.1    Zwaan, R.A.2
  • 20
    • 0027771156 scopus 로고
    • A bimodal map of space: Somatosensory receptive fields in the macaque putamen with corresponding visual receptive fields
    • doi:10.1007/BF00228820
    • Graziano, M. S. A., & Gross, C. G. (1993). A bimodal map of space: Somatosensory receptive fields in the macaque putamen with corresponding visual receptive fields. Experimental Brain Research, 97, 96-109. doi:10.1007/BF00228820
    • (1993) Experimental Brain Research , vol.97 , pp. 96-109
    • Graziano, M.S.A.1    Gross, C.G.2
  • 21
    • 0002993998 scopus 로고
    • The representation of extrapersonal space: A possible role for bimodal, visual-tactile neurons
    • M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
    • Graziano, M. S. A., & Gross, C. G. (1995). The representation of extrapersonal space: A possible role for bimodal, visual-tactile neurons. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 1021-1034). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    • (1995) The cognitive neurosciences , pp. 1021-1034
    • Graziano, M.S.A.1    Gross, C.G.2
  • 22
  • 23
    • 0039049591 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The role of fixation position in detecting scene changes across saccades
    • doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00183
    • Henderson, J. M., & Hollingworth, A. (1999). The role of fixation position in detecting scene changes across saccades. Psychological Science, 10, 438-443. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00183
    • (1999) Psychological Science , vol.10 , pp. 438-443
    • Henderson, J.M.1    Hollingworth, A.2
  • 25
    • 0035495020 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning
    • doi:10.1017/S0140525X01000103
    • Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849-937. doi:10.1017/S0140525X01000103
    • (2001) Behavioral and Brain Sciences , vol.24 , pp. 849-937
    • Hommel, B.1    Müsseler, J.2    Aschersleben, G.3    Prinz, W.4
  • 26
    • 0029913509 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurons
    • doi:10.1097/00001756-199610020-00010
    • Iwamura, Y. (1996). Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurons. NeuroReport, 7, 2325-2330. doi:10.1097/00001756-199610020-00010
    • (1996) NeuroReport , vol.7 , pp. 2325-2330
    • Iwamura, Y.1
  • 27
    • 0033991770 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Pseudoneglect: A review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks
    • doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00045-7
    • Jewell, G., & Mccourt, M. E. (2000). Pseudoneglect: A review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38, 93-110. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00045-7
    • (2000) Neuropsychologia , vol.38 , pp. 93-110
    • Jewell, G.1    Mccourt, M.E.2
  • 28
    • 0036776853 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Visuo-tactile links in covert exogenous spatial attention remap across changes in unseen hand posture
    • doi:10.3758/BF03194758
    • Kennett, S., Spence, C., & Driver, J. (2002). Visuo-tactile links in covert exogenous spatial attention remap across changes in unseen hand posture. Perception & Psychophysics, 64, 1083-1094. doi:10.3758/BF03194758
    • (2002) Perception & Psychophysics , vol.64 , pp. 1083-1094
    • Kennett, S.1    Spence, C.2    Driver, J.3
  • 29
    • 0018181568 scopus 로고
    • Memory psychophysics for visual area and length
    • doi:10.3758/BF03197463
    • Kerst, S. M., & Howard, J. H. (1978). Memory psychophysics for visual area and length. Memory & Cognition, 6, 327-335. doi:10.3758/BF03197463
    • (1978) Memory & Cognition , vol.6 , pp. 327-335
    • Kerst, S.M.1    Howard, J.H.2
  • 31
    • 74549124801 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Asymmetrical body perception: A possible role for neural body representations
    • doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02447.x
    • Linkenauger, S. A., Witt, J. K., Bakdash, J. Z., Stefanucci, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2009). Asymmetrical body perception: A possible role for neural body representations. Psychological Science, 20, 1373-1380. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02447.x
    • (2009) Psychological Science , vol.20 , pp. 1373-1380
    • Linkenauger, S.A.1    Witt, J.K.2    Bakdash, J.Z.3    Stefanucci, J.K.4    Proffitt, D.R.5
  • 33
    • 33644960889 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • On the nature of near space: Effects of tool use and the transition to far space
    • doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.09.003
    • Longo, M. R., & Lourenco, S. F. (2006). On the nature of near space: Effects of tool use and the transition to far space. Neuropsychologia, 44, 977-981. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.09.003
    • (2006) Neuropsychologia , vol.44 , pp. 977-981
    • Longo, M.R.1    Lourenco, S.F.2
  • 34
    • 0036984214 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Active tool use with the contralesional hand can reduce cross-modal extinction of touch on that hand
    • doi:10.1093/neucas/8.6.411
    • Maravita, A., Clarke, K., Husain, M., & Driver, J. (2002). Active tool use with the contralesional hand can reduce cross-modal extinction of touch on that hand. NeuroCase, 8, 411-416. doi:10.1093/neucas/8.6.411
    • (2002) NeuroCase , vol.8 , pp. 411-416
    • Maravita, A.1    Clarke, K.2    Husain, M.3    Driver, J.4
  • 35
    • 0035795127 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Reaching with a tool extends visual-tactile interactions into far space: Evidence from cross-modal extinction
    • doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00150-0
    • Maravita, A., Husain, M., Clarke, K., & Driver, J. (2001). Reaching with a tool extends visual-tactile interactions into far space: Evidence from cross-modal extinction. Neuropsychologia, 39, 580-585. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00150-0
    • (2001) Neuropsychologia , vol.39 , pp. 580-585
    • Maravita, A.1    Husain, M.2    Clarke, K.3    Driver, J.4
  • 36
    • 0036181317 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Tool-use changes multimodal spatial interactions between vision and touch in normal humans
    • doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00003-3
    • Maravita, A., Spence, C., Kennett, S., & Driver, J. (2002). Tool-use changes multimodal spatial interactions between vision and touch in normal humans. Cognition, 83, B25-B34. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00003-3
    • (2002) Cognition , vol.83
    • Maravita, A.1    Spence, C.2    Kennett, S.3    Driver, J.4
  • 37
    • 0017817151 scopus 로고
    • Psychophysical functions for perceived and remembered size
    • doi:10.1126/science.635592
    • Moyer, R. S., Bradley, D. R., Sorensen, M. H., Whiting, J. C., & Mansfield, D. P. (1978). Psychophysical functions for perceived and remembered size. Science, 200, 330-332. doi:10.1126/science.635592
    • (1978) Science , vol.200 , pp. 330-332
    • Moyer, R.S.1    Bradley, D.R.2    Sorensen, M.H.3    Whiting, J.C.4    Mansfield, D.P.5
  • 38
    • 0032134590 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Comparing measures of monocular distance perception: Verbal and reaching errors are not correlated
    • doi:10.1037/0096-1523.24.4.1037
    • Pagano, C. C., & Bingham, G. P. (1998). Comparing measures of monocular distance perception: Verbal and reaching errors are not correlated. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(4), 1037-1051. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.24.4.1037
    • (1998) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.24 , Issue.4 , pp. 1037-1051
    • Pagano, C.C.1    Bingham, G.P.2
  • 39
    • 0031062594 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Comparison of two indicators of perceived egocentric distance under full-cue and reduced-cue conditions
    • Philbeck, J. W., & Loomis, J. M. (1997). Comparison of two indicators of perceived egocentric distance under full-cue and reduced-cue conditions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23(1), 72-85.
    • (1997) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.23 , Issue.1 , pp. 72-85
    • Philbeck, J.W.1    Loomis, J.M.2
  • 42
    • 0008734185 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes
    • doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00427.x
    • Rensink, R. A., O'Regan, J. K., & Clark, J. J. (1997). To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological Science, 8, 368-373. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00427.x
    • (1997) Psychological Science , vol.8 , pp. 368-373
    • Rensink, R.A.1    O'Regan, J.K.2    Clark, J.J.3
  • 43
    • 3242884951 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Reaching out to see: Arm position can attenuate human visual loss
    • doi:10.1162/0898929041502698
    • Schendel, K., & Robertson, L. C. (2004). Reaching out to see: Arm position can attenuate human visual loss. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 935-943. doi:10.1162/0898929041502698
    • (2004) Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , vol.16 , pp. 935-943
    • Schendel, K.1    Robertson, L.C.2
  • 45
    • 34447546202 scopus 로고
    • On the psychophysical law
    • doi:10.1037/h0046162
    • Stevens, S. S. (1957). On the psychophysical law. Psychological Review, 64, 153-181. doi:10.1037/h0046162
    • (1957) Psychological Review , vol.64 , pp. 153-181
    • Stevens, S.S.1
  • 47
    • 34547566590 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Planning to reach for an object changes how the reacher perceives it
    • doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01965.x
    • Vishton, P. M., Stephens, N. J., Nelson, L. A., Morra, S. E., Brunick, K. L., & Stevens, J. A. (2007). Planning to reach for an object changes how the reacher perceives it. Psychological Science, 18, 713-719. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01965.x
    • (2007) Psychological Science , vol.18 , pp. 713-719
    • Vishton, P.M.1    Stephens, N.J.2    Nelson, L.A.3    Morra, S.E.4    Brunick, K.L.5    Stevens, J.A.6
  • 50
    • 76549106060 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Kicking to bigger uprights: Field goal kicking performance influences perceived size
    • doi:10.1068/p6325
    • Witt, J. K., & Dorsch, T. E. (2009). Kicking to bigger uprights: Field goal kicking performance influences perceived size. Perception, 38, 1328-1340. doi:10.1068/p6325
    • (2009) Perception , vol.38 , pp. 1328-1340
    • Witt, J.K.1    Dorsch, T.E.2
  • 51
    • 50949133085 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Putting to a bigger hole: Golf performance relates to perceived size
    • doi:10.3758/PBR.15.3.581
    • Witt, J. K., Linkenauger, S. A., Bakdash, J. Z., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). Putting to a bigger hole: Golf performance relates to perceived size. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 581-585. doi:10.3758/PBR.15.3.581
    • (2008) Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , vol.15 , pp. 581-585
    • Witt, J.K.1    Linkenauger, S.A.2    Bakdash, J.Z.3    Proffitt, D.R.4
  • 52
    • 57749192754 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Action-specific Influences on Distance Perception: A Role for Motor Simulation
    • doi: 10.1037/a0010781
    • Witt, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). Action-specific Influences on Distance Perception: A Role for Motor Simulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 1479-1492. doi: 10.1037/a0010781
    • (2008) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.34 , pp. 1479-1492
    • Witt, J.K.1    Proffitt, D.R.2
  • 53
    • 3042736094 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Perceiving distance: A role of effort and intent
    • doi:10.1068/p5090
    • Witt, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., & Epstein, W. (2004). Perceiving distance: A role of effort and intent. Perception, 33, 570-590. doi:10.1068/p5090
    • (2004) Perception , vol.33 , pp. 570-590
    • Witt, J.K.1    Proffitt, D.R.2    Epstein, W.3
  • 56
    • 78549263695 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Performance and ease influence speed
    • doi:10.1068/p6699
    • Witt, J. K., & Sugovic, M. (2010). Performance and ease influence speed. Perception, 39, 1341-1353. doi:10.1068/p6699
    • (2010) Perception , vol.39 , pp. 1341-1353
    • Witt, J.K.1    Sugovic, M.2


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.