-
1
-
-
0000656011
-
The scattering of α and β particles by matter and the structure of the atom
-
E. Rutherford, The scattering of α and β particles by matter and the structure of the atom, Philos. Mag. 6, 21 (1911).
-
(1911)
Philos. Mag
, vol.6
, pp. 21
-
-
Rutherford, E.1
-
2
-
-
34250720411
-
-
J. C. Maxwell, in The Scientific Papers of James Clerk Maxwell, edited by W. D. Niven aCambridge University Press, Cambridge, U.K., 1890; Dover, New York, 1952, pp. 367-369.
-
J. C. Maxwell, in The Scientific Papers of James Clerk Maxwell, edited by W. D. Niven aCambridge University Press, Cambridge, U.K., 1890; Dover, New York, 1952), pp. 367-369.
-
-
-
-
3
-
-
0031516327
-
How analogies foster learning from science texts
-
M. K. Iding, How analogies foster learning from science texts, Instr. Sci. 25, 233 (1997).
-
(1997)
Instr. Sci
, vol.25
, pp. 233
-
-
Iding, M.K.1
-
4
-
-
48749148075
-
Structure-mapping: A theoretical framework for analogy
-
D. Gentner, Structure-mapping: A theoretical framework for analogy, Cogn. Sci. 7, 155 (1983).
-
(1983)
Cogn. Sci
, vol.7
, pp. 155
-
-
Gentner, D.1
-
5
-
-
0034548454
-
Structure mapping in the comparison process
-
A. B. Markman and D. Gentner, Structure mapping in the comparison process, Am. J. Psychol. 113, 501 (2000).
-
(2000)
Am. J. Psychol
, vol.113
, pp. 501
-
-
Markman, A.B.1
Gentner, D.2
-
6
-
-
0000007251
-
Overcoming misconceptions via analogical reasoning: Abstract transfer versus explanatory model construction
-
D. E. Brown and J. Clement, Overcoming misconceptions via analogical reasoning: Abstract transfer versus explanatory model construction, Instr. Sci. 18, 237 (1989).
-
(1989)
Instr. Sci
, vol.18
, pp. 237
-
-
Brown, D.E.1
Clement, J.2
-
8
-
-
0004223412
-
-
edited by S. Vosniadou and A. Ortony Cambridge University :Press, Cambridge, U.K
-
R. J. Spiro, P. J. Feltovich, R. L. Coulson, and D. K. Anderson, in Similarity and Analogical Reasoning, edited by S. Vosniadou and A. Ortony (Cambridge University :Press, Cambridge, U.K., 1989).
-
(1989)
Similarity and Analogical Reasoning
-
-
Spiro, R.J.1
Feltovich, P.J.2
Coulson, R.L.3
Anderson, D.K.4
-
9
-
-
33746441053
-
Use of analogy in learning physics: The role of representations
-
N. S. Podolefsky and N. D. Finkelstein, Use of analogy in learning physics: The role of representations, Phys. Rev. ST Phys. Educ. Res. 2, 020101 (2006).
-
(2006)
Phys. Rev. ST Phys. Educ. Res
, vol.2
, pp. 020101
-
-
Podolefsky, N.S.1
Finkelstein, N.D.2
-
10
-
-
0004196619
-
-
edited by D. Gentner and A. Stevens Lawrence Erlbaum Press, Hillsdale, NJ
-
D. Gentner and D. R. Gentner, in Mental Models, edited by D. Gentner and A. Stevens (Lawrence Erlbaum Press, Hillsdale, NJ, 1983).
-
(1983)
Mental Models
-
-
Gentner, D.1
Gentner, D.R.2
-
11
-
-
0004289389
-
-
2nd ed, edited by A. Ortony Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, U.K
-
G. Lakoff, in Metaphor and Thought, 2nd ed., edited by A. Ortony (Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, U.K., 1993).
-
(1993)
Metaphor and Thought
-
-
Lakoff, G.1
-
14
-
-
0013073402
-
-
edited by D. Gentner, K. J. Holyoak, and B. N. Kokinov MIT Press, Cambridge, MA
-
G. Fauconnier, In The Analogical Mind, edited by D. Gentner, K. J. Holyoak, and B. N. Kokinov (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 2001).
-
(2001)
The Analogical Mind
-
-
Fauconnier, G.1
-
15
-
-
0346097025
-
Complexities of graphical representations during lectures: A phenomenological approach
-
W. M. Roth and G. M. Bowen, Complexities of graphical representations during lectures: A phenomenological approach, Learn. Instr. 9, 235 (1999).
-
(1999)
Learn. Instr
, vol.9
, pp. 235
-
-
Roth, W.M.1
Bowen, G.M.2
-
17
-
-
0039260303
-
Facilitating conceptual change using analogies and explanatory models
-
D. E. Brown, Facilitating conceptual change using analogies and explanatory models, Int. J. Sci. Educ. 16, 201 (1994).
-
(1994)
Int. J. Sci. Educ
, vol.16
, pp. 201
-
-
Brown, D.E.1
-
18
-
-
84982556581
-
Analogical problem solving
-
M. L. Gick and K. J. Holyoak, Analogical problem solving, Cogn. Psychol. 12, 306 (1980).
-
(1980)
Cogn. Psychol
, vol.12
, pp. 306
-
-
Gick, M.L.1
Holyoak, K.J.2
-
19
-
-
0003081266
-
Schema induction and analogical transfer
-
M. L. Gick and K. J. Holyoak, Schema induction and analogical transfer, Cogn. Psychol. 15, 1 (1983).
-
(1983)
Cogn. Psychol
, vol.15
, pp. 1
-
-
Gick, M.L.1
Holyoak, K.J.2
-
20
-
-
0002167651
-
-
edited by G. Forman and P. B. Pufall Lawerence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ
-
A. A. diSessa, in Constructivism in the Computer Age, edited by G. Forman and P. B. Pufall (Lawerence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1988).
-
(1988)
Constructivism in the Computer Age
-
-
diSessa, A.A.1
-
21
-
-
33646014253
-
-
edited by J. Mestre aInformation Age Publishing, Greenwich, CT
-
D. Hammer, A. Elby, R. F. Scherr, and E. F. Redish, in Transfer of Learning from a Modern Multidisciplinary Perspective, edited by J. Mestre aInformation Age Publishing, Greenwich, CT, 2005), pp. 89-120.
-
(2005)
Transfer of Learning from a Modern Multidisciplinary Perspective
, pp. 89-120
-
-
Hammer, D.1
Elby, A.2
Scherr, R.F.3
Redish, E.F.4
-
22
-
-
58149410841
-
Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices
-
M. Chi, P. Feltovich, and R. Glaser, Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices, Cogn. Sci. 5, 121 (1981).
-
(1981)
Cogn. Sci
, vol.5
, pp. 121
-
-
Chi, M.1
Feltovich, P.2
Glaser, R.3
-
23
-
-
0035639244
-
Learning by analogical bootstrapping
-
K. J. Kurtz, C. Mao, and D. Gentner, Learning by analogical bootstrapping, J. Learn. Sci. 10, 417 (2001).
-
(2001)
J. Learn. Sci
, vol.10
, pp. 417
-
-
Kurtz, K.J.1
Mao, C.2
Gentner, D.3
-
24
-
-
84925120231
-
Making a silk purse out of two sow's ears: Childrens' use of comparison in category learning
-
L. L. Namy and D. Gentner, Making a silk purse out of two sow's ears: Childrens' use of comparison in category learning, J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 131, 5 (2002).
-
(2002)
J. Exp. Psychol. Gen
, vol.131
, pp. 5
-
-
Namy, L.L.1
Gentner, D.2
-
25
-
-
0037780076
-
The transfer of abstract principles governing complex adaptive systems
-
R. L. Goldstone and Y. Sakamoto, The transfer of abstract principles governing complex adaptive systems, Cogn. Psychol. 46, 414 (2003).
-
(2003)
Cogn. Psychol
, vol.46
, pp. 414
-
-
Goldstone, R.L.1
Sakamoto, Y.2
-
26
-
-
25644452766
-
The advantage of simple symbols for learning and transfer
-
V. M. Sloutsky, J. A. Kaminsky, and A. F. Heckler, The advantage of simple symbols for learning and transfer, Psychon. Bull. Rev. 12, 508 (2005).
-
(2005)
Psychon. Bull. Rev
, vol.12
, pp. 508
-
-
Sloutsky, V.M.1
Kaminsky, J.A.2
Heckler, A.F.3
-
27
-
-
84902523835
-
Student representational competence and self-assessment when solving physics problems
-
P. B. Kohl and N. D. Finkelstein, Student representational competence and self-assessment when solving physics problems, Phys. Rev. ST Phys. Educ. Res. 1, 010104 (2005).
-
(2005)
Phys. Rev. ST Phys. Educ. Res
, vol.1
, pp. 010104
-
-
Kohl, P.B.1
Finkelstein, N.D.2
-
28
-
-
20044365090
-
Relation between students' problem-solving performance and representational mode
-
D. E. Meltzer, Relation between students' problem-solving performance and representational mode, Am. J. Phys. 73, 463 (2005).
-
(2005)
Am. J. Phys
, vol.73
, pp. 463
-
-
Meltzer, D.E.1
-
29
-
-
34250728427
-
-
We note that this objectlike reasoning may also apply when the sign is in the form of words, and e.g, interpreting the phrase step function as describing a stair-step-like object. See, for example, D. T. Brookes and E. Etkina, Do our words really matter? Case Studies From Quantum Mechanics, Proceedings of the 2005 Physics Education Research Conference AIP, Melville, NY, 2006
-
We note that this objectlike reasoning may also apply when the sign is in the form of words, and e.g., interpreting the phrase "step function" as describing a stair-step-like object. See, for example, D. T. Brookes and E. Etkina, Do our words really matter? Case Studies From Quantum Mechanics, Proceedings of the 2005 Physics Education Research Conference (AIP, Melville, NY, 2006).
-
-
-
-
30
-
-
0006766998
-
Naïve physics reasoning: A commitment to substance-based conceptions
-
M. Reiner, J. D. Slotta, M. T. H. Chi, and L. B. Resnick, Naïve physics reasoning: A commitment to substance-based conceptions, Cogn. Instruct. 18, 1 (2000).
-
(2000)
Cogn. Instruct
, vol.18
, pp. 1
-
-
Reiner, M.1
Slotta, J.D.2
Chi, M.T.H.3
Resnick, L.B.4
-
31
-
-
0028802442
-
Reasoning about the referent of a picture versus reasoning about the picture as the referent: An effect on visual realism
-
D. L. Schwartz, Reasoning about the referent of a picture versus reasoning about the picture as the referent: An effect on visual realism, Mem. Cognit. 23, 709 (1995).
-
(1995)
Mem. Cognit
, vol.23
, pp. 709
-
-
Schwartz, D.L.1
-
32
-
-
0034358353
-
What students' learning of representation tells us about constructivism
-
A. Elby, What students' learning of representation tells us about constructivism, J. Math. Behav. 19, 481 (2000).
-
(2000)
J. Math. Behav
, vol.19
, pp. 481
-
-
Elby, A.1
-
33
-
-
21144479999
-
Confusion by representation: On students' comprehension of the electric field concept
-
S. Tornkvist, K. A. Pettersson, and G. Transtromer, Confusion by representation: On students' comprehension of the electric field concept, Am. J. Phys. 61, 335 (1993).
-
(1993)
Am. J. Phys
, vol.61
, pp. 335
-
-
Tornkvist, S.1
Pettersson, K.A.2
Transtromer, G.3
-
34
-
-
0033461958
-
Student understanding of light as an electromagnetic wave: Relating the formalism to physical phenomena
-
B. S. Ambrose, P. R. L. Heron, S. Vokos, and L. C. McDermott, Student understanding of light as an electromagnetic wave: Relating the formalism to physical phenomena, Am. J. Phys. 67, 891 (1999).
-
(1999)
Am. J. Phys
, vol.67
, pp. 891
-
-
Ambrose, B.S.1
Heron, P.R.L.2
Vokos, S.3
McDermott, L.C.4
-
36
-
-
34250703040
-
-
Alternatively, one might hypothesize that this approach would lead to more confusion for students. The string and sound analogies may interfere in problematic ways, or there may be simply too much information for students to learn at once
-
Alternatively, one might hypothesize that this approach would lead to more confusion for students. The string and sound analogies may interfere in problematic ways, or there may be simply too much information for students to learn at once.
-
-
-
-
37
-
-
34250764102
-
-
N. Podolefsky and N. Finkelstein, http://per.colorado.edu/analogy/ index.htm
-
N. Podolefsky and N. Finkelstein, http://per.colorado.edu/analogy/ index.htm
-
-
-
-
38
-
-
34250723191
-
-
In common physics parlance, sign and representation share the same meaning. In other sciences, representation often refers to internal or mental representations, along the lines of mental models or schemata
-
In common physics parlance, sign and representation share the same meaning. In other sciences, representation often refers to internal or mental representations, along the lines of mental models or schemata.
-
-
-
-
39
-
-
0001957861
-
-
edited by R. J. Spiro, B. C. Bruce, and W. F. Brewer Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ
-
D. E. Rumelhart, in Theoretical Issues in Reading Comprehension, edited by R. J. Spiro, B. C. Bruce, and W. F. Brewer (Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ 1980), pp. 35-38.
-
(1980)
Theoretical Issues in Reading Comprehension
, pp. 35-38
-
-
Rumelhart, D.E.1
-
40
-
-
34250764631
-
-
This was the state of scientific knowledge prior to Rutherford
-
This was the state of scientific knowledge prior to Rutherford.
-
-
-
-
41
-
-
34248711923
-
Compression and representation
-
M. Turner, Compression and representation, Lang. Lit. 15, 17 (2006).
-
(2006)
Lang. Lit
, vol.15
, pp. 17
-
-
Turner, M.1
-
42
-
-
34250724754
-
-
In the language of blending theory, this is referred to as a single scope blend, where the organizing frame of one mental space (solar system) is applied to the elements of another atom, For more, see Ref. 12
-
In the language of blending theory, this is referred to as a "single scope" blend, where the organizing frame of one mental space (solar system) is applied to the elements of another (atom). For more, see Ref. 12.
-
-
-
-
43
-
-
0037631686
-
Six views of embodied cognition
-
One might ask why an external representation is contained within a mental space. We side with the view that artifacts of the environment, such as pictures on paper, are key components of cognition, and hence mental spaces. See
-
One might ask why an external representation is contained within a mental space. We side with the view that artifacts of the environment, such as pictures on paper, are key components of cognition, and hence mental spaces. See M. Wilson, Six views of embodied cognition, Psychon. Bull. Rev. 9, 625 (2002).
-
(2002)
Psychon. Bull. Rev
, vol.9
, pp. 625
-
-
Wilson, M.1
-
44
-
-
34250694012
-
-
Each of these sign-schema-referent triangles may be considered a blend, whereby the sign, referent, and schema are connected by vital relations. In the case of the solar system, a picture of the solar system (sign) may be connected to the real solar system (referent) via the vital relation representation. In the Rutherford atom blend, the size of both the solar system and atom referents are scaled to a human scale (the actual size of the sign) via the vital relation space. Role and value vital relations may be involved, whereby the role central object takes the values sun or nucleus, and the role outer object takes the values planet or electron.
-
Each of these sign-schema-referent triangles may be considered a blend, whereby the sign, referent, and schema are connected by vital relations. In the case of the solar system, a picture of the solar system (sign) may be connected to the real solar system (referent) via the vital relation representation. In the Rutherford atom blend, the size of both the solar system and atom referents are scaled to a human scale (the actual size of the sign) via the vital relation space. Role and value vital relations may be involved, whereby the role central object takes the values sun or nucleus, and the role outer object takes the values planet or electron.
-
-
-
-
45
-
-
34250703564
-
-
In this case, the historical progression happens to match a pedagogy that may be productive. We do not mean to suggest, however, that pedagogy should, in general, follow historical accounts of discovery
-
In this case, the historical progression happens to match a pedagogy that may be productive. We do not mean to suggest, however, that pedagogy should, in general, follow historical accounts of discovery.
-
-
-
-
46
-
-
33748290373
-
Using resource graphs to represent conceptual change
-
These changes to sign-schema-referent relations may be along the lines of changes to Wittmann's resource graphs. See
-
These changes to sign-schema-referent relations may be along the lines of changes to Wittmann's resource graphs. See M. C. Wittmann, Using resource graphs to represent conceptual change, Phys. Rev. ST Phys. Educ. Res. 2, 020105 (2006).
-
(2006)
Phys. Rev. ST Phys. Educ. Res
, vol.2
, pp. 020105
-
-
Wittmann, M.C.1
-
47
-
-
34250746265
-
-
It might be more accurate to say Planck's quantization of energy, which Bohr blended with Rutherford's model.
-
It might be more accurate to say Planck's quantization of energy, which Bohr blended with Rutherford's model.
-
-
-
-
48
-
-
34250743497
-
-
The delineation of abstract and concrete may depend on the level of expertise. To an expert physicist, an electron is a particular type of particle, and thus the electron is more concrete. To a student, to whom an electron may be an unfamiliar idea, particle may be more concrete in the sense of being connected to a real object, like a dust particle. In this sense, students' prior knowledge plays a role in our model to the extent that we can determine which ideas are already concrete for students, and which remain abstract. For a detailed analysis of levels of abstraction, see S. I. Hayakawa, Language in Thought and Action, 3rd ed. (Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, New York, 1972).
-
The delineation of abstract and concrete may depend on the level of expertise. To an expert physicist, an electron is a particular type of particle, and thus the electron is more concrete. To a student, to whom an electron may be an unfamiliar idea, "particle" may be more concrete in the sense of being connected to a real object, like a dust particle. In this sense, students' prior knowledge plays a role in our model to the extent that we can determine which ideas are already concrete for students, and which remain abstract. For a detailed analysis of levels of abstraction, see S. I. Hayakawa, Language in Thought and Action, 3rd ed. (Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, New York, 1972).
-
-
-
-
49
-
-
34250750629
-
-
Such a notion of abstraction being a series of blends is consistent with Lakoff and Nunez's notion of layering (Ref. 16). The level of perceived abstraction may depend on the student, in that as students become increasingly familiar with abstract sign-schema relations, these relations may become increasingly treated as concrete. In this case, nodes may become so tightly coupled for an expert that the sign-schema link is compressed and the nodes disappear. For instance, to the expert physicist, the notions of light and wave are not separate ideas - to this expert, light is a wave.
-
Such a notion of abstraction being a series of blends is consistent with Lakoff and Nunez's notion of layering (Ref. 16). The level of perceived abstraction may depend on the student, in that as students become increasingly familiar with abstract sign-schema relations, these relations may become increasingly treated as concrete. In this case, nodes may become so tightly coupled for an expert that the sign-schema link is compressed and the nodes disappear. For instance, to the expert physicist, the notions of "light" and "wave" are not separate ideas - to this expert, light is a wave.
-
-
-
-
50
-
-
34250714371
-
-
private communication
-
Saalih Allie (private communication).
-
-
-
Allie, S.1
|