메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 286, Issue 5445, 1999, Pages 1692-1695

Interacting minds - A biological basis

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords

BEHAVIOR; BRAIN FUNCTION; FRONTAL CORTEX; HUMAN; INTELLIGENCE; MENTAL DEVELOPMENT; MENTAL HEALTH; PRIORITY JOURNAL; REVIEW; SOCIAL INTERACTION;

EID: 0033607509     PISSN: 00368075     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1126/science.286.5445.1692     Document Type: Review
Times cited : (1383)

References (59)
  • 2
  • 5
    • 0039044485 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A. D. Milner, Ed. Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford
    • R. W. Byrne, in Comparative Neuropsychology, A. D. Milner, Ed. (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 1998), pp. 228-244.
    • (1998) Comparative Neuropsychology , pp. 228-244
    • Byrne, R.W.1
  • 14
    • 0001605571 scopus 로고
    • A. Whiten, Ed. Blackwell, Oxford
    • C. Butterworth, in Natural Theories of Mind, A. Whiten, Ed. (Blackwell, Oxford, 1991), pp. 223-232. Although early gaze following does not necessarily indicate mentalizing, it is highly plausible that shared attention is a critical stage in the development of mentalizing. S. Baron-Cohen, in Natural Theories of Mind, A. Whiten, Ed. (Blackwell, Oxford, 1991), pp. 233-251.
    • (1991) Natural Theories of Mind , pp. 223-232
    • Butterworth, C.1
  • 15
    • 0001452442 scopus 로고
    • A. Whiten, Ed. Blackwell, Oxford
    • C. Butterworth, in Natural Theories of Mind, A. Whiten, Ed. (Blackwell, Oxford, 1991), pp. 223-232. Although early gaze following does not necessarily indicate mentalizing, it is highly plausible that shared attention is a critical stage in the development of mentalizing. S. Baron-Cohen, in Natural Theories of Mind, A. Whiten, Ed. (Blackwell, Oxford, 1991), pp. 233-251.
    • (1991) Natural Theories of Mind , pp. 233-251
    • Baron-Cohen, S.1
  • 17
    • 0030068050 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • S. Baron-Cohen et al., Br. J. Psychiatr. 168, 158 (1996); M. Sigman, P. Mundy, J. Ungerer, T. Sherman, Child Psychol. Psychiatr. 27, 647 (1986).
    • (1996) Br. J. Psychiatr. , vol.168 , pp. 158
    • Baron-Cohen, S.1
  • 25
    • 0000447810 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • R. Corcoran, G. Mercer, C. D. Frith, Schizophr. Res. 17, 5 (1995); R. Langdon et al., Cogn. Neuropsychiatr. 2, 167 (1997).
    • (1997) Cogn. Neuropsychiatr. , vol.2 , pp. 167
    • Langdon, R.1
  • 26
    • 0003836110 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford
    • J. Russell, Ed., Autism as an Executive Disorder (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 1998); T. E. Goldberg and D. R. Weinberger, Schizophr. Bull. 14, 179 (1988).
    • (1998) Autism As An Executive Disorder
    • Russell, J.1
  • 27
    • 0024248861 scopus 로고
    • J. Russell, Ed., Autism as an Executive Disorder (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 1998); T. E. Goldberg and D. R. Weinberger, Schizophr. Bull. 14, 179 (1988).
    • (1988) Schizophr. Bull. , vol.14 , pp. 179
    • Goldberg, T.E.1    Weinberger, D.R.2
  • 28
    • 0026875041 scopus 로고
    • A. M. Leslie and L. Thaiss, Cognition 43, 225 (1992); T. Charman and S. Baron-Cohen, J. Child Psychol. Psychiatr. 33, 1105 (1992). These studies compare formally identical tasks where children with autism were better able to understand the implications of a false photograph, map or drawing, for the location of a displaced object than the implications of having a false belief (as in the Sally-Anne task).
    • (1992) Cognition , vol.43 , pp. 225
    • Leslie, A.M.1    Thaiss, L.2
  • 29
    • 0026779256 scopus 로고
    • A. M. Leslie and L. Thaiss, Cognition 43, 225 (1992); T. Charman and S. Baron-Cohen, J. Child Psychol. Psychiatr. 33, 1105 (1992). These studies compare formally identical tasks where children with autism were better able to understand the implications of a false photograph, map or drawing, for the location of a displaced object than the implications of having a false belief (as in the Sally-Anne task).
    • (1992) J. Child Psychol. Psychiatr. , vol.33 , pp. 1105
    • Charman, T.1    Baron-Cohen, S.2
  • 31
    • 0031760883 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • V. Stone, S. Baron-Cohen, and R. T. Knight [J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 640 (1998)] found that patients with dorsolateral prefrontal cortex lesions performed poorly on false belief tasks although this was attributed to memory problems. F. Happé, H. Brownell, and E. Winner [Cognition 70, 211 (1999)] found impairments on a range of mentalizing tasks in patients with right hemisphere lesions, but not with left hemisphere lesions.
    • (1998) J. Cogn. Neurosci. , vol.10 , pp. 640
    • Stone, V.1    Baron-Cohen, S.2    Knight, R.T.3
  • 32
    • 0033120269 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • V. Stone, S. Baron-Cohen, and R. T. Knight [J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 640 (1998)] found that patients with dorsolateral prefrontal cortex lesions performed poorly on false belief tasks although this was attributed to memory problems. F. Happé, H. Brownell, and E. Winner [Cognition 70, 211 (1999)] found impairments on a range of mentalizing tasks in patients with right hemisphere lesions, but not with left hemisphere lesions.
    • (1999) Cognition , vol.70 , pp. 211
    • Happé, F.1    Brownell, H.2    Winner, E.3
  • 33
    • 0032496356 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • E. A. Maguire et al., Science 280, 921 (1998).
    • (1998) Science , vol.280 , pp. 921
    • Maguire, E.A.1
  • 35
    • 0030825636 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • P. Rainville et al., Science 277, 968 (1997). Hypnosis was used to alter pain perception even though the intensity of stimulation remained constant.
    • (1997) Science , vol.277 , pp. 968
    • Rainville, P.1
  • 38
    • 0032076255 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • C. S. Carter et al., Science 280, 747 (1998). Demonstration that activity in anterior cingulate cortex is greater on trials in which competing responses are elicited. The authors conclude that activity in ACC represents the current state of the response system.
    • (1998) Science , vol.280 , pp. 747
    • Carter, C.S.1
  • 39
    • 33644699514 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • S.-J. Blakemore, D. Wolpert, C. D. Frith, Nature Neurasci. 1, 635 (1998). Identical tactile stimuli were applied to the palm of the hand, either by the volunteer herself or by the experimenter. Greater activity was elicited by externally generated stimuli, which were also rated as feeling more intense.
    • (1998) Nature Neurasci. , vol.1 , pp. 635
    • Blakemore, S.-J.1    Wolpert, D.2    Frith, C.D.3
  • 40
    • 0029038035 scopus 로고
    • V. Goel, J. Grafman, N. Sadato, M. Hallett, Neuroreport 6, 1741 (1995). Volunteers were asked to judge whether someone living in the 15th century (for example, Christopher Columbus) would have known the use of a series of objects. This mentalizing task was contrasted with memory retrieval and with simple inferencing. Mentalizing was associated with activity in medial prefrontal cortex, and left temporoparietal junction. In the study of Fletcher et al. [Cognition 57, 109 (1995)], volunteers were asked to explain the behavior of characters in short stories involving deception and pretence. In the control task volunteers read stories in which the mental states of the characters did not play a role. Mentalizing was associated with activity in medial prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex and the right temporoparietal junction.
    • (1995) Neuroreport , vol.6 , pp. 1741
    • Goel, V.1    Grafman, J.2    Sadato, N.3    Hallett, M.4
  • 41
    • 0029398979 scopus 로고
    • V. Goel, J. Grafman, N. Sadato, M. Hallett, Neuroreport 6, 1741 (1995). Volunteers were asked to judge whether someone living in the 15th century (for example, Christopher Columbus) would have known the use of a series of objects. This mentalizing task was contrasted with memory retrieval and with simple inferencing. Mentalizing was associated with activity in medial prefrontal cortex, and left temporoparietal junction. In the study of Fletcher et al. [Cognition 57, 109 (1995)], volunteers were asked to explain the behavior of characters in short stories involving deception and pretence. In the control task volunteers read stories in which the mental states of the characters did not play a role. Mentalizing was associated with activity in medial prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex and the right temporoparietal junction.
    • (1995) Cognition , vol.57 , pp. 109
    • Fletcher1
  • 43
    • 0017771466 scopus 로고
    • F. Jacob, Science 196, 1161 (1977).
    • (1977) Science , vol.196 , pp. 1161
    • Jacob, F.1
  • 47
    • 0032521104 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A. Puce et al., J. Neurosci. 18, 2188 (1998); J. Grèzes, N. Costes, J. Decety, J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 553 (1999).
    • (1998) J. Neurosci. , vol.18 , pp. 2188
    • Puce, A.1
  • 55
    • 0019450947 scopus 로고
    • J. K. Hietanen and D. I. Perrett, Exp. Brain Res. 93, 117 (1993); P. Müller-Preuss and D. Ploog, Brain Res. 215, 61 (1981).
    • (1981) Brain Res. , vol.215 , pp. 61
    • Müller-Preuss, P.1    Ploog, D.2
  • 56
    • 0025971275 scopus 로고
    • K. Shima et al., J. Neurophysiol. 65, 188 (1991); for a review see R. E. Passingham, The Frontal Lobes and Voluntary Action (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 1993), chap. 4.
    • (1991) J. Neurophysiol. , vol.65 , pp. 188
    • Shima, K.1
  • 57
    • 0003509280 scopus 로고
    • Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, chap. 4
    • K. Shima et al., J. Neurophysiol. 65, 188 (1991); for a review see R. E. Passingham, The Frontal Lobes and Voluntary Action (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 1993), chap. 4.
    • (1993) The Frontal Lobes and Voluntary Action
    • Passingham, R.E.1
  • 58
    • 0002651020 scopus 로고
    • D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, R. J. W. Mansfield, Eds. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA
    • Ventral visual pathways are for identifying stimuli (the "what" stream) and dorsal visual pathways for locating stimuli (the "where" stream) [L. G. Ungerleider and M. Mishkin, in Analysis of Visual Behavior, D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, R. J. W. Mansfield, Eds. (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1982), pp. 549-586]. The dorsal visual pathways are for the visual control of action (for example, reaching and grasping objects) rather than simply for the location of objects [A. D. Milner and M. Goodale, The Visual Brain in Action (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 1995)].
    • (1982) Analysis of Visual Behavior , pp. 549-586
    • Ungerleider, L.G.1    Mishkin, M.2
  • 59
    • 0003802772 scopus 로고
    • Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford
    • Ventral visual pathways are for identifying stimuli (the "what" stream) and dorsal visual pathways for locating stimuli (the "where" stream) [L. G. Ungerleider and M. Mishkin, in Analysis of Visual Behavior, D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, R. J. W. Mansfield, Eds. (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1982), pp. 549-586]. The dorsal visual pathways are for the visual control of action (for example, reaching and grasping objects) rather than simply for the location of objects [A. D. Milner and M. Goodale, The Visual Brain in Action (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 1995)].
    • (1995) The Visual Brain in Action
    • Milner, A.D.1    Goodale, M.2


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.