메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 102, Issue 1, 2011, Pages 1-18

From over-imitation to super-copying: Adults imitate causally irrelevant aspects of tool use with higher fidelity than young children

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords


EID: 79751514604     PISSN: 00071268     EISSN: 20448325     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1348/000712610X493115     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (208)

References (55)
  • 1
    • 0031155657 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Exploring the roles of peers and adult video models for infant imitation
    • Abravnel, E., & DeYoung, N. G. (1997). Exploring the roles of peers and adult video models for infant imitation. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 158, 133-150. doi:
    • (1997) Journal of Genetic Psychology , vol.158 , pp. 133-150
    • Abravnel, E.1    DeYoung, N.G.2
  • 3
    • 0033192189 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Developmental changes in imitation from television during infancy
    • Barr, R., & Hayne, H. (1999). Developmental changes in imitation from television during infancy. Child Development, 70, 1067-1081. doi:
    • (1999) Child Development , vol.70 , pp. 1067-1081
    • Barr, R.1    Hayne, H.2
  • 5
    • 34248647054 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Doing the right thing: Infants' selection of actions to imitate from observed event sequences
    • Brugger, A., Lariviere, L. A., Mumme, D. L., & Bushnell, E. W. (2007). Doing the right thing: Infants' selection of actions to imitate from observed event sequences. Child Development, 78, 806-824. doi:
    • (2007) Child Development , vol.78 , pp. 806-824
    • Brugger, A.1    Lariviere, L.A.2    Mumme, D.L.3    Bushnell, E.W.4
  • 6
    • 68949203701 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Behavior: Monkeys like mimics
    • Call, J., & Carpenter, M. (2009). Behavior: Monkeys like mimics. Science, 325, 824-825. doi:
    • (2009) Science , vol.325 , pp. 824-825
    • Call, J.1    Carpenter, M.2
  • 7
    • 84987485440 scopus 로고
    • Talk and silence sequences in informal conversations: III. Interspeaker influence
    • Cappella, J. N., & Planalp, S. (1981). Talk and silence sequences in informal conversations: III. Interspeaker influence. Human Communication Research, 7, 117-132. doi:
    • (1981) Human Communication Research , vol.7 , pp. 117-132
    • Cappella, J.N.1    Planalp, S.2
  • 8
    • 33845783395 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Imitation and the social mind: Autism and typical development
    • S. J. Rogers & J. H. G. Williams (Eds.), New York: Guilford Press.
    • Carpenter, M. (2006). Instrumental, social, and shared goals and intentions in imitation. In S. J. Rogers & J. H. G. Williams (Eds.), Imitation and the social mind: Autism and typical development (pp. 48-70). New York: Guilford Press.
    • (2006) Instrumental, social, and shared goals and intentions in imitation , pp. 48-70
    • Carpenter, M.1
  • 9
    • 0033238097 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction
    • Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893-910. doi:
    • (1999) Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , vol.76 , pp. 893-910
    • Chartrand, T.L.1    Bargh, J.A.2
  • 13
    • 0003700595 scopus 로고
    • British Picture Vocabulary Scale
    • Windsor: NFER-Nelson.
    • Dunn, L., Dunn, L., Whetton, C., & Pintilie, D. (1982). British Picture Vocabulary Scale. Windsor: NFER-Nelson.
    • (1982)
    • Dunn, L.1    Dunn, L.2    Whetton, C.3    Pintilie, D.4
  • 14
    • 47649114688 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Cultural transmission of tool-use in young children: A diffusion chain study
    • Flynn, E., & Whiten, A. (2008). Cultural transmission of tool-use in young children: A diffusion chain study. Social Development, 17, 699-718. doi:
    • (2008) Social Development , vol.17 , pp. 699-718
    • Flynn, E.1    Whiten, A.2
  • 15
    • 0037074952 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Rational imitation in preverbal infants
    • Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., & Király, I. (2002). Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415, 755.
    • (2002) Nature , vol.415 , pp. 755
    • Gergely, G.1    Bekkering, H.2    Király, I.3
  • 18
    • 0034311260 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Children's coding of human action: Cognitive factors influencing imitation in 3-year-olds
    • Gleissner, B., Meltzoff, A. N., & Bekkering, H. (2000). Children's coding of human action: Cognitive factors influencing imitation in 3-year-olds. Developmental Science, 3, 405-414. doi:
    • (2000) Developmental Science , vol.3 , pp. 405-414
    • Gleissner, B.1    Meltzoff, A.N.2    Bekkering, H.3
  • 19
    • 21144470724 scopus 로고
    • Peer imitation by toddlers in laboratory, home, and day-care contexts: Implications for social learning and memory
    • Hannah, E., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1993). Peer imitation by toddlers in laboratory, home, and day-care contexts: Implications for social learning and memory. Developmental Psychology, 29, 701-710. doi:
    • (1993) Developmental Psychology , vol.29 , pp. 701-710
    • Hannah, E.1    Meltzoff, A.N.2
  • 20
    • 0003886280 scopus 로고
    • Emotional contagion
    • Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    • Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional contagion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    • (1994)
    • Hatfield, E.1    Cacioppo, J.T.2    Rapson, R.L.3
  • 21
    • 0013179906 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Imitation from television by 24- and 30- month-olds
    • Hayne, H., Herbert, J., & Simcock, G. (2003). Imitation from television by 24- and 30- month-olds. Developmental Science, 6, 254-261. doi:
    • (2003) Developmental Science , vol.6 , pp. 254-261
    • Hayne, H.1    Herbert, J.2    Simcock, G.3
  • 22
    • 23944443282 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens)
    • Horner, V., & Whiten, A. (2005). Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8, 164-181. doi:
    • (2005) Animal Cognition , vol.8 , pp. 164-181
    • Horner, V.1    Whiten, A.2
  • 24
    • 0141525325 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Do humans ape? Or do apes human? Imitation and intention in humans (Homo sapiens) and other animals
    • Horowitz, A. C. (2003). Do humans ape? Or do apes human? Imitation and intention in humans (Homo sapiens) and other animals. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117, 325-336. doi:
    • (2003) Journal of Comparative Psychology , vol.117 , pp. 325-336
    • Horowitz, A.C.1
  • 25
    • 33748690330 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Adults don't always know best: Preschoolers use past reliability over age when learning
    • Jaswal, V. K., & Neely, L. A. (2006). Adults don't always know best: Preschoolers use past reliability over age when learning. Psychological Science, 17, 757-758. doi:
    • (2006) Psychological Science , vol.17 , pp. 757-758
    • Jaswal, V.K.1    Neely, L.A.2
  • 26
    • 7444269929 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Trust in testimony: Children's use of true and false statements
    • Koenig, M. A., Clément, F., & Harris, P. L. (2004). Trust in testimony: Children's use of true and false statements. Psychological Science, 15, 694-698. doi:
    • (2004) Psychological Science , vol.15 , pp. 694-698
    • Koenig, M.A.1    Clément, F.2    Harris, P.L.3
  • 27
    • 28144453335 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers
    • Koenig, M. A., & Harris, P. L. (2005). Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child Development, 76, 1261-1277. doi:
    • (2005) Child Development , vol.76 , pp. 1261-1277
    • Koenig, M.A.1    Harris, P.L.2
  • 30
    • 77956521894 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Cultural transmission of irrelevant tool actions in diffusion chains of 3- and 5-year-old children
    • (in press).
    • McGuigan, N., & Graham, M. (in press). Cultural transmission of irrelevant tool actions in diffusion chains of 3- and 5-year-old children. European Journal of Developmental Psychology. doi:
    • European Journal of Developmental Psychology
    • McGuigan, N.1    Graham, M.2
  • 31
    • 70349464689 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Emulation and over-emulation in the social learning of causally opaque versus causally transparent tool use by 23- and 30-month-old children
    • McGuigan, N., & Whiten, A. (2009). Emulation and over-emulation in the social learning of causally opaque versus causally transparent tool use by 23- and 30-month-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 367-381. doi:
    • (2009) Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , vol.104 , pp. 367-381
    • McGuigan, N.1    Whiten, A.2
  • 32
    • 70349467183 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Imitation of causally necessary versus unnecessary tool use by 3- and 5-year-old children
    • McGuigan, N., Whiten, A., Flynn, E. F., & Horner, V. (2007). Imitation of causally necessary versus unnecessary tool use by 3- and 5-year-old children. Cognitive Development, 22, 356-364. doi:
    • (2007) Cognitive Development , vol.22 , pp. 356-364
    • McGuigan, N.1    Whiten, A.2    Flynn, E.F.3    Horner, V.4
  • 33
    • 58149323600 scopus 로고
    • Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children
    • Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 838-850. doi:
    • (1995) Developmental Psychology , vol.31 , pp. 838-850
    • Meltzoff, A.N.1
  • 34
    • 33746816136 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Copying actions and copying outcomes: Social learning through the second year
    • Nielsen, M. (2006). Copying actions and copying outcomes: Social learning through the second year. Developmental Psychology, 42, 555-565. doi:
    • (2006) Developmental Psychology , vol.42 , pp. 555-565
    • Nielsen, M.1
  • 35
    • 42049117543 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The social motivation for social learning
    • Nielsen, M. (2008). The social motivation for social learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 33. doi:
    • (2008) Behavioral and Brain Sciences , vol.31 , pp. 33
    • Nielsen, M.1
  • 37
    • 49549105968 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The effect of social engagement on 24-month-olds' imitation from live and televised models
    • Nielsen, M., Simcock, G., & Jenkins, L. (2008). The effect of social engagement on 24-month-olds' imitation from live and televised models. Developmental Science, 11, 722-731. doi:
    • (2008) Developmental Science , vol.11 , pp. 722-731
    • Nielsen, M.1    Simcock, G.2    Jenkins, L.3
  • 39
    • 0034242533 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Mood contagion: The automatic transfer of mood between persons
    • Neumann, R., & Strack, F. (2000). Mood contagion: The automatic transfer of mood between persons. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 211-223. doi:
    • (2000) Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , vol.79 , pp. 211-223
    • Neumann, R.1    Strack, F.2
  • 40
    • 0039235499 scopus 로고
    • Effects of the model's age, perceived similarity, and familiarity in children's donating
    • Owens, C. R., & Ascione, F. R. (1991). Effects of the model's age, perceived similarity, and familiarity in children's donating. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 152, 341-357.
    • (1991) Journal of Genetic Psychology , vol.152 , pp. 341-357
    • Owens, C.R.1    Ascione, F.R.2
  • 41
    • 0004099266 scopus 로고
    • The language and thought of the child
    • London: Kegan Paul.
    • Piaget, J. (1962). The language and thought of the child. London: Kegan Paul.
    • (1962)
    • Piaget, J.1
  • 42
    • 11244327940 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution
    • Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
    • Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
    • (2005)
    • Richerson, P.J.1    Boyd, R.2
  • 43
    • 0034394429 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Infant imitation of peer and adult models: Evidence for a peer model advantage
    • Ryalls, B. O., Gul, R. E., & Ryalls, K. R. (2000). Infant imitation of peer and adult models: Evidence for a peer model advantage. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 46, 188-202.
    • (2000) Merrill-Palmer Quarterly , vol.46 , pp. 188-202
    • Ryalls, B.O.1    Gul, R.E.2    Ryalls, K.R.3
  • 48
    • 61349171203 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Who knows best? Preschoolers sometimes prefer child informants over adult informants
    • VanderBorght, M., & Jaswal, V. K. (2009). Who knows best? Preschoolers sometimes prefer child informants over adult informants. Infant and Child Development, 18, 61-71. doi:
    • (2009) Infant and Child Development , vol.18 , pp. 61-71
    • VanderBorght, M.1    Jaswal, V.K.2
  • 49
    • 0003605655 scopus 로고
    • Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes
    • Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    • (1978)
    • Vygotsky, L.S.1
  • 51
    • 33750303070 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Imitation of hierarchical structure in actions by young children
    • Whiten, A., Flynn, F., Brown, K., & Lee, T. (2006). Imitation of hierarchical structure in actions by young children. Developmental Science, 9, 574-582. doi:
    • (2006) Developmental Science , vol.9 , pp. 574-582
    • Whiten, A.1    Flynn, F.2    Brown, K.3    Lee, T.4
  • 54
    • 38549091260 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Prior experiences and perceived efficacy influence 3-year-olds' imitation
    • Williamson, R. A., Meltzoff, A. N., & Markman, E. M. (2008). Prior experiences and perceived efficacy influence 3-year-olds' imitation. Developmental Psychology, 44, 275-285. doi:
    • (2008) Developmental Psychology , vol.44 , pp. 275-285
    • Williamson, R.A.1    Meltzoff, A.N.2    Markman, E.M.3


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.