-
1
-
-
77953218579
-
Etymological and grammatical notes
-
Allen, F. D. (1880). Etymological and grammatical notes. The American Journal of Philology, 1(2), 127-145.
-
(1880)
The American Journal of Philology
, vol.1
, Issue.2
, pp. 127-145
-
-
Allen, F.D.1
-
2
-
-
0029825774
-
Time perception in a neglected space
-
Basso, G., Nicheli P., Frasinetti F., & di Pellegrino G. (1996). Time perception in a neglected space. Neuroreport, 7, 2111-2114.
-
(1996)
Neuroreport
, vol.7
, pp. 2111-2114
-
-
Basso, G.1
Nicheli, P.2
Frasinetti, F.3
di Pellegrino, G.4
-
3
-
-
0034646590
-
Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors
-
Boroditsky, L. (2000). Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 75(1), 1-28.
-
(2000)
Cognition
, vol.75
, Issue.1
, pp. 1-28
-
-
Boroditsky, L.1
-
4
-
-
0035430237
-
Does language shape thought? Mandarin and English speakers' conceptions of time
-
Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought? Mandarin and English speakers' conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43(1), 1-22.
-
(2001)
Cognitive Psychology
, vol.43
, Issue.1
, pp. 1-22
-
-
Boroditsky, L.1
-
5
-
-
54949132881
-
Similarity and proximity: When does close in space mean close in mind? Memory &
-
Casasanto, D. (2008a). Similarity and proximity: When does close in space mean close in mind? Memory & Cognition, 36(6), 1047-1056.
-
(2008)
Cognition
, vol.36
, Issue.6
, pp. 1047-1056
-
-
Casasanto, D.1
-
6
-
-
56749132333
-
Who's afraid of the big bad Whorf: Crosslingusitic differences in temporal language and thought
-
Casasanto, D. (2008b). Who's afraid of the big bad Whorf: Crosslingusitic differences in temporal language and thought. Language Learning, 58, 63-79.
-
(2008)
Language Learning
, vol.58
, pp. 63-79
-
-
Casasanto, D.1
-
7
-
-
68949213074
-
Embodiment of abstract concepts: Good and bad in right-and left-handers
-
Casasanto, D. (2009a). Embodiment of abstract concepts: Good and bad in right-and left-handers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(3), 351-367.
-
(2009)
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
, vol.138
, Issue.3
, pp. 351-367
-
-
Casasanto, D.1
-
8
-
-
85190155838
-
When is a linguistic metaphor a conceptual metaphor?
-
V. Evans & S. Pourcel (Eds.), Amsterdam: John Benjamins
-
Casasanto, D. (2009b). When is a linguistic metaphor a conceptual metaphor? In V. Evans & S. Pourcel (Eds.), New directions in cognitive linguistics (pp. 127-145). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
-
(2009)
New directions in cognitive linguistics
, pp. 127-145
-
-
Casasanto, D.1
-
9
-
-
37249071799
-
Time in the mind: Using space to think about time
-
Casasanto, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2008). Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106, 579-593.
-
(2008)
Cognition
, vol.106
, pp. 579-593
-
-
Casasanto, D.1
Boroditsky, L.2
-
10
-
-
33748870092
-
How deep are effects of language on thought? Time estimation in speakers of English, Indonesian, Greek, and Spanish
-
Chicago, IL
-
Casasanto, D., Boroditsky, L., Phillips, W., Greene, J., Goswami, S., Bocanegra-Thiel, S., Santiago-Diaz, I., Fotakopoulu, O., Pita, R., & Gil, D. (2004). How deep are effects of language on thought? Time estimation in speakers of English, Indonesian, Greek, and Spanish. Proceedings of the 26th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Chicago, IL.
-
(2004)
Proceedings of the 26th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society
-
-
Casasanto, D.1
Boroditsky, L.2
Phillips, W.3
Greene, J.4
Goswami, S.5
Bocanegra-Thiel, S.6
Santiago-Diaz, I.7
Fotakopoulu, O.8
Pita, R.9
Gil, D.10
-
11
-
-
0003303784
-
The numerical attribute of stimuli
-
H. L. Roitblat, T. G. Beaver, & H. S. Terrace (Eds.), Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
-
Church, R. M., & Meck, W. H. (1984). The numerical attribute of stimuli. In H. L. Roitblat, T. G. Beaver, & H. S. Terrace (Eds.), Animal cognition (pp. 445-464). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
-
(1984)
Animal cognition
, pp. 445-464
-
-
Church, R.M.1
Meck, W.H.2
-
12
-
-
0002284231
-
Space, time, semantics and the child
-
T. E. Moore (Ed.), New York: Academic Press
-
Clark, H. H. (1973). Space, time, semantics and the child. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (pp. 27-63). New York: Academic Press.
-
(1973)
Cognitive development and the acquisition of language
, pp. 27-63
-
-
Clark, H.H.1
-
13
-
-
74349096077
-
Languages and representations
-
D. Gentner & S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
-
Clark, E. (2003). Languages and representations. In D. Gentner & S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), Language in mind (pp. 17-23). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
-
(2003)
Language in mind
, pp. 17-23
-
-
Clark, E.1
-
15
-
-
21144480604
-
The mental representation of pairity and number magnitude
-
Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of pairity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(3), 371-396.
-
(1993)
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
, vol.122
, Issue.3
, pp. 371-396
-
-
Dehaene, S.1
Bossini, S.2
Giraux, P.3
-
16
-
-
27844563651
-
The body as a cognitive artifact in kinship representations
-
Enfield, N. J. (2005). The body as a cognitive artifact in kinship representations. Current Anthropology, 46(1), 51-81.
-
(2005)
Current Anthropology
, vol.46
, Issue.1
, pp. 51-81
-
-
Enfield, N.J.1
-
18
-
-
0033968177
-
Non-verbal numerical cognition: From reals to integers
-
Gallistell, R. C., & Gellman, R. (2000). Non-verbal numerical cognition: From reals to integers. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 59-65.
-
(2000)
Trends in Cognitive Science
, vol.4
, pp. 59-65
-
-
Gallistell, R.C.1
Gellman, R.2
-
19
-
-
38349146580
-
Interaction of stimulus dimensions in concept and choice processes
-
Garner, W. R. (1976). Interaction of stimulus dimensions in concept and choice processes. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 98-123.
-
(1976)
Cognitive Psychology
, vol.8
, pp. 98-123
-
-
Garner, W.R.1
-
20
-
-
0005096385
-
Spatial metaphors in temporal reasoning
-
M. Gattis (Ed.), Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
-
Gentner, D. (2001). Spatial metaphors in temporal reasoning. In M. Gattis (Ed.), Spacial Schemas in abstract thought (pp. 203-222). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
-
(2001)
Spacial Schemas in abstract thought
, pp. 203-222
-
-
Gentner, D.1
-
21
-
-
0346769835
-
The tau effect-an example of psychological relativity
-
Helson, H. (1930). The tau effect-an example of psychological relativity. Science, 71(1847), 536-537.
-
(1930)
Science
, vol.71
, Issue.1847
, pp. 536-537
-
-
Helson, H.1
-
25
-
-
77953211463
-
-
Transl. C. M. Williams Chicago: Open Court Publ. Co., 1897 (1st German ed., 1886)
-
Mach, E. (1886/1897). (Transl. C. M. Williams), Contributions to the analysis of sensations. Chicago: Open Court Publ. Co., 1897 (1st German ed., 1886).
-
(1886)
Contributions to the analysis of sensations
-
-
Mach, E.1
-
26
-
-
2042506346
-
Why the sunny side is up. Associations between affect and vertical position
-
Meier, B. P., & Robinson, M. D. (2004). Why the sunny side is up. Associations between affect and vertical position. Psychological Science, 15, 243-247.
-
(2004)
Psychological Science
, vol.15
, pp. 243-247
-
-
Meier, B.P.1
Robinson, M.D.2
-
27
-
-
77953220535
-
Do monkeys use space to think about time?
-
Denver, CO
-
Merritt, D. J., Casasanto, D., & Brannon, E. M. (2009). Do monkeys use space to think about time? Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development. Denver, CO.
-
(2009)
Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development
-
-
Merritt, D.J.1
Casasanto, D.2
Brannon, E.M.3
-
28
-
-
33745927428
-
Looking ahead to the past: Convergent evidence from Aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time
-
Núñez, R., & Sweetser, E. (2006). Looking ahead to the past: Convergent evidence from Aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time. Cognitive Science, 30(3), 401-450.
-
(2006)
Cognitive Science
, vol.30
, Issue.3
, pp. 401-450
-
-
Núñez, R.1
Sweetser, E.2
-
33
-
-
2642535380
-
The kappa effect
-
Price-Williams, D. R. (1954). The kappa effect. Nature, 173(4399), 363-364.
-
(1954)
Nature
, vol.173
, Issue.4399
, pp. 363-364
-
-
Price-Williams, D.R.1
-
34
-
-
33644908767
-
Spatial representation of pitch height: The SMARC effect
-
Rusconi, E., Kwan, B., Giordano, B., Umilta, C., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Spatial representation of pitch height: The SMARC effect. Cognition, 99, 113-129.
-
(2006)
Cognition
, vol.99
, pp. 113-129
-
-
Rusconi, E.1
Kwan, B.2
Giordano, B.3
Umilta, C.4
Butterworth, B.5
-
35
-
-
23844475419
-
Your highness: Vertical positions as perceptual symbols of power
-
Schubert, T. (2005). Your highness: Vertical positions as perceptual symbols of power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(1), 1-21.
-
(2005)
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
, vol.89
, Issue.1
, pp. 1-21
-
-
Schubert, T.1
-
38
-
-
34248875622
-
Force dynamics in language and cognition
-
Talmy, L. (1988). Force dynamics in language and cognition. Cognitive Science, 12, 49-100.
-
(1988)
Cognitive Science
, vol.12
, pp. 49-100
-
-
Talmy, L.1
-
39
-
-
0001574910
-
Cross-cultural and developmental trends in graphic productions
-
Tversky, B., Kugelmass, S., & Winter, A. (1991). Cross-cultural and developmental trends in graphic productions. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 515-557.
-
(1991)
Cognitive Psychology
, vol.23
, pp. 515-557
-
-
Tversky, B.1
Kugelmass, S.2
Winter, A.3
-
40
-
-
0142216244
-
A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity
-
Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7(11), 483-488.
-
(2003)
Trends in Cognitive Science
, vol.7
, Issue.11
, pp. 483-488
-
-
Walsh, V.1
-
41
-
-
40249101788
-
Keeping one's distance: The influence of spatial distance cues on affect and evaluation
-
Williams, L. E., & Bargh, J. A. (2008). Keeping one's distance: The influence of spatial distance cues on affect and evaluation. Psychological Science, 19, 302-308.
-
(2008)
Psychological Science
, vol.19
, pp. 302-308
-
-
Williams, L.E.1
Bargh, J.A.2
|