메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 19, Issue 1, 2009, Pages 12-19

Spatial representation across species: geometry, language, and maps

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords

BRAIN DAMAGE; BRAIN DISEASE; BRAIN NERVE CELL; BRAIN REGION; COGNITION; GEOMETRY; HIPPOCAMPUS; HUMAN; LANGUAGE; LOCOMOTION; NERVOUS SYSTEM DEVELOPMENT; NONHUMAN; PARIETAL LOBE; PRIORITY JOURNAL; REVIEW; SPATIAL ORIENTATION; WILLIAMS BEUREN SYNDROME; WORKING MEMORY;

EID: 67649382967     PISSN: 09594388     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.02.001     Document Type: Review
Times cited : (50)

References (69)
  • 2
    • 0022746162 scopus 로고
    • A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation
    • Cheng K. A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation. Cognition 23 (1986) 149-178
    • (1986) Cognition , vol.23 , pp. 149-178
    • Cheng, K.1
  • 4
    • 0030337572 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Modularity and development: the case of spatial reorientation
    • Hermer L., and Spelke E. Modularity and development: the case of spatial reorientation. Cognition 61 (1996) 195-232
    • (1996) Cognition , vol.61 , pp. 195-232
    • Hermer, L.1    Spelke, E.2
  • 5
    • 0025486511 scopus 로고
    • Geometric modules in animals' spatial representations: a test with chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus)
    • Vallortigara G., Zanforlin M., and Pasti G. Geometric modules in animals' spatial representations: a test with chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). J Comp Psychol 104 (1990) 248-254
    • (1990) J Comp Psychol , vol.104 , pp. 248-254
    • Vallortigara, G.1    Zanforlin, M.2    Pasti, G.3
  • 6
    • 33845327149 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • How fish do geometry in large and in small spaces
    • Sovrano V.A., Bisazza A., and Vallortigara G. How fish do geometry in large and in small spaces. Anim Cogn 10 (2007) 47-54
    • (2007) Anim Cogn , vol.10 , pp. 47-54
    • Sovrano, V.A.1    Bisazza, A.2    Vallortigara, G.3
  • 7
    • 85047683197 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Rhesus monkeys use geometric and nongeometric information during a reorientation task
    • Gouteux S., Thinus-Blanc C., and Vauclair J. Rhesus monkeys use geometric and nongeometric information during a reorientation task. J Exp Psychol: Gen 130 (2001) 505-519
    • (2001) J Exp Psychol: Gen , vol.130 , pp. 505-519
    • Gouteux, S.1    Thinus-Blanc, C.2    Vauclair, J.3
  • 8
    • 0001103328 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Pigeons' encoding of geometric and featural properties of a spatial environment
    • Kelly D.M., Spetch M.L., and Heth C.D. Pigeons' encoding of geometric and featural properties of a spatial environment. J Comp Psychol 112 (1998) 259-269
    • (1998) J Comp Psychol , vol.112 , pp. 259-269
    • Kelly, D.M.1    Spetch, M.L.2    Heth, C.D.3
  • 9
    • 0028339910 scopus 로고
    • A geometric process for spatial representation in young children
    • Hermer L., and Spelke E. A geometric process for spatial representation in young children. Nature 370 (1994) 57-59
    • (1994) Nature , vol.370 , pp. 57-59
    • Hermer, L.1    Spelke, E.2
  • 10
  • 11
    • 36348988257 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Explaining the development of spatial reorientation: modularity-plus-language versus the emergence of adaptive combination
    • Plumert J., and Spencer J. (Eds), Oxford University Press. A review of evidence from humans and other species on reorientation, and presentation of the adaptive combination hypothesis, an alternative to modularity.
    • Newcombe N., and Ratliff K. Explaining the development of spatial reorientation: modularity-plus-language versus the emergence of adaptive combination. In: Plumert J., and Spencer J. (Eds). The Emerging Spatial Mind (2007), Oxford University Press 53-76. A review of evidence from humans and other species on reorientation, and presentation of the adaptive combination hypothesis, an alternative to modularity.
    • (2007) The Emerging Spatial Mind , pp. 53-76
    • Newcombe, N.1    Ratliff, K.2
  • 12
    • 36749088022 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Is there an innate geometric module? Effects of experience with angular geometric cues on spatial re-orientation based on the shape of the environment
    • This study provides compelling evidence for an innate geometric system. Chicks were reared in a rectangular or circular-shaped environment and then placed in a rectangular shaped environment, with salient featural cues, where they underwent a reorientation task. Both groups of chicks showed that they spontaneously encoded the geometry, suggesting that the experience with particular geometric properties of layouts is not necessary for later geometric encoding.
    • Chianetti C., and Vallortigara G. Is there an innate geometric module? Effects of experience with angular geometric cues on spatial re-orientation based on the shape of the environment. Anim Cognition 11 (2008) 139-146. This study provides compelling evidence for an innate geometric system. Chicks were reared in a rectangular or circular-shaped environment and then placed in a rectangular shaped environment, with salient featural cues, where they underwent a reorientation task. Both groups of chicks showed that they spontaneously encoded the geometry, suggesting that the experience with particular geometric properties of layouts is not necessary for later geometric encoding.
    • (2008) Anim Cognition , vol.11 , pp. 139-146
    • Chianetti, C.1    Vallortigara, G.2
  • 13
    • 0041905324 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Reorientation in a small-scale environment by 3-, 4-, and 5-year-old children
    • Gouteux S., Vauclair J., and Thinus-Blanc C. Reorientation in a small-scale environment by 3-, 4-, and 5-year-old children. Cogn Dev 16 (2001) 853-869
    • (2001) Cogn Dev , vol.16 , pp. 853-869
    • Gouteux, S.1    Vauclair, J.2    Thinus-Blanc, C.3
  • 14
    • 21744449660 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Reorientation in a rhombic environment: no evidence for an encapsulated geometric module
    • Hupbach A., and Nadel L. Reorientation in a rhombic environment: no evidence for an encapsulated geometric module. Cogn Dev 20 (2005) 279-302
    • (2005) Cogn Dev , vol.20 , pp. 279-302
    • Hupbach, A.1    Nadel, L.2
  • 15
    • 0141867631 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • How toddlers represent enclosed spaces
    • Huttenlocher J., and Vasilyeva M. How toddlers represent enclosed spaces. Cogn Sci 27 (2003) 749-766
    • (2003) Cogn Sci , vol.27 , pp. 749-766
    • Huttenlocher, J.1    Vasilyeva, M.2
  • 16
    • 33745153737 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • How do young children determine location? Evidence from disorientation tasks
    • Lourenco S.F., and Huttenlocher J. How do young children determine location? Evidence from disorientation tasks. Cognition 100 (2006) 511-529
    • (2006) Cognition , vol.100 , pp. 511-529
    • Lourenco, S.F.1    Huttenlocher, J.2
  • 17
    • 49549122175 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Children's use of geometry for reorientation
    • This study explores the conditions under which humans use geometry to reorient. Four-year-old children used the geometry of the environment to reorient when extended, three-dimensional, 30 cm high surfaces defined the shape of the room. Children did not reorient using geometry when the shape of the room was defined by two-dimensional lines on the floor or by several large, stable columns.
    • Lee S.A., and Spelke E.S. Children's use of geometry for reorientation. Dev Sci 11 (2008) 743-749. This study explores the conditions under which humans use geometry to reorient. Four-year-old children used the geometry of the environment to reorient when extended, three-dimensional, 30 cm high surfaces defined the shape of the room. Children did not reorient using geometry when the shape of the room was defined by two-dimensional lines on the floor or by several large, stable columns.
    • (2008) Dev Sci , vol.11 , pp. 743-749
    • Lee, S.A.1    Spelke, E.S.2
  • 18
    • 35549000574 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Coding location in enclosed spaces: is geometry the principle?
    • Eighteen-month to 24-month-old-children performed a reorientation task in a square room. When the adjoining walls had circles of different sizes, children searched the correct corner where the toy was hidden (34.4%) and the corner diagonally opposite the correct corner (36.5%). However, this pattern was not found when the adjoining walls had different colors (red and blue), or when two of the walls were plain and two were patterned. The authors conclude that relative features on the same scale, rather than geometry per se, may guide reorientation.
    • Huttenlocher J., and Lourenco S.F. Coding location in enclosed spaces: is geometry the principle?. Dev Sci 10 (2007) 741-746. Eighteen-month to 24-month-old-children performed a reorientation task in a square room. When the adjoining walls had circles of different sizes, children searched the correct corner where the toy was hidden (34.4%) and the corner diagonally opposite the correct corner (36.5%). However, this pattern was not found when the adjoining walls had different colors (red and blue), or when two of the walls were plain and two were patterned. The authors conclude that relative features on the same scale, rather than geometry per se, may guide reorientation.
    • (2007) Dev Sci , vol.10 , pp. 741-746
    • Huttenlocher, J.1    Lourenco, S.F.2
  • 19
    • 5144230156 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Hippocampal lesions disrupt navigation based on the shape of the environment
    • McGregor A., Hayward A.J., Pearce J.M., and Good M.A. Hippocampal lesions disrupt navigation based on the shape of the environment. Behav Neurosci 118 (2004) 1011-1021
    • (2004) Behav Neurosci , vol.118 , pp. 1011-1021
    • McGregor, A.1    Hayward, A.J.2    Pearce, J.M.3    Good, M.A.4
  • 20
    • 8144227587 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Hippocampal formation is required for geometric navigation in pigeons
    • Vargas J.P., Petruso E.J., and Bingman V.P. Hippocampal formation is required for geometric navigation in pigeons. Eur J Neurosci 20 (2004) 1937-1944
    • (2004) Eur J Neurosci , vol.20 , pp. 1937-1944
    • Vargas, J.P.1    Petruso, E.J.2    Bingman, V.P.3
  • 21
    • 38149134505 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Impaired processing of local geometric features during navigation in a water maze following hippocampal lesions in rats
    • When performing a navigation task to a hidden goal, rats with hippocampal lesions were impaired at discriminating a right-angle corner from its mirror reversal corner and were impaired at discriminating a short versus long wall. However, they were able to use salient features (an acute angle; a colored wall) that highlighted the direction of search.
    • Jones P.M., Pearce J.M., Davies V.J., Good M.A., and McGregor A. Impaired processing of local geometric features during navigation in a water maze following hippocampal lesions in rats. Behav Neurosci 121 (2007) 1258-1271. When performing a navigation task to a hidden goal, rats with hippocampal lesions were impaired at discriminating a right-angle corner from its mirror reversal corner and were impaired at discriminating a short versus long wall. However, they were able to use salient features (an acute angle; a colored wall) that highlighted the direction of search.
    • (2007) Behav Neurosci , vol.121 , pp. 1258-1271
    • Jones, P.M.1    Pearce, J.M.2    Davies, V.J.3    Good, M.A.4    McGregor, A.5
  • 22
    • 33846922306 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Asymmetrical participation of the left and right hippocampus for representing environmental geometry in homing pigeons
    • Nardi D., and Bingman V.P. Asymmetrical participation of the left and right hippocampus for representing environmental geometry in homing pigeons. Behav Brain Res 178 (2007) 160-171
    • (2007) Behav Brain Res , vol.178 , pp. 160-171
    • Nardi, D.1    Bingman, V.P.2
  • 23
    • 1642353031 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Separate geometric and non-geometric modules for spatial reorientation: evidence from a lopsided animal brain
    • Vallortigara G., Pagni P., and Sovrano V.A. Separate geometric and non-geometric modules for spatial reorientation: evidence from a lopsided animal brain. J Cogn Neurosci 16 (2004) 390-400
    • (2004) J Cogn Neurosci , vol.16 , pp. 390-400
    • Vallortigara, G.1    Pagni, P.2    Sovrano, V.A.3
  • 24
    • 0038526279 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Separate processing mechanisms for encoding geometric and landmark information in the avian hippocampus
    • Tommasi L., Gagliardo A., Andrew R.J., and Vallortigara G. Separate processing mechanisms for encoding geometric and landmark information in the avian hippocampus. Eur J Neurosci 17 (2003) 1695-1702
    • (2003) Eur J Neurosci , vol.17 , pp. 1695-1702
    • Tommasi, L.1    Gagliardo, A.2    Andrew, R.J.3    Vallortigara, G.4
  • 25
    • 0032499196 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A cortical representation of the local visual environment
    • Epstein R., and Kanwisher N. A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature 392 (1998) 598-601
    • (1998) Nature , vol.392 , pp. 598-601
    • Epstein, R.1    Kanwisher, N.2
  • 27
    • 0037034801 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Long-term plasticity in hippocampal place-cell representation of environmental geometry
    • Lever C., Wills T., Cacucci F., Burgess N., and O'Keefe J. Long-term plasticity in hippocampal place-cell representation of environmental geometry. Nature 416 (2002) 90-94
    • (2002) Nature , vol.416 , pp. 90-94
    • Lever, C.1    Wills, T.2    Cacucci, F.3    Burgess, N.4    O'Keefe, J.5
  • 28
    • 23844454091 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex
    • Hafting T., Fyhn M., Molden S., Moser M.B., and Moser E.I. Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature 436 (2005) 801-806
    • (2005) Nature , vol.436 , pp. 801-806
    • Hafting, T.1    Fyhn, M.2    Molden, S.3    Moser, M.B.4    Moser, E.I.5
  • 29
    • 30044451433 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A metric for the cognitive map: found at last?
    • Jeffery K.J., and Burgess N. A metric for the cognitive map: found at last?. Trends Cogn Sci 10 (2006) 1-3
    • (2006) Trends Cogn Sci , vol.10 , pp. 1-3
    • Jeffery, K.J.1    Burgess, N.2
  • 30
    • 57149088654 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • How heterogeneous place cell responding arises from homogenous grids - a contextual gating hypothesis
    • Hayman R.M., and Jeffery K.J. How heterogeneous place cell responding arises from homogenous grids - a contextual gating hypothesis. Hippocampus 18 (2008) 1301-1313
    • (2008) Hippocampus , vol.18 , pp. 1301-1313
    • Hayman, R.M.1    Jeffery, K.J.2
  • 31
    • 0032836794 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Topographical disorientation: a synthesis and taxonomy
    • Aguirre G.K., and D'Esposito M. Topographical disorientation: a synthesis and taxonomy. Brain 122 (1999) 1613-1628
    • (1999) Brain , vol.122 , pp. 1613-1628
    • Aguirre, G.K.1    D'Esposito, M.2
  • 32
    • 0033930555 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Memory deficits characterized by patterns of lesions to the hippocampus and parahippocampal cortex
    • Bohbot V.D., Allen J.J.B., and Nadel L. Memory deficits characterized by patterns of lesions to the hippocampus and parahippocampal cortex. Ann N Y Acad Sci 911 (2000) 355-368
    • (2000) Ann N Y Acad Sci , vol.911 , pp. 355-368
    • Bohbot, V.D.1    Allen, J.J.B.2    Nadel, L.3
  • 33
    • 42149156156 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Navigational skills correlate with hippocampal fractional anisotropy in humans
    • Iaria G., Lanyon L.J., Fox C.J., Giaschi D., and Barton J.J.S. Navigational skills correlate with hippocampal fractional anisotropy in humans. Hippocampus 18 (2008) 335-339
    • (2008) Hippocampus , vol.18 , pp. 335-339
    • Iaria, G.1    Lanyon, L.J.2    Fox, C.J.3    Giaschi, D.4    Barton, J.J.S.5
  • 34
    • 57249089079 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Developmental topographical disorientation: case one
    • Iaria G., Bogod N., Fox C.J., and Barton J.J.S. Developmental topographical disorientation: case one. Neuropsychologia 47 (2009) 30-40
    • (2009) Neuropsychologia , vol.47 , pp. 30-40
    • Iaria, G.1    Bogod, N.2    Fox, C.J.3    Barton, J.J.S.4
  • 37
    • 84859784991 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The organization and development of spatial cognition: insights from Williams syndrome
    • Burack J. (Ed), Oxford University Press. A proposal for the organization of spatial cognition, its development in humans, and its susceptibility to impairment, using the framework of the dorsal and ventral streams of the brain.
    • Landau B. The organization and development of spatial cognition: insights from Williams syndrome. In: Burack J. (Ed). Handbook of Intellectual Disabilities and Development (2009), Oxford University Press. A proposal for the organization of spatial cognition, its development in humans, and its susceptibility to impairment, using the framework of the dorsal and ventral streams of the brain.
    • (2009) Handbook of Intellectual Disabilities and Development
    • Landau, B.1
  • 38
    • 0034717822 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • TENS modulates spatial reorientation in neglect patients
    • Guariglia C., Coriale G., and Cosentino T. TENS modulates spatial reorientation in neglect patients. Neuroreport 11 (2000) 1945-1948
    • (2000) Neuroreport , vol.11 , pp. 1945-1948
    • Guariglia, C.1    Coriale, G.2    Cosentino, T.3
  • 39
    • 33947140090 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Molecular basis of a multisystem disorder
    • Morris C.A., Lenhoff H.M., and Wang P.P. (Eds), JHU Press
    • Osborne L.R. Molecular basis of a multisystem disorder. In: Morris C.A., Lenhoff H.M., and Wang P.P. (Eds). Williams-Beuren Syndrome: Research, Evaluation, and Treatment (2006), JHU Press 18-58
    • (2006) Williams-Beuren Syndrome: Research, Evaluation, and Treatment , pp. 18-58
    • Osborne, L.R.1
  • 40
    • 0036636932 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Children's use of landmarks: implications for modularity theory
    • Learmonth A.E., Nadel L., and Newcombe N.S. Children's use of landmarks: implications for modularity theory. Psychol Sci 13 (2002) 337-341
    • (2002) Psychol Sci , vol.13 , pp. 337-341
    • Learmonth, A.E.1    Nadel, L.2    Newcombe, N.S.3
  • 41
    • 33745610873 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Dissecting the geometric module: a sense linkage for metric and landmark information in animals' spatial reorientation
    • This paper explores effects of space size observed in nonhuman animals that are similar to those observed in human children, and proposes a hypothesis to explain the effects of space size across both species.
    • Sovrano V.A., and Vallortigara G. Dissecting the geometric module: a sense linkage for metric and landmark information in animals' spatial reorientation. Psychol Sci 17 (2006) 616-621. This paper explores effects of space size observed in nonhuman animals that are similar to those observed in human children, and proposes a hypothesis to explain the effects of space size across both species.
    • (2006) Psychol Sci , vol.17 , pp. 616-621
    • Sovrano, V.A.1    Vallortigara, G.2
  • 42
    • 0033172834 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Source of flexibility in human cognition: dual task studies of space and language
    • Hermer-Vazquez L., Spelke E., and Katsnelson A. Source of flexibility in human cognition: dual task studies of space and language. Cogn Psychol 39 (1999) 3-36
    • (1999) Cogn Psychol , vol.39 , pp. 3-36
    • Hermer-Vazquez, L.1    Spelke, E.2    Katsnelson, A.3
  • 43
    • 0141523221 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The cognitive functions of language
    • Carruthers P. The cognitive functions of language. Behav Brain Sci 25 (2002) 657-726
    • (2002) Behav Brain Sci , vol.25 , pp. 657-726
    • Carruthers, P.1
  • 44
    • 0035142597 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Language, space, and the development of cognitive flexibility in humans: the case of two spatial memory tasks
    • Hermer-Vazquez L., Moffet A., and Munkholm P. Language, space, and the development of cognitive flexibility in humans: the case of two spatial memory tasks. Cognition 79 (2001) 263-299
    • (2001) Cognition , vol.79 , pp. 263-299
    • Hermer-Vazquez, L.1    Moffet, A.2    Munkholm, P.3
  • 45
    • 0036063125 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Modularity and spatial reorientation in a simple mind: encoding of geometric and nongeometric properties of a spatial environment by fish
    • Sovrano V.A., Bisazza A., and Vallortigara G. Modularity and spatial reorientation in a simple mind: encoding of geometric and nongeometric properties of a spatial environment by fish. Cognition 85 (2002) B51-B59
    • (2002) Cognition , vol.85
    • Sovrano, V.A.1    Bisazza, A.2    Vallortigara, G.3
  • 46
    • 38149142419 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Is language necessary for human spatial reorientation? Reconsidering evidence from dual task paradigms
    • In Experiment 1, adults performed a reorientation task in a rectangular room with one colored wall, while undergoing verbal interference. Adults who received vague task instructions showed impaired performance, while those that received explicit task instructions searched the correct corner. In Experiment 2, nonverbal, spatial interference (visualization task) also impaired performance. The authors argue that spatial language is not necessary for the use of features when reorienting.
    • Ratcliff K.R., and Newcombe N.S. Is language necessary for human spatial reorientation? Reconsidering evidence from dual task paradigms. Cogn Psychol 56 (2008) 142-163. In Experiment 1, adults performed a reorientation task in a rectangular room with one colored wall, while undergoing verbal interference. Adults who received vague task instructions showed impaired performance, while those that received explicit task instructions searched the correct corner. In Experiment 2, nonverbal, spatial interference (visualization task) also impaired performance. The authors argue that spatial language is not necessary for the use of features when reorienting.
    • (2008) Cogn Psychol , vol.56 , pp. 142-163
    • Ratcliff, K.R.1    Newcombe, N.S.2
  • 47
    • 38149058142 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Spatial reorientation: effects of verbal and spatial shadowing
    • Hupbach A., Hardt O., Nadel L., and Bohbot V.D. Spatial reorientation: effects of verbal and spatial shadowing. Spatial Cogn Comput 7 (2007) 213-226
    • (2007) Spatial Cogn Comput , vol.7 , pp. 213-226
    • Hupbach, A.1    Hardt, O.2    Nadel, L.3    Bohbot, V.D.4
  • 48
    • 84870940666 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Language and space: momentary interactions
    • Evans V., Bergen B., and Zinken J. (Eds), Equinox Publishing. Reviews a range of evidence supporting the view that language plays a momentary role in modulating visual and spatial representations.
    • Landau B., Dessalegn B., and Goldberg A. Language and space: momentary interactions. In: Evans V., Bergen B., and Zinken J. (Eds). Language, Cognition, and Space: The State of the Art and New Directions. Advances in Cognitive Linguistics Series (2008), Equinox Publishing. Reviews a range of evidence supporting the view that language plays a momentary role in modulating visual and spatial representations.
    • (2008) Language, Cognition, and Space: The State of the Art and New Directions. Advances in Cognitive Linguistics Series
    • Landau, B.1    Dessalegn, B.2    Goldberg, A.3
  • 49
    • 38949168093 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • More than meets the eye: The role of language in binding visual properties
    • This study provides an alternative to strong Whorfian and non-Whorfian claims. The authors show that language can exert powerful, yet momentary, effects on visual-spatial representations. See text for details.
    • Dessalegn B., and Landau B. More than meets the eye: The role of language in binding visual properties. Psychol Sci 19 (2008) 189-195. This study provides an alternative to strong Whorfian and non-Whorfian claims. The authors show that language can exert powerful, yet momentary, effects on visual-spatial representations. See text for details.
    • (2008) Psychol Sci , vol.19 , pp. 189-195
    • Dessalegn, B.1    Landau, B.2
  • 50
    • 0018878142 scopus 로고
    • A feature integration theory of attention
    • Treisman A., and Gelade G. A feature integration theory of attention. Cogn Psychol 12 (1980) 97-136
    • (1980) Cogn Psychol , vol.12 , pp. 97-136
    • Treisman, A.1    Gelade, G.2
  • 51
    • 0020013048 scopus 로고
    • Illusory conjunctions in the perception of objects
    • Treisman A., and Schmidt H. Illusory conjunctions in the perception of objects. Cogn Psychol 14 (1982) 107-141
    • (1982) Cogn Psychol , vol.14 , pp. 107-141
    • Treisman, A.1    Schmidt, H.2
  • 52
    • 0027493537 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Visual search for feature and conjunction targets with an attention deficit
    • Arguin M., Joanette Y., and Cavanagh P. Visual search for feature and conjunction targets with an attention deficit. J Cogn Neurosci 5 (2003) 436-452
    • (2003) J Cogn Neurosci , vol.5 , pp. 436-452
    • Arguin, M.1    Joanette, Y.2    Cavanagh, P.3
  • 53
    • 0001878223 scopus 로고
    • How language structures space
    • Pick H., and Acredolo L. (Eds), Plenum Press
    • Talmy L. How language structures space. In: Pick H., and Acredolo L. (Eds). Spatial Orientation: Theory, Research and Application (1983), Plenum Press 225-282
    • (1983) Spatial Orientation: Theory, Research and Application , pp. 225-282
    • Talmy, L.1
  • 56
    • 30544433910 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Language and the development of spatial reasoning
    • Carruthers P., Laurence S., and Stich S. (Eds), Oxford University Press
    • Shusterman A., and Spelke E. Language and the development of spatial reasoning. In: Carruthers P., Laurence S., and Stich S. (Eds). The Structure of the Innate Mind (2005), Oxford University Press
    • (2005) The Structure of the Innate Mind
    • Shusterman, A.1    Spelke, E.2
  • 57
    • 31044435892 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Whorf hypothesis is supported in the right visual field but not the left
    • Gilbert A.L., Regier T., Kay P., and Ivry R.B. Whorf hypothesis is supported in the right visual field but not the left. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103 (2006) 489-494
    • (2006) Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A , vol.103 , pp. 489-494
    • Gilbert, A.L.1    Regier, T.2    Kay, P.3    Ivry, R.B.4
  • 58
    • 42149189391 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Categorical perception of color is lateralized to the right hemisphere in infants, but to the left hemisphere in adults
    • By showing different category effect patterns for adults and prelinguistic infants, this paper provides strong evidence that language affects color perception. Adults discriminate between-category colors faster than within category colors and this effect is strongest when the stimuli are presented in the right visual field (left hemisphere processing). However, for four-month to six-month-old infants, the advantage for between-category discrimination is greater for the left visual field (right hemisphere processing).
    • Franklin A., Drivonikou G.V., Bevis L., Davies I.R.L., Kay P., and Regier T. Categorical perception of color is lateralized to the right hemisphere in infants, but to the left hemisphere in adults. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105 (2008) 3221-3225. By showing different category effect patterns for adults and prelinguistic infants, this paper provides strong evidence that language affects color perception. Adults discriminate between-category colors faster than within category colors and this effect is strongest when the stimuli are presented in the right visual field (left hemisphere processing). However, for four-month to six-month-old infants, the advantage for between-category discrimination is greater for the left visual field (right hemisphere processing).
    • (2008) Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A , vol.105 , pp. 3221-3225
    • Franklin, A.1    Drivonikou, G.V.2    Bevis, L.3    Davies, I.R.L.4    Kay, P.5    Regier, T.6
  • 59
    • 57449121623 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Lateralization of categorical perception of color changes with color term acquisition
    • Follow-up to their earlier study on lateralization, showing that this changes developmentally as children acquire color terms.
    • Franklin A., Drivonikou G.V., Clifford A., Kay P., Regier T., and Davies I.R.L. Lateralization of categorical perception of color changes with color term acquisition. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105 (2008) 18221-18225. Follow-up to their earlier study on lateralization, showing that this changes developmentally as children acquire color terms.
    • (2008) Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A , vol.105 , pp. 18221-18225
    • Franklin, A.1    Drivonikou, G.V.2    Clifford, A.3    Kay, P.4    Regier, T.5    Davies, I.R.L.6
  • 60
    • 44949104147 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Developmental changes in children's understanding of maps: What when, and how?
    • Plumert J., and Spencer J. (Eds), Oxford University Press. Review of the cognitive requirements for map use and the developmental changes in children's understanding of these.
    • Liben L., and Myers L. Developmental changes in children's understanding of maps: What when, and how?. In: Plumert J., and Spencer J. (Eds). The Emerging Spatial Mind (2007), Oxford University Press 193-218. Review of the cognitive requirements for map use and the developmental changes in children's understanding of these.
    • (2007) The Emerging Spatial Mind , pp. 193-218
    • Liben, L.1    Myers, L.2
  • 61
    • 84972606069 scopus 로고
    • Early understanding and use of symbols: the model model
    • DeLoache J. Early understanding and use of symbols: the model model. Curr Perspect Psychol Sci 4 (1995) 109-113
    • (1995) Curr Perspect Psychol Sci , vol.4 , pp. 109-113
    • DeLoache, J.1
  • 62
    • 0022704815 scopus 로고
    • Early map use as an unlearned ability
    • Landau B. Early map use as an unlearned ability. Cognition 22 (1986) 201-223
    • (1986) Cognition , vol.22 , pp. 201-223
    • Landau, B.1
  • 64
    • 40549127497 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Shusterman A, Lee SA, Spelke ES: Young children's spontaneous use of geometry in maps. Dev Sci 2008, 11:F1-F7. This study is the first to show that, without training, four-year-old children use the geometry in a map to guide their search for a hidden object. Children used landmarks, angle, and distance information, however they did not use left-right sense information, as shown by their tendencies to confuse the two geometrically equivalent corners in an isosceles triangle.
    • Shusterman A, Lee SA, Spelke ES: Young children's spontaneous use of geometry in maps. Dev Sci 2008, 11:F1-F7. This study is the first to show that, without training, four-year-old children use the geometry in a map to guide their search for a hidden object. Children used landmarks, angle, and distance information, however they did not use left-right sense information, as shown by their tendencies to confuse the two geometrically equivalent corners in an isosceles triangle.
  • 66
    • 85047683860 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The effect of response contingencies on scale model task performance by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)
    • Kuhlmeier V., and Boysen S. The effect of response contingencies on scale model task performance by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 115 (2001) 300-306
    • (2001) J Comp Psychol , vol.115 , pp. 300-306
    • Kuhlmeier, V.1    Boysen, S.2
  • 68
    • 31144467288 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Core knowledge of geometry in an Amazonian indigene group
    • Several experiments examined the use of geometric representations by individuals from a remote culture having little to no experience with symbolic devices. Children older than 6 and adults had no difficulty interpreting and using simple maps to guide search in an adjacent space.
    • Dehaene S., Izard V., Pica P., and Spelke E.S. Core knowledge of geometry in an Amazonian indigene group. Science 311 (2006) 381-384. Several experiments examined the use of geometric representations by individuals from a remote culture having little to no experience with symbolic devices. Children older than 6 and adults had no difficulty interpreting and using simple maps to guide search in an adjacent space.
    • (2006) Science , vol.311 , pp. 381-384
    • Dehaene, S.1    Izard, V.2    Pica, P.3    Spelke, E.S.4
  • 69
    • 0000898422 scopus 로고
    • Developmental changes in map reading skills
    • Bluestein M., and Acredolo L. Developmental changes in map reading skills. Child Dev 50 (1979) 691-697
    • (1979) Child Dev , vol.50 , pp. 691-697
    • Bluestein, M.1    Acredolo, L.2


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.