메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 72, Issue 5, 2008, Pages 542-552

An egocentric frame of reference in implicit motor sequence learning

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords

ARTICLE; ATTENTION; EGO; FEMALE; HUMAN; LEARNING; MALE; PSYCHOMOTOR PERFORMANCE;

EID: 46349087839     PISSN: 03400727     EISSN: 14302772     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1007/s00426-007-0129-z     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (16)

References (40)
  • 1
    • 0031049135 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements
    • Andersen, R. A., Snyder, L. H., Bradley, D. C., & Xing, J. (1997). Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements. Annual Review of Neurosciences, 20, 303-330.
    • (1997) Annual Review of Neurosciences , vol.20 , pp. 303-330
    • Andersen, R.A.1    Snyder, L.H.2    Bradley, D.C.3    Xing, J.4
  • 2
    • 0034065906 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Evidence for effector independent and dependent representations and their differential time course of acquisition during motor sequence learning
    • Bapi, R. S., Doya, K., & Harner, A. M. (2000). Evidence for effector independent and dependent representations and their differential time course of acquisition during motor sequence learning. Experimental Brain Research, 132, 149-162.
    • (2000) Experimental Brain Research , vol.132 , pp. 149-162
    • Bapi, R.S.1    Doya, K.2    Harner, A.M.3
  • 4
    • 8544257005 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Subtypes of optic ataxia: Reframing the disconnection account
    • Buxbaum, L. J., & Coslett, H. B. (1997). Subtypes of optic ataxia: reframing the disconnection account. Neurocase, 3, 159-166.
    • (1997) Neurocase , vol.3 , pp. 159-166
    • Buxbaum, L.J.1    Coslett, H.B.2
  • 6
    • 0031934111 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Action-oriented spatial reference frames in cortex
    • Colby, C. L. (1998). Action-oriented spatial reference frames in cortex. Neuron, 20, 15-24.
    • (1998) Neuron , vol.20 , pp. 15-24
    • Colby, C.L.1
  • 8
    • 0035312419 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Neuroimaging of cognitive functions in human parietal cortex
    • Culham, J. C., & Kanwisher, N. G. (2001). Neuroimaging of cognitive functions in human parietal cortex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11, 157-163.
    • (2001) Current Opinion in Neurobiology , vol.11 , pp. 157-163
    • Culham, J.C.1    Kanwisher, N.G.2
  • 10
    • 0028243230 scopus 로고
    • Accuracy of planar reaching movements. I. Independence of direction and extent variability
    • Gordon, J., Ghilardi, M. F., & Ghez, C. (1994). Accuracy of planar reaching movements. I. Independence of direction and extent variability. Experimental Brain Research, 99, 97-111.
    • (1994) Experimental Brain Research , vol.99 , pp. 97-111
    • Gordon, J.1    Ghilardi, M.F.2    Ghez, C.3
  • 11
    • 0032533324 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Abstract and effector-specific representations of motor sequences identified with PET
    • Grafton, S. T., Hazeltine, E., & Ivry, R. I. (1998). Abstract and effector-specific representations of motor sequences identified with PET. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 9420-9428.
    • (1998) Journal of Neuroscience , vol.18 , pp. 9420-9428
    • Grafton, S.T.1    Hazeltine, E.2    Ivry, R.I.3
  • 12
    • 0242483997 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The representational nature of sequence learning: Evidence for goal-based codes
    • Oxford University Press Oxford
    • Hazeltine, E. (2002). The representational nature of sequence learning: Evidence for goal-based codes. In W. H. Prinz, & B. Hommel (Eds.), Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action (pp. 673-689). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    • (2002) Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action , pp. 673-689
    • Hazeltine, E.1    Prinz, W.H.2    Hommel, B.3
  • 13
    • 0036545967 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Motor learning by observation: Evidence from a serial reaction time task
    • Heyes, C. M., & Foster, C. L. (2002). Motor learning by observation: Evidence from a serial reaction time task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 593-607.
    • (2002) The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , vol.55 , pp. 593-607
    • Heyes, C.M.1    Foster, C.L.2
  • 15
    • 0142155617 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Unique transitions between stimuli and responses in SRT tasks: Evidence for the primacy of response predictions
    • Hoffman, J., Martin, C., & Schilling, A. (2003). Unique transitions between stimuli and responses in SRT tasks: Evidence for the primacy of response predictions. Psychological Research, 67, 160-173.
    • (2003) Psychological Research , vol.67 , pp. 160-173
    • Hoffman, J.1    Martin, C.2    Schilling, A.3
  • 17
    • 0037212969 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Intermanual transfer of procedural learning after extended practice of probabilistic sequences.
    • Japikse, K. C., Negash, S., Howard, J. H., & Howard, D. V. (2003). Intermanual transfer of procedural learning after extended practice of probabilistic sequences. Experimental Brain Research, 148, 38-49.
    • (2003) Experimental Brain Research , vol.148 , pp. 38-49
    • Japikse, K.C.1    Negash, S.2    Howard, J.H.3    Howard, D.V.4
  • 19
    • 0035230735 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Learning complex sequences: No role for observation?
    • Kelly, S. W., & Burton, A. M. (2001). Learning complex sequences: No role for observation? Psychological Research, 65, 15-23.
    • (2001) Psychological Research , vol.65 , pp. 15-23
    • Kelly, S.W.1    Burton, A.M.2
  • 20
    • 0242391110 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Sequence learning by action and observation: Evidence for separate mechanisms
    • Kelly, S. W., Burton, A. M., Riedel, B., & Lynch, E. (2003). Sequence learning by action and observation: Evidence for separate mechanisms. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 355-372.
    • (2003) British Journal of Psychology , vol.94 , pp. 355-372
    • Kelly, S.W.1    Burton, A.M.2    Riedel, B.3    Lynch, E.4
  • 21
    • 34250899079 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The representation of explicit motor sequence knowledge
    • Knee, R., Thomason, S., Ashe, J., & Willingham, D. T. (2007). The representation of explicit motor sequence knowledge. Memory & Cognition, 35, 326-333.
    • (2007) Memory & Cognition , vol.35 , pp. 326-333
    • Knee, R.1    Thomason, S.2    Ashe, J.3    Willingham, D.T.4
  • 23
    • 0028028276 scopus 로고
    • Coding of spatial information in the somatosensory system: Evidence from patients with neglect following parietal lobe damage
    • Moscovitch, M., & Behrmann, M. (1994). Coding of spatial information in the somatosensory system: Evidence from patients with neglect following parietal lobe damage. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 151-155.
    • (1994) Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , vol.6 , pp. 151-155
    • Moscovitch, M.1    Behrmann, M.2
  • 24
    • 0001847553 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Stimulus and response anticipation in a serial reaction time task
    • Nattkemper, D., & Prinz, W. (1997). Stimulus and response anticipation in a serial reaction time task. Psychological Research, 60, 98-112.
    • (1997) Psychological Research , vol.60 , pp. 98-112
    • Nattkemper, D.1    Prinz, W.2
  • 25
    • 0003109533 scopus 로고
    • Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures
    • Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1-32.
    • (1987) Cognitive Psychology , vol.19 , pp. 1-32
    • Nissen, M.J.1    Bullemer, P.2
  • 26
    • 0037374108 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Effect of practice on effector independence
    • Park, J., & Shea, C. (2003). Effect of practice on effector independence. Journal of Motor Behavior, 35, 33-40.
    • (2003) Journal of Motor Behavior , vol.35 , pp. 33-40
    • Park, J.1    Shea, C.2
  • 29
    • 0034150896 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Implicit and explicit learning of event sequences: Evidence for distinct coding of perceptual and motor representations
    • Russeler, J., & Rosler, F. (2000). Implicit and explicit learning of event sequences: Evidence for distinct coding of perceptual and motor representations. Acta Psychologia, 104, 45-67.
    • (2000) Acta Psychologia , vol.104 , pp. 45-67
    • Russeler, J.1    Rosler, F.2
  • 30
    • 0032572753 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Separate body- and world- referenced representations of visual space in parietal cortex
    • Snyder, L. H., Grieve, K. L., Brotchie, P., & Andersen, R. A. (1998). Separate body- and world- referenced representations of visual space in parietal cortex. Nature, 394, 887-891.
    • (1998) Nature , vol.394 , pp. 887-891
    • Snyder, L.H.1    Grieve, K.L.2    Brotchie, P.3    Andersen, R.A.4
  • 31
    • 3042764477 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Effector-independent and effector-dependent learning in the discrete sequence production task
    • Verwey, W. B., & Wright, D. (2004). Effector-independent and effector-dependent learning in the discrete sequence production task. Psychological Research, 68, 64-70.
    • (2004) Psychological Research , vol.68 , pp. 64-70
    • Verwey, W.B.1    Wright, D.2
  • 32
    • 15544371626 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The serial RT task: Indications for components with different learning rates
    • Verwey, W. B., & Clegg, B. (2005). The serial RT task: Indications for components with different learning rates. Psychological Research, 69, 242-251.
    • (2005) Psychological Research , vol.69 , pp. 242-251
    • Verwey, W.B.1    Clegg, B.2
  • 34
    • 0032116811 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning
    • Willingham, D. B. (1998). A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 105, 558-584.
    • (1998) Psychological Review , vol.105 , pp. 558-584
    • Willingham, D.B.1
  • 35
    • 0032916025 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Implicit motor sequence learning is not purely perceptual
    • Willingham, D. B. (1999). Implicit motor sequence learning is not purely perceptual. Memory & Cognition, 27, 561-572.
    • (1999) Memory & Cognition , vol.27 , pp. 561-572
    • Willingham, D.B.1
  • 37
    • 0034090066 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Implicit motor sequence learning is represented in response locations
    • Willingham, D. B., Wells, L. A., Farrell, J. M., & Stemwedel, M. E. (2000). Implicit motor sequence learning is represented in response locations. Memory & Cognition, 28, 366-375.
    • (2000) Memory & Cognition , vol.28 , pp. 366-375
    • Willingham, D.B.1    Wells, L.A.2    Farrell, J.M.3    Stemwedel, M.E.4
  • 39
    • 0028725151 scopus 로고
    • The impact of motor responses on serial-pattern learning
    • Ziessler, M. (1994). The impact of motor responses on serial-pattern learning. Psychological Research, 57, 30-41.
    • (1994) Psychological Research , vol.57 , pp. 30-41
    • Ziessler, M.1
  • 40
    • 85047682514 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Learning of event sequences is based on response-effect learning: Further evidence from a serial reaction task
    • Ziessler, M., & Nattkemper, D. (2001). Learning of event sequences is based on response-effect learning: Further evidence from a serial reaction task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 595-613.
    • (2001) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , vol.27 , pp. 595-613
    • Ziessler, M.1    Nattkemper, D.2


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.