메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 130, Issue 1, 2005, Pages 74-118

Relating musical structure and content to aesthetic response: A model and analysis of Beethoven's Piano Sonata op. 110

(1)  Ockelford, Adam a  

a NONE

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords


EID: 39749121492     PISSN: 02690403     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1093/jrma/fki002     Document Type: Review
Times cited : (14)

References (120)
  • 1
    • 77951918827 scopus 로고
    • Integration and Narrative Design in Beethoven's Piano Sonata in Amusic flat sign Major, Opus 110
    • ed. Christopher Reynolds, Lewis Lockwood and James Webster, Lincoln, NE, and London, (p. 111)
    • William Kinderman, 'Integration and Narrative Design in Beethoven's Piano Sonata in Amusic flat sign Major, Opus 110', Beethoven Forum, 1, ed. Christopher Reynolds, Lewis Lockwood and James Webster (Lincoln, NE, and London, 1992), 111-45 (p. 111).
    • (1992) Beethoven Forum , vol.1 , pp. 111-145
    • Kinderman, W.1
  • 3
    • 0039576772 scopus 로고
    • The Role of Repetition in Perceived Musical Structures
    • ed. Peter Howell, Robert West and Ian Cross (London)
    • See, for example, Adam Ockelford, 'The Role of Repetition in Perceived Musical Structures', Representing Musical Structure, ed. Peter Howell, Robert West and Ian Cross (London, 1991), 129-60;
    • (1991) Representing Musical Structure , pp. 129-160
    • Ockelford, A.1
  • 6
    • 84990394511 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The Magical Number Two, Plus or Minus One: Some Limits on our Capacity for Processing Musical Information
    • 'The Magical Number Two, Plus or Minus One: Some Limits on our Capacity for Processing Musical Information', Musicae scientiae, 6 (2002), 177-215;
    • (2002) Musicae Scientiae , vol.6 , pp. 177-215
  • 7
    • 39749111332 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • On Similarity, Derivation, and the Cognition of Musical Structure
    • 'On Similarity, Derivation, and the Cognition of Musical Structure', Psychology of Music, 32 (2004), 23-74.
    • (2004) Psychology of Music , vol.32 , pp. 23-74
  • 9
    • 79956463354 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • David Temperley, personal communication, 2003
    • David Temperley, personal communication, 2003.
  • 10
    • 4243388972 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • An Experimental Music Theory?
    • ed. Nicholas Cook and Mark Everist (Oxford), p. 166
    • Robert Gjerdingen, 'An Experimental Music Theory?', Rethinking Music, ed. Nicholas Cook and Mark Everist (Oxford, 1999), 161-70 (p. 166).
    • (1999) Rethinking Music , pp. 161-170
    • Gjerdingen, R.1
  • 12
    • 84959944843 scopus 로고
    • Basic Emotions, Rationality, and Folk Theory
    • (p. 201)
    • Since, as Philip Johnson-Laird and Keith Oatley observe, survival often requires that, as sentient creatures interacting with highly complex environments that are ultimately irreducible to logical analysis, we gauge sensory input intuitively. Johnson-Laird and Oatley, 'Basic Emotions, Rationality, and Folk Theory', Cognition and Emotion, 6 (1992), 201-23 (p. 201).
    • (1992) Cognition and Emotion , vol.6 , pp. 201-223
    • Johnson-Laird1    Oatley2
  • 13
    • 8644270315 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Music and Emotion: Introduction
    • ed. Juslin and Sloboda, p. 3
    • Patrik Juslin and John Sloboda, 'Music and Emotion: Introduction', Music and Emotion, ed. Juslin and Sloboda, 3-20 (p. 3).
    • Music and Emotion , pp. 3-20
    • Juslin, P.1    Sloboda, J.2
  • 14
    • 64549160971 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Introduction: New Ways of Thinking about Musical Meaning
    • ed. Jenefer Robinson (Ithaca, NY, and London), pp. 2-3
    • See Jenefer Robinson, 'Introduction: New Ways of Thinking about Musical Meaning', Music and Meaning, ed. Jenefer Robinson (Ithaca, NY, and London, 1997), 1-20 (pp. 2-3).
    • (1997) Music and Meaning , pp. 1-20
    • Robinson, J.1
  • 16
    • 1642268490 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Musicological Approaches to Emotion
    • ed. Juslin and Sloboda p. 50
    • Nicholas Cook and Nicola Dibben, 'Musicological Approaches to Emotion', Music and Emotion, ed. Juslin and Sloboda, 45-70 (p. 50).
    • Music and Emotion , pp. 45-70
    • Cook, N.1    Dibben, N.2
  • 18
    • 0002107983 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Experimental Aesthetics and Everyday Music Listening
    • ed. David Hargreaves and Adrian North (Oxford), p. 84
    • See Adrian North and David Hargreaves, 'Experimental Aesthetics and Everyday Music Listening', The Social Psychology of Music, ed. David Hargreaves and Adrian North (Oxford, 1997), 84-103 (p. 84);
    • (1997) The Social Psychology of Music , pp. 84-103
    • North, A.1    Hargreaves, D.2
  • 19
    • 33744563482 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Does Music Mean Anything?
    • 26
    • John Sloboda, 'Does Music Mean Anything?', Musicae scientiae, 2 (1998), 21-32 (p. 26).
    • (1998) Musicae Scientiae , vol.2 , pp. 21-32
    • Sloboda, J.1
  • 20
    • 0005756545 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Psychological Perspectives on Music and Emotion
    • ed. Juslin and Sloboda, p. 81
    • John Sloboda and Patrik Juslin, 'Psychological Perspectives on Music and Emotion', Music and Emotion, ed. Juslin and Sloboda, 71-104 (p. 81).
    • Music and Emotion , pp. 71-104
    • Sloboda, J.1    Juslin, P.2
  • 23
    • 0242422832 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Current Trends in the Study of Music and Emotion: Overture
    • (p. 3)
    • See Patrik Juslin and Marcel Zentner, 'Current Trends in the Study of Music and Emotion: Overture', Musicae scientiae, special issue 2001/02, 3-21 (p. 3).
    • (2001) Musicae Scientiae , vol.2 , pp. 3-21
    • Juslin, P.1    Zentner, M.2
  • 24
    • 33745249958 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Psychological Connotations of Harmonic Musical Intervals
    • See, for instance, Marco Costa, Pio Bitti and Luisa Bonfiglio, 'Psychological Connotations of Harmonic Musical Intervals', Psychology of Music, 28 (2000), 4-22;
    • (2000) Psychology of Music , vol.28 , pp. 4-22
    • Costa, M.1    Bitti, P.2    Bonfiglio, L.3
  • 25
    • 0038071870 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Perceiving Emotion in Melody: Interactive Effects of Pitch and Rhythm
    • Glenn Schellenberg, Ania Krysciak and Jane Campbell, 'Perceiving Emotion in Melody: Interactive Effects of Pitch and Rhythm', Music Perception, 18 (2000), 155-71;
    • (2000) Music Perception , vol.18 , pp. 155-171
    • Schellenberg, G.1    Krysciak, A.2    Campbell, J.3
  • 26
    • 1642325338 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The Influence of Musical Structure on Musical Expression
    • ed. Juslin and Sloboda
    • and Alf Gabrielsson and Erik Lindström, 'The Influence of Musical Structure on Musical Expression', Music and Emotion, ed. Juslin and Sloboda, 223-48.
    • Music and Emotion , pp. 223-248
    • Gabrielsson, A.1    Lindström, E.2
  • 27
    • 27144470753 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Aesthetic Agency and Musical Practice: New Directions in the Sociology of Music and Emotion
    • ed. Juslin and Sloboda
    • Tia DeNora, 'Aesthetic Agency and Musical Practice: New Directions in the Sociology of Music and Emotion', Music and Emotion, ed. Juslin and Sloboda, 161-80.
    • Music and Emotion , pp. 161-180
    • Denora, T.1
  • 28
    • 0032147330 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Music and Emotion: Perceptual Determinants, Immediacy, and Isolation after Brain Damage
    • Isabelle Peretz, Lise Gagnon and Bernard Bouchard, 'Music and Emotion: Perceptual Determinants, Immediacy, and Isolation after Brain Damage', Cognition, 68 (1998), 111-41;
    • (1998) Cognition , vol.68 , pp. 111-141
    • Peretz, I.1    Gagnon, L.2    Bouchard, B.3
  • 29
    • 0000288293 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Listen to the Brain: A Biological Perspective on Music and Emotion
    • ed. Juslin and Sloboda
    • Isabelle Peretz, 'Listen to the Brain: A Biological Perspective on Music and Emotion', Music and Emotion, ed. Juslin and Sloboda, 105-34.
    • Music and Emotion , pp. 105-134
    • Peretz, I.1
  • 30
    • 0242506153 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Anthropological Perspectives on Music and Emotion
    • ed. Juslin and Sloboda
    • Judith Becker, 'Anthropological Perspectives on Music and Emotion', Music and Emotion, ed. Juslin and Sloboda, 135-60.
    • Music and Emotion , pp. 135-160
    • Becker, J.1
  • 31
    • 0038410367 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Emotional Effects of Music: Production Rules
    • ed. Juslin and Sloboda, p. 380
    • Klaus Scherer and Marcel Zentner, 'Emotional Effects of Music: Production Rules', Music and Emotion, ed. Juslin and Sloboda, 361-92 (p. 380);
    • Music and Emotion , pp. 361-392
    • Scherer, K.1    Zentner, M.2
  • 32
    • 10144237638 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A Review of the Musical Mood Induction Procedure
    • Daniel Västfjäll, 'A Review of the Musical Mood Induction Procedure', Musicae scientiae, special issue 2001/02, 173-211.
    • (2002) Musicae Scientiae , pp. 173-211
    • Västfjäll, D.1
  • 34
    • 61049093477 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Emotional Responses to Music: Implicit and Explicit Effects in Listeners and Performers
    • 66
    • Mitch Waterman, 'Emotional Responses to Music: Implicit and Explicit Effects in Listeners and Performers', Psychology of Music, 24 (1996), 53-67 (p. 66).
    • (1996) Psychology of Music , vol.24 , pp. 53-67
    • Waterman, M.1
  • 37
    • 0242718437 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Some Theories of Emotion in Music and their Implications for Research in Music Psychology
    • (p. 25)
    • Justin London, 'Some Theories of Emotion in Music and their Implications for Research in Music Psychology', Musicae scientiae, special issue 2001/02, 23-36 (p. 25).
    • (2001) Musicae Scientiae , vol.2 , pp. 23-36
    • London, J.1
  • 38
    • 33645214167 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • What are Musical Identities, and Why are They Important?
    • ed. Raymond MacDonald, David Hargreaves and Dorothy Miell (Oxford), p. 11
    • David Hargreaves, Dorothy Miell and Raymond MacDonald, 'What are Musical Identities, and Why are They Important?', Musical Identities, ed. Raymond MacDonald, David Hargreaves and Dorothy Miell (Oxford, 2002), 1-20 (p. 11).
    • (2002) Musical Identities , pp. 1-20
    • Hargreaves, D.1    Miell, D.2    MacDonald, R.3
  • 40
    • 84970188402 scopus 로고
    • An Experimental Study of Deryck Cooke's Theory of Music and Meaning'
    • See, for example, Clive Gabriel, 'An Experimental Study of Deryck Cooke's Theory of Music and Meaning', Psychology of Music, 6 (1978), 13-20;
    • (1978) Psychology of Music , vol.6 , pp. 13-20
    • Gabriel, C.1
  • 41
    • 79956456836 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A Naturalistic Empirical Investigation of Deryck Cooke's Theory of Music and Meaning
    • ed. Chris Woods, Geoff Luck, Renaud Brochard, Fred Seddon and John Sloboda (CD, Keele)
    • and Peter Hampson, 'A Naturalistic Empirical Investigation of Deryck Cooke's Theory of Music and Meaning', Sixth International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition, ed. Chris Woods, Geoff Luck, Renaud Brochard, Fred Seddon and John Sloboda (CD, Keele, 2000).
    • (2000) Sixth International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition
    • Hampson, P.1
  • 42
    • 0012784374 scopus 로고
    • The Nature and Measurement of Musical Meanings
    • Karl Watson, 'The Nature and Measurement of Musical Meanings', Psychological Monographs, 54 (1942), 1-43.
    • (1942) Psychological Monographs , vol.54 , pp. 1-43
    • Watson, K.1
  • 43
    • 0001309343 scopus 로고
    • Cue Utilization in Emotion Attribution from Auditory Stimuli
    • Klaus Scherer and James Oshinsky, 'Cue Utilization in Emotion Attribution from Auditory Stimuli', Motivation and Emotion, 1 (1977), 331-46.
    • (1977) Motivation and Emotion , vol.1 , pp. 331-346
    • Scherer, K.1    Oshinsky, J.2
  • 44
    • 0015453033 scopus 로고
    • Multidimensional Study of Perceptual-Emotional Qualities in Music
    • Watson, 'The Nature and Measurement of Musical Meanings'; Lage Wedin, 'Multidimensional Study of Perceptual-Emotional Qualities in Music', Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 13 (1972), 241-57.
    • (1972) Scandinavian Journal of Psychology , vol.13 , pp. 241-257
    • Wedin, L.1
  • 45
  • 46
    • 0000839868 scopus 로고
    • Factors Determining the Characterization of Musical Phrases
    • Ralph Gundlach, 'Factors Determining the Characterization of Musical Phrases', American Journal of Psychology, 47 (1935), 624-44.
    • (1935) American Journal of Psychology , vol.47 , pp. 624-644
    • Gundlach, R.1
  • 47
    • 0002120254 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A Cross-Cultural Investigation of the Perception of Emotion in Music: Psychology and Cultural Cues
    • Laura-Lee Balkwill and William Thompson, 'A Cross-Cultural Investigation of the Perception of Emotion in Music: Psychology and Cultural Cues', Music Perception, 17 (1999), 43-64.
    • (1999) Music Perception , vol.17 , pp. 43-64
    • Balkwill, L.-L.1    Thompson, W.2
  • 50
    • 84965693928 scopus 로고
    • Emotional Experience of Music as a Function of Musical Structure
    • Sören Nielzén and Zvonimir Cesarec, 'Emotional Experience of Music as a Function of Musical Structure', Psychology of Music, 10 (1982), 7-17.
    • (1982) Psychology of Music , vol.10 , pp. 7-17
    • Nielzén, S.1    Cesarec, Z.2
  • 51
    • 84990321313 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Perceived Emotional Expression in Synthesized Performances of a Short Melody. Capturing the Listener's Judgement Policy'
    • Patrik Juslin, 'Perceived Emotional Expression in Synthesized Performances of a Short Melody. Capturing the Listener's Judgement Policy', Musicae scientiae, 1 (1997), 225-56.
    • (1997) Musicae Scientiae , vol.1 , pp. 225-256
    • Juslin, P.1
  • 52
    • 17644425937 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Music and Emotion: Distinctions and Uncertainties
    • ed. Juslin and Sloboda, p. 342
    • Leonard Meyer, 'Music and Emotion: Distinctions and Uncertainties', Music and Emotion, ed. Juslin and Sloboda, 341-60 (p. 342).
    • Music and Emotion , pp. 341-360
    • Meyer, L.1
  • 54
    • 0242506128 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Toward a Computational Model of Expression in Music Performance: The GERM Model
    • Patrik Juslin, Anders Friberg and Roberto Bresin, 'Toward a Computational Model of Expression in Music Performance: The GERM Model', Musicae scientiae, special issue 2001/02, 63-122.
    • (2002) Musicae Scientiae , pp. 63-122
    • Juslin, P.1    Friberg, A.2    Bresin, R.3
  • 55
    • 23044523714 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Emotion Inferences from Vocal Expression Correlate Across Languages and Cultures
    • Klaus Scherer, Rainer Banse and Harad Wallbott, 'Emotion Inferences from Vocal Expression Correlate Across Languages and Cultures', Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32 (2001), 76-92.
    • (2001) Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology , vol.32 , pp. 76-92
    • Scherer, K.1    Banse, R.2    Wallbott, H.3
  • 56
    • 0002138882 scopus 로고
    • Emotion Expression in Speech and Music
    • ed. Johan Sundberg, Lennart Nord and Rolf Carlson London
    • Klaus Scherer, 'Emotion Expression in Speech and Music', Music, Language, Speech and Brain, ed. Johan Sundberg, Lennart Nord and Rolf Carlson (London, 1991), 146-56;
    • (1991) Music, Language, Speech and Brain , pp. 146-156
    • Scherer, K.1
  • 57
    • 0002730887 scopus 로고
    • Affect Bursts
    • ed. Stephanie van Goozen, Nanne van de Poll and Joseph Sergeant (Hillsdale, NJ)
    • 'Affect Bursts', Emotions: Essays on Emotion Theory, ed. Stephanie van Goozen, Nanne van de Poll and Joseph Sergeant (Hillsdale, NJ, 1994), 161-96;
    • (1994) Emotions: Essays on Emotion Theory , pp. 161-196
  • 58
    • 0029083813 scopus 로고
    • Expression of Emotion in Voice and Music
    • 'Expression of Emotion in Voice and Music', Journal of Voice, 9 (1995), 235-48.
    • (1995) Journal of Voice , vol.9 , pp. 235-248
  • 59
    • 0005050738 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Mothers and Infants and Communicative Musicality
    • Stephen Malloch, 'Mothers and Infants and Communicative Musicality', Musicae scientiae, special issue 1999/2000, 29-54;
    • (1999) Musicae Scientiae , pp. 29-54
    • Malloch, S.1
  • 60
    • 0038748493 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Emotion and Music in Infancy
    • Sandra Trehub and Takayuki Nakata, 'Emotion and Music in Infancy', Musicae scientiae, special issue 2001/02, 37-61;
    • (2002) Musicae Scientiae , pp. 37-61
    • Trehub, S.1    Nakata, T.2
  • 61
    • 9244245016 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Origins of Musical Identity: Evidence from Infancy for Musical Social Awareness
    • ed. MacDonald, Hargreaves and Miell
    • Colwyn Trevarthen, 'Origins of Musical Identity: Evidence from Infancy for Musical Social Awareness', Musical Identities, ed. MacDonald, Hargreaves and Miell, 21-38.
    • Musical Identities , pp. 21-38
    • Trevarthen, C.1
  • 64
    • 79958750096 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • An Exploratory Study of Musical Emotions and Psychophysiology
    • Carol Krumhansl, 'An Exploratory Study of Musical Emotions and Psychophysiology', Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51 (1997), 336-52;
    • (1997) Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology , vol.51 , pp. 336-352
    • Krumhansl, C.1
  • 67
    • 0001175419 scopus 로고
    • Experimental Studies of the Elements of Expression in Music
    • Kate Hevner, 'Experimental Studies of the Elements of Expression in Music', American Journal of Psychology, 48 (1936), 246-68;
    • (1936) American Journal of Psychology , vol.48 , pp. 246-268
    • Hevner, K.1
  • 68
    • 0039832719 scopus 로고
    • Perception of the Major/Minor Distinction: III. Hedonic, Musical and Affective Discriminations
    • Robert Crowder, 'Perception of the Major/Minor Distinction: III. Hedonic, Musical and Affective Discriminations', Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 23 (1985), 314-16;
    • (1985) Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society , vol.23 , pp. 314-316
    • Crowder, R.1
  • 72
    • 84968249289 scopus 로고
    • Music Theory, Phenomenology, and Modes of Perception
    • (pp. 329ff.)
    • revisited in David Lewin, 'Music Theory, Phenomenology, and Modes of Perception', Music Perception, 3 (1986), 327-92 (pp. 329ff.).
    • (1986) Music Perception , vol.3 , pp. 327-392
    • Lewin, D.1
  • 74
    • 26544454533 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Correlation Analysis of Continuous Emotional Response to Music: Correcting for the Effects of Serial Correlation
    • Emery Schubert, 'Correlation Analysis of Continuous Emotional Response to Music: Correcting for the Effects of Serial Correlation', Musicae scientiae, special issue 2001/02, 213-36.
    • (2002) Musicae Scientiae , pp. 213-236
    • Schubert, E.1
  • 75
    • 84970156704 scopus 로고
    • Music Structure and Emotional Response: Some Empirical Findings
    • On the basis of listeners' retrospective accounts, John Sloboda confirmed musicians' intuitive notion that melodic appoggiaturas are one of the features of Western classical music capable of triggering a marked emotional response. See Sloboda, 'Music Structure and Emotional Response: Some Empirical Findings', Psychology of Music, 19 (1991), 110-20.
    • (1991) Psychology of Music , vol.19 , pp. 110-120
    • Sloboda1
  • 79
    • 79956515054 scopus 로고
    • Cambridge, MA
    • Reflecting further on the comparison with language, while it is the meanings of words that tend to elicit an emotional response, it is their syntactical structure and sonic qualities that induce the aesthetic component of our reaction to a text (beauty, felicity of expression and so forth). Repetition (the foundation of structure in music) sometimes plays a part: through rhyme, assonance, alliteration, anaphora and metre, for example. See Carlos Chavéz, Musical Thought (Cambridge, MA, 1961), 39, 40;
    • (1961) Musical Thought , vol.39 , pp. 40
    • Chavéz, C.1
  • 82
    • 79956456628 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Some Theories of Emotion in Music
    • See London, 'Some Theories of Emotion in Music', 25.
    • London1
  • 83
    • 61049216615 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Is There even a Grammar of Music?
    • 58
    • See Douglas Dempster, 'Is There Even a Grammar of Music?', Musicae scientiae, 2 (1998), 55-65 (p. 58).
    • (1998) Musicae Scientiae , vol.2 , pp. 55-65
    • Dempster, D.1
  • 84
    • 39749124312 scopus 로고
    • Thematic Content: A Schenkerian Views
    • ed. David Beach, New Haven and London, (p. 39)
    • See John Rothgeb, 'Thematic Content: A Schenkerian View', Aspects of Schenkerian Theory, ed. David Beach (New Haven and London, 1983), 39-60 (p. 39): 'Music, lacking access to the kinds of direct association with the phenomenal world central to most other art forms, was able to satisfy the universal requirement of association only through the "likeness of itself" - through self-repetition.'
    • (1983) Aspects of Schenkerian Theory , pp. 39-60
    • Rothgeb, J.1
  • 85
    • 0009188764 scopus 로고
    • Music and Duration
    • ed. Suzanne Langer, London, (p. 156)
    • Also Basil de Selincourt, 'Music and Duration', Reflections on Art, ed. Suzanne Langer (London, 1958), 152-60 (p. 156): 'The value of repetition in music belongs of course to the peculiar inwardness of the art. A musical composition must be content to be itself. The reference and relations into which analysis resolves its life-current need point to no object, no event; they take the form of the creative impulse which is their unity and they repeat one another because iteration is the only outward sign of identity which is available to them.'
    • (1958) Reflections on Art , pp. 152-160
    • De Selincourt, B.1
  • 86
    • 33749867240 scopus 로고
    • On Derivation: Syntax and Rhetoric
    • 237
    • Edward Cone, 'On Derivation: Syntax and Rhetoric', Music Analysis, 6 (1987), 237-55 (p. 237).
    • (1987) Music Analysis , vol.6 , pp. 237-255
    • Cone, E.1
  • 89
    • 0007326398 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Metaphor and Music Theory
    • vol. 1
    • See Lawrence Zbikowski, 'Metaphor and Music Theory', Music Theory Online, 4 (1998), vol. 1;
    • (1998) Music Theory Online , vol.4
    • Zbikowski, L.1
  • 90
    • 1442316304 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • New York
    • and Conceptualizing Music: Cognitive Structure, Theory, and Analysis (New York, 2002). Building on the work of George Lakoff, Zbikowski shows how such notions are underpinned by culture-specific conceptual metaphors, mapped onto the domain of music-space from our perception of the physical world.
    • (2002) Conceptualizing Music: Cognitive Structure, Theory, and Analysis
  • 92
    • 2442617540 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Music Expectations by Cognitive Rule-Mapping
    • 364
    • See Eugene Narmour, 'Music Expectations by Cognitive Rule-Mapping', Music Perception, 17 (2000), 329-98 (p. 364).
    • (2000) Music Perception , vol.17 , pp. 329-398
    • Narmour, E.1
  • 93
    • 79956515701 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A fuller discussion of key issues in zygonic theory, including what counts as sameness and similarity, and how these interact with the notions of salience, categorization and derivation, is to be found elsewhere. See Ockelford, 'On Similarity, Derivation, and the Cognition of Musical Structure'.
    • On Similarity, Derivation, and the Cognition of Musical Structure
    • Ockelford1
  • 94
    • 0020019359 scopus 로고
    • Referential Continuity and the Coherence of Discourse
    • See Alan Garnham, Jane Oakhill and Philip Johnson-Laird, 'Referential Continuity and the Coherence of Discourse', Cognition, 11 (1982), 29-46.
    • (1982) Cognition , vol.11 , pp. 29-46
    • Garnham, A.1    Oakhill, J.2    Johnson-Laird, P.3
  • 96
    • 0011238726 scopus 로고
    • Philadelphia, 84ff
    • Cf. Meyer's reason for preferring the notion of implication to that of causation in music theory and analysis: that the former makes more sense in the context of uncertainty, which he holds to be the catalyst of emotional response to music, see, for example, Meyer, Style and Music: Theory, History, and Ideology, Philadelphia, 1989, 84ff.
    • (1989) Style and Music: Theory, History, and Ideology
    • Meyer1
  • 97
    • 79956456531 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • 'Music and Emotion, Distinctions and Uncertainties', 346-7. However, as I argue above, since informed anticipation appears to play a central role in musical affect, uncertainty cannot, and it is because relationships between objects in music are only metaphorical that the concept of implication is preferable to that of causation.
    • Music and Emotion, Distinctions and Uncertainties , pp. 346-347
  • 98
    • 84888338149 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • See also Sperber and Wilson, Relevance, 72, who contend that 'for a representation to be amenable to logical processing, all that is necessary is for it to be well-formed' (rather than semantically complete - that is, representative of a state of affairs in a possible or actual world). In Sperber and Wilson's terms, logic in language (that is semantically complete, and therefore capable of being true or false) can be described as propositional, whereas logic in music would be considered non-propositional
    • Relevance , pp. 72
    • Sperber1    Wilson2
  • 99
    • 79956515630 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Is There even a Grammar of Music?
    • (see also Dempster, 'Is There Even a Grammar of Music?', 61). In language, Sperber and Wilson contend (p. 84), 'logical implication' is a syntactic relation which holds purely by virtue of the formal properties of assumptions, involving no reference to their semantic properties. This supports the notion that logical implication - the keystone of zygonic theory - is applicable to music too.
    • Dempster1
  • 100
    • 10244272445 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Cambridge, MA
    • A comparable line of argument is advanced by Bob Snyder, Music and Memory (Cambridge, MA, 2000), 230: 'By causing musical events to "point to" other musical events, it connects those events in a relation of implication.... Musical linearity is like a metaphor of physical causation, and indeed linearity is an attempt to make musical events seem to cause each other. (Note that much of the linearity in verbal narrative structures is based on causation - events are connected to other events through earlier events causing the later events.)'
    • (2000) Music and Memory , pp. 230
    • Snyder, B.1
  • 101
    • 4444317492 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Narrative, Drama and Emotion in Instrumental Music
    • See also Fred Maus, 'Narrative, Drama and Emotion in Instrumental Music', Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism, 55 (1997), 293-303.
    • (1997) Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism , vol.55 , pp. 293-303
    • Maus, F.1
  • 103
    • 84990352312 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The Compositionality of Tonal Structures: A Generative Approach to the Notion of Musical Meaning
    • 143
    • Tim Horton, 'The Compositionality of Tonal Structures: A Generative Approach to the Notion of Musical Meaning', Musicae scientiae, 5 (2001), 131-56 (p. 143).
    • (2001) Musicae Scientiae , vol.5 , pp. 131-156
    • Horton, T.1
  • 106
    • 0003684441 scopus 로고
    • Cambridge, MA
    • Thus creating an effect in some ways comparable to that produced by the technique of photomontage when it is used to form the image of a human face from many smaller pictures of faces. The expressions (happy, sad, etc.) on these individual physiognomies need not correspond to those evinced by the single, larger face. Hence there could be a number of smaller, sad images coming together to make a happy whole. So it is with minor chords in a major context and vice versa: by focusing one's attention on the detail, a particular aesthetic response may come to the fore; by attending more globally, a different reaction may be engendered. In fact, it is likely there will be an interaction between the two: the effect of a major tonality will be coloured by the minor elements within it (and vice versa). In his Auditory Scene Analysis (Cambridge, MA, 1990)
    • (1990) Auditory Scene Analysis
  • 107
    • 33646451969 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Repetition in Music: Theoretical and Metatheoretical Perspectives
    • London
    • Ockelford, Repetition in Music: Theoretical and Metatheoretical Perspectives, RMA Monographs, 13 (London, 2005).
    • (2005) RMA Monographs , vol.13
    • Ockelford1
  • 109
    • 0001773107 scopus 로고
    • On Emotion, Mood, and Related Affective Constructs
    • ed. Paul Ekman and Richard Davidson, Oxford, p. 52
    • Richard Davidson, 'On Emotion, Mood, and Related Affective Constructs', The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions, ed. Paul Ekman and Richard Davidson (Oxford, 1994), 51-5 (p. 52).
    • (1994) The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions , pp. 51-55
    • Davidson, R.1
  • 114
    • 61049447253 scopus 로고
    • Oxford
    • However, this is a typical feature of Beethoven's late style - see Martin Cooper, Beethoven: The Last Decade 1817-1827 (Oxford, 1985), 425 - and the opening sonority foreshadows greater extremes of register later in the movement; see, for example, bars 25-8.
    • (1985) Beethoven: The Last Decade 1817-1827 , pp. 425
    • Cooper, M.1
  • 115
    • 84965728985 scopus 로고
    • Verbal and Exploratory Responses to Melodic Musical Intervals
    • See Timothy Maher and Daniel Berlyne, 'Verbal and Exploratory Responses to Melodic Musical Intervals', Psychology of Music, 10 (1982), 11-27.
    • (1982) Psychology of Music , vol.10 , pp. 11-27
    • Maher, T.1    Berlyne, D.2
  • 117
    • 79956488905 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Integration and Narrative Design in Beethoven's Piano Sonata in Amusic flat sign Major
    • Kinderman, 'Integration and Narrative Design in Beethoven's Piano Sonata in Amusic flat sign Major, Opus 110', 117.
    • Opus , vol.110 , pp. 117
    • Kinderman1


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.