메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 7, Issue 1, 2004, Pages 33-41

Two-year-olds' sensitivity to speakers' intent: An alternative account of Samuelson and Smith

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords

ARTICLE; BEHAVIOR; COMPARATIVE STUDY; FEMALE; HUMAN; LANGUAGE; LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT; LEARNING; LINGUISTICS; MALE; PHYSIOLOGY; PRESCHOOL CHILD; SPEECH PERCEPTION;

EID: 1142266483     PISSN: 1363755X     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00320.x     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (55)

References (31)
  • 1
    • 0030559475 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The role of discourse novelty in early word learning
    • Akhtar, N., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (1996). The role of discourse novelty in early word learning. Child Development, 67, 635-645.
    • (1996) Child Development , vol.67 , pp. 635-645
    • Akhtar, N.1    Carpenter, M.2    Tomasello, M.3
  • 2
    • 0012994419 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The social nature of words and word learning
    • R.M. Golinkoff & K. Hirsh-Pasek (Eds.). Oxford: Oxford University Press
    • Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (2000). The social nature of words and word learning. In R.M. Golinkoff & K. Hirsh-Pasek (Eds.), Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition (pp. 115-135). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    • (2000) Becoming a Word Learner: A Debate on Lexical Acquisition , pp. 115-135
    • Akhtar, N.1    Tomasello, M.2
  • 3
    • 21144470546 scopus 로고
    • Early referential understanding: Young children's ability to recognize referential acts for what they are
    • Baldwin, D.A. (1993). Early referential understanding: young children's ability to recognize referential acts for what they are. Developmental Psychology, 29, 832-843.
    • (1993) Developmental Psychology , vol.29 , pp. 832-843
    • Baldwin, D.A.1
  • 7
    • 0036513702 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Preschoolers are sensitive to speakers' knowledge when learning proper names
    • Birch, S.A.J., & Bloom, P. (2002). Preschoolers are sensitive to speakers' knowledge when learning proper names. Child Development, 73, 434-444.
    • (2002) Child Development , vol.73 , pp. 434-444
    • Birch, S.A.J.1    Bloom, P.2
  • 8
    • 0000950069 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Language acquisition in its developmental context
    • W. Damon (Series Ed.), D. Kuhn & R.S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.). New York: Wiley
    • Bloom, L. (1998). Language acquisition in its developmental context. In W. Damon (Series Ed.), D. Kuhn & R.S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2, Cognition, perception, and language (5th edn., pp. 309-370). New York: Wiley.
    • (1998) Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol.2, Cognition, Perception, and Language (5th Edn) , vol.2 , pp. 309-370
    • Bloom, L.1
  • 10
    • 0000705584 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Fourteen through eighteen month infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions
    • Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Fourteen through eighteen month infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 315-330.
    • (1998) Infant Behavior and Development , vol.21 , pp. 315-330
    • Carpenter, M.1    Akhtar, N.2    Tomasello, M.3
  • 11
    • 0035458735 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Children's avoidance of lexical overlap: A pragmatic account
    • Diesendruck, G., & Markson, L. (2001). Children's avoidance of lexical overlap: a pragmatic account. Developmental Psychology, 37, 630-641.
    • (2001) Developmental Psychology , vol.37 , pp. 630-641
    • Diesendruck, G.1    Markson, L.2
  • 12
    • 0032058609 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Shape and representational status in children's early naming
    • Gelman, S.A., & Ebeling, K.S. (1998). Shape and representational status in children's early naming. Cognition, 66, B35-B47.
    • (1998) Cognition , vol.66
    • Gelman, S.A.1    Ebeling, K.S.2
  • 13
    • 0029355476 scopus 로고
    • Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age
    • Gergely, G., Nadasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Biro, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56, 165-193.
    • (1995) Cognition , vol.56 , pp. 165-193
    • Gergely, G.1    Nadasdy, Z.2    Csibra, G.3    Biro, S.4
  • 14
    • 0027550323 scopus 로고
    • When is communication a 'meeting of minds'?
    • Golinkoff, R.M. (1993). When is communication a 'meeting of minds'? Journal of Child Language, 20, 199-207.
    • (1993) Journal of Child Language , vol.20 , pp. 199-207
    • Golinkoff, R.M.1
  • 15
    • 0032609085 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Lexical form class information guides word-to-object mappings in preschoolers
    • Hall, D.G., & Graham, S.A. (1999). Lexical form class information guides word-to-object mappings in preschoolers. Child Development, 70, 78-91.
    • (1999) Child Development , vol.70 , pp. 78-91
    • Hall, D.G.1    Graham, S.A.2
  • 18
    • 58149323600 scopus 로고
    • Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children
    • Meltzoff, A.N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 838-850.
    • (1995) Developmental Psychology , vol.31 , pp. 838-850
    • Meltzoff, A.N.1
  • 19
    • 0032615428 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Word learning in the context of referential and salience cues
    • Moore, C., Angelopoulos, M., & Bennett, P. (1999). Word learning in the context of referential and salience cues. Developmental Psychology, 35, 60-68.
    • (1999) Developmental Psychology , vol.35 , pp. 60-68
    • Moore, C.1    Angelopoulos, M.2    Bennett, P.3
  • 20
    • 0022704120 scopus 로고
    • The effect of experimenter's intention on the child's understanding of conservation
    • Moore, C., & Frye, D. (1986). The effect of experimenter's intention on the child's understanding of conservation. Cognition, 22, 283-298.
    • (1986) Cognition , vol.22 , pp. 283-298
    • Moore, C.1    Frye, D.2
  • 21
    • 0035347227 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Evidence for referential understanding in the emotions domain at 12 and 18 months
    • Moses, L.J., Baldwin, D.A., Rosicky, J.G., & Tidball, G. (2001). Evidence for referential understanding in the emotions domain at 12 and 18 months. Child Development, 72, 718-735.
    • (2001) Child Development , vol.72 , pp. 718-735
    • Moses, L.J.1    Baldwin, D.A.2    Rosicky, J.G.3    Tidball, G.4
  • 22
    • 0030542942 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Two-year-old children's sensitivity to a parent's knowledge state when making requests
    • O'Neill, D.K. (1996). Two-year-old children's sensitivity to a parent's knowledge state when making requests. Child Development, 67, 659-677.
    • (1996) Child Development , vol.67 , pp. 659-677
    • O'Neill, D.K.1
  • 23
    • 0031990889 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Memory and attention make smart word learning: An alternative account of Akhtar, Carpenter, and Tomasello
    • Samuelson, L.K., & Smith, L.B. (1998). Memory and attention make smart word learning: an alternative account of Akhtar, Carpenter, and Tomasello. Child Development, 69, 94-104.
    • (1998) Child Development , vol.69 , pp. 94-104
    • Samuelson, L.K.1    Smith, L.B.2
  • 24
    • 0030207401 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Naming in young children: A dumb attentional mechanism?
    • Smith, L.B., Jones, S.S., & Landau, B. (1996). Naming in young children: a dumb attentional mechanism? Cognition, 60, 143-171.
    • (1996) Cognition , vol.60 , pp. 143-171
    • Smith, L.B.1    Jones, S.S.2    Landau, B.3
  • 25
    • 0026110670 scopus 로고
    • Ontological categories guide young children's inductions of word meaning: Object terms and substance terms
    • Soja, N.N., Carey, S., & Spelke, E.S. (1991). Ontological categories guide young children's inductions of word meaning: object terms and substance terms. Cognition, 38, 179-211.
    • (1991) Cognition , vol.38 , pp. 179-211
    • Soja, N.N.1    Carey, S.2    Spelke, E.S.3
  • 26
    • 21844491489 scopus 로고
    • Learning words in non-ostensive contexts
    • Tomasello, M., & Barton, M. (1994). Learning words in non-ostensive contexts. Developmental Psychology, 30, 639-650.
    • (1994) Developmental Psychology , vol.30 , pp. 639-650
    • Tomasello, M.1    Barton, M.2
  • 27
    • 0030078684 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Eighteen-month-old children learn words in non-ostensive contexts
    • Tomasello, M., Strosberg, R., & Akhtar, N. (1996). Eighteen-month-old children learn words in non-ostensive contexts. Journal of Child Language, 23, 157-176.
    • (1996) Journal of Child Language , vol.23 , pp. 157-176
    • Tomasello, M.1    Strosberg, R.2    Akhtar, N.3
  • 28
    • 0001621758 scopus 로고
    • Linguistic biases and establishment of conceptual hierarchies: Evidence from preschool children
    • Waxman, S. (1990). Linguistic biases and establishment of conceptual hierarchies: evidence from preschool children. Cognitive Development, 5, 123-150.
    • (1990) Cognitive Development , vol.5 , pp. 123-150
    • Waxman, S.1
  • 29
    • 0000400188 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Infants' ability to distinguish between purposeful and nonpurposeful behaviors
    • Woodward, A.L. (1999). Infants' ability to distinguish between purposeful and nonpurposeful behaviors. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 145-160.
    • (1999) Infant Behavior and Development , vol.22 , pp. 145-160
    • Woodward, A.L.1


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.