-
1
-
-
0030559475
-
The role of discourse novelty in early word learning
-
Akhtar, N., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (1996). The role of discourse novelty in early word learning. Child Development, 67, 635-645.
-
(1996)
Child Development
, vol.67
, pp. 635-645
-
-
Akhtar, N.1
Carpenter, M.2
Tomasello, M.3
-
2
-
-
0012994419
-
The social nature of words and word learning
-
R.M. Golinkoff & K. Hirsh-Pasek (Eds.). Oxford: Oxford University Press
-
Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (2000). The social nature of words and word learning. In R.M. Golinkoff & K. Hirsh-Pasek (Eds.), Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition (pp. 115-135). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
-
(2000)
Becoming a Word Learner: A Debate on Lexical Acquisition
, pp. 115-135
-
-
Akhtar, N.1
Tomasello, M.2
-
3
-
-
21144470546
-
Early referential understanding: Young children's ability to recognize referential acts for what they are
-
Baldwin, D.A. (1993). Early referential understanding: young children's ability to recognize referential acts for what they are. Developmental Psychology, 29, 832-843.
-
(1993)
Developmental Psychology
, vol.29
, pp. 832-843
-
-
Baldwin, D.A.1
-
4
-
-
0035344923
-
Infants parse dynamic action
-
Baldwin, D.A., Baird, J.A., Saylor, M.M., & Clark, M.A. (2001). Infants parse dynamic action. Child Development, 72, 708-717.
-
(2001)
Child Development
, vol.72
, pp. 708-717
-
-
Baldwin, D.A.1
Baird, J.A.2
Saylor, M.M.3
Clark, M.A.4
-
5
-
-
0030347862
-
Infants' reliance on a social criterion for establishing word-object relations
-
Baldwin, D.A., Markman, E.M., Bill, B., Desjardins, R.N., Irwin, J., & Tidball, G. (1996). Infants' reliance on a social criterion for establishing word-object relations. Child Development, 67, 3135-3153.
-
(1996)
Child Development
, vol.67
, pp. 3135-3153
-
-
Baldwin, D.A.1
Markman, E.M.2
Bill, B.3
Desjardins, R.N.4
Irwin, J.5
Tidball, G.6
-
7
-
-
0036513702
-
Preschoolers are sensitive to speakers' knowledge when learning proper names
-
Birch, S.A.J., & Bloom, P. (2002). Preschoolers are sensitive to speakers' knowledge when learning proper names. Child Development, 73, 434-444.
-
(2002)
Child Development
, vol.73
, pp. 434-444
-
-
Birch, S.A.J.1
Bloom, P.2
-
8
-
-
0000950069
-
Language acquisition in its developmental context
-
W. Damon (Series Ed.), D. Kuhn & R.S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.). New York: Wiley
-
Bloom, L. (1998). Language acquisition in its developmental context. In W. Damon (Series Ed.), D. Kuhn & R.S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2, Cognition, perception, and language (5th edn., pp. 309-370). New York: Wiley.
-
(1998)
Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol.2, Cognition, Perception, and Language (5th Edn)
, vol.2
, pp. 309-370
-
-
Bloom, L.1
-
10
-
-
0000705584
-
Fourteen through eighteen month infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions
-
Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Fourteen through eighteen month infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 315-330.
-
(1998)
Infant Behavior and Development
, vol.21
, pp. 315-330
-
-
Carpenter, M.1
Akhtar, N.2
Tomasello, M.3
-
11
-
-
0035458735
-
Children's avoidance of lexical overlap: A pragmatic account
-
Diesendruck, G., & Markson, L. (2001). Children's avoidance of lexical overlap: a pragmatic account. Developmental Psychology, 37, 630-641.
-
(2001)
Developmental Psychology
, vol.37
, pp. 630-641
-
-
Diesendruck, G.1
Markson, L.2
-
12
-
-
0032058609
-
Shape and representational status in children's early naming
-
Gelman, S.A., & Ebeling, K.S. (1998). Shape and representational status in children's early naming. Cognition, 66, B35-B47.
-
(1998)
Cognition
, vol.66
-
-
Gelman, S.A.1
Ebeling, K.S.2
-
13
-
-
0029355476
-
Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age
-
Gergely, G., Nadasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Biro, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56, 165-193.
-
(1995)
Cognition
, vol.56
, pp. 165-193
-
-
Gergely, G.1
Nadasdy, Z.2
Csibra, G.3
Biro, S.4
-
14
-
-
0027550323
-
When is communication a 'meeting of minds'?
-
Golinkoff, R.M. (1993). When is communication a 'meeting of minds'? Journal of Child Language, 20, 199-207.
-
(1993)
Journal of Child Language
, vol.20
, pp. 199-207
-
-
Golinkoff, R.M.1
-
15
-
-
0032609085
-
Lexical form class information guides word-to-object mappings in preschoolers
-
Hall, D.G., & Graham, S.A. (1999). Lexical form class information guides word-to-object mappings in preschoolers. Child Development, 70, 78-91.
-
(1999)
Child Development
, vol.70
, pp. 78-91
-
-
Hall, D.G.1
Graham, S.A.2
-
16
-
-
0034456014
-
Breaking the language barrier: An Emergentist Coalition Model for the origins of word learning
-
Serial no. 262
-
Hollich, G.J., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R.M. (2000). Breaking the language barrier: an Emergentist Coalition Model for the origins of word learning. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 65 (3), Serial no. 262.
-
(2000)
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development
, vol.65
, Issue.3
-
-
Hollich, G.J.1
Hirsh-Pasek, K.2
Golinkoff, R.M.3
-
18
-
-
58149323600
-
Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children
-
Meltzoff, A.N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 838-850.
-
(1995)
Developmental Psychology
, vol.31
, pp. 838-850
-
-
Meltzoff, A.N.1
-
19
-
-
0032615428
-
Word learning in the context of referential and salience cues
-
Moore, C., Angelopoulos, M., & Bennett, P. (1999). Word learning in the context of referential and salience cues. Developmental Psychology, 35, 60-68.
-
(1999)
Developmental Psychology
, vol.35
, pp. 60-68
-
-
Moore, C.1
Angelopoulos, M.2
Bennett, P.3
-
20
-
-
0022704120
-
The effect of experimenter's intention on the child's understanding of conservation
-
Moore, C., & Frye, D. (1986). The effect of experimenter's intention on the child's understanding of conservation. Cognition, 22, 283-298.
-
(1986)
Cognition
, vol.22
, pp. 283-298
-
-
Moore, C.1
Frye, D.2
-
21
-
-
0035347227
-
Evidence for referential understanding in the emotions domain at 12 and 18 months
-
Moses, L.J., Baldwin, D.A., Rosicky, J.G., & Tidball, G. (2001). Evidence for referential understanding in the emotions domain at 12 and 18 months. Child Development, 72, 718-735.
-
(2001)
Child Development
, vol.72
, pp. 718-735
-
-
Moses, L.J.1
Baldwin, D.A.2
Rosicky, J.G.3
Tidball, G.4
-
22
-
-
0030542942
-
Two-year-old children's sensitivity to a parent's knowledge state when making requests
-
O'Neill, D.K. (1996). Two-year-old children's sensitivity to a parent's knowledge state when making requests. Child Development, 67, 659-677.
-
(1996)
Child Development
, vol.67
, pp. 659-677
-
-
O'Neill, D.K.1
-
23
-
-
0031990889
-
Memory and attention make smart word learning: An alternative account of Akhtar, Carpenter, and Tomasello
-
Samuelson, L.K., & Smith, L.B. (1998). Memory and attention make smart word learning: an alternative account of Akhtar, Carpenter, and Tomasello. Child Development, 69, 94-104.
-
(1998)
Child Development
, vol.69
, pp. 94-104
-
-
Samuelson, L.K.1
Smith, L.B.2
-
24
-
-
0030207401
-
Naming in young children: A dumb attentional mechanism?
-
Smith, L.B., Jones, S.S., & Landau, B. (1996). Naming in young children: a dumb attentional mechanism? Cognition, 60, 143-171.
-
(1996)
Cognition
, vol.60
, pp. 143-171
-
-
Smith, L.B.1
Jones, S.S.2
Landau, B.3
-
25
-
-
0026110670
-
Ontological categories guide young children's inductions of word meaning: Object terms and substance terms
-
Soja, N.N., Carey, S., & Spelke, E.S. (1991). Ontological categories guide young children's inductions of word meaning: object terms and substance terms. Cognition, 38, 179-211.
-
(1991)
Cognition
, vol.38
, pp. 179-211
-
-
Soja, N.N.1
Carey, S.2
Spelke, E.S.3
-
26
-
-
21844491489
-
Learning words in non-ostensive contexts
-
Tomasello, M., & Barton, M. (1994). Learning words in non-ostensive contexts. Developmental Psychology, 30, 639-650.
-
(1994)
Developmental Psychology
, vol.30
, pp. 639-650
-
-
Tomasello, M.1
Barton, M.2
-
27
-
-
0030078684
-
Eighteen-month-old children learn words in non-ostensive contexts
-
Tomasello, M., Strosberg, R., & Akhtar, N. (1996). Eighteen-month-old children learn words in non-ostensive contexts. Journal of Child Language, 23, 157-176.
-
(1996)
Journal of Child Language
, vol.23
, pp. 157-176
-
-
Tomasello, M.1
Strosberg, R.2
Akhtar, N.3
-
28
-
-
0001621758
-
Linguistic biases and establishment of conceptual hierarchies: Evidence from preschool children
-
Waxman, S. (1990). Linguistic biases and establishment of conceptual hierarchies: evidence from preschool children. Cognitive Development, 5, 123-150.
-
(1990)
Cognitive Development
, vol.5
, pp. 123-150
-
-
Waxman, S.1
-
29
-
-
0000400188
-
Infants' ability to distinguish between purposeful and nonpurposeful behaviors
-
Woodward, A.L. (1999). Infants' ability to distinguish between purposeful and nonpurposeful behaviors. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 145-160.
-
(1999)
Infant Behavior and Development
, vol.22
, pp. 145-160
-
-
Woodward, A.L.1
-
30
-
-
0000963322
-
Early word learning
-
W. Damon (Series Ed.), D. Kuhn & R.S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.). New York: Wiley
-
Woodward, A.L., & Markman, E.M. (1998). Early word learning. In W. Damon (Series Ed.), D. Kuhn & R.S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2, Cognition, perception, and language (5th edn., pp. 371-420). New York: Wiley.
-
(1998)
Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol. 2, Cognition, Perception, and Language (5th Edn)
, vol.2
, pp. 371-420
-
-
Woodward, A.L.1
Markman, E.M.2
-
31
-
-
0003785245
-
-
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
-
Zelazo, P.D., Astington, J.W, & Olson, D.R. (Eds.) (1999). Developing theories of intention. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
-
(1999)
Developing Theories of Intention
-
-
Zelazo, P.D.1
Astington, J.W.2
Olson, D.R.3
|