메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 18, Issue 4, 2004, Pages 76-91

The intuitive executive: Understanding and applying 'gut feel' in decision-making

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords


EID: 11044239138     PISSN: 10795545     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.5465/AME.2004.15268692     Document Type: Review
Times cited : (343)

References (103)
  • 1
    • 11044226571 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Robert Salmon, former vice-president of L'Oreal (http://www. refresher.com/lsignals); Lindsey Owen-Jones, chairman and chief executive officer of L'Oreal, in Business Week Online. 28 June 1999.
    • Salmon, R.1    Owen-Jones, L.2
  • 2
    • 11044221185 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • 28 June
    • Robert Salmon, former vice-president of L'Oreal (http://www. refresher.com/lsignals); Lindsey Owen-Jones, chairman and chief executive officer of L'Oreal, in Business Week Online. 28 June 1999.
    • (1999) Business Week Online
  • 3
    • 11044235446 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Lanham: Scarecrow
    • Davis, S.H., & Davis, P.B. 2003. The intuitive dimensions of administrative decision-making. Lanham: Scarecrow; Eisen-hardt, K.M. 1999. Strategy as decision-making. Sloan Management Review, Spring: 65-72. For a summary of the dominant paradigms, see Das, T.K., & Teng, B-S. 1999. Cognitive biases and strategic decision processes: An integrative perspective. Journal of Management Studies, 36(6): 757-778. The question remains as to where an intuitive decision mode fits within the spectrum of approaches identified by Das and Teng, op. cit., 759.
    • (2003) The Intuitive Dimensions of Administrative Decision-making
    • Davis, S.H.1    Davis, P.B.2
  • 4
    • 0002466471 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Strategy as decision-making
    • Spring
    • Davis, S.H., & Davis, P.B. 2003. The intuitive dimensions of administrative decision-making. Lanham: Scarecrow; Eisen-hardt, K.M. 1999. Strategy as decision-making. Sloan Management Review, Spring: 65-72. For a summary of the dominant paradigms, see Das, T.K., & Teng, B-S. 1999. Cognitive biases and strategic decision processes: An integrative perspective. Journal of Management Studies, 36(6): 757-778. The question remains as to where an intuitive decision mode fits within the spectrum of approaches identified by Das and Teng, op. cit., 759.
    • (1999) Sloan Management Review , pp. 65-72
    • Eisen-Hardt, K.M.1
  • 5
    • 0033228946 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Cognitive biases and strategic decision processes: An integrative perspective
    • Davis, S.H., & Davis, P.B. 2003. The intuitive dimensions of administrative decision-making. Lanham: Scarecrow; Eisen-hardt, K.M. 1999. Strategy as decision-making. Sloan Management Review, Spring: 65-72. For a summary of the dominant paradigms, see Das, T.K., & Teng, B-S. 1999. Cognitive biases and strategic decision processes: An integrative perspective. Journal of Management Studies, 36(6): 757-778. The question remains as to where an intuitive decision mode fits within the spectrum of approaches identified by Das and Teng, op. cit., 759.
    • (1999) Journal of Management Studies , vol.36 , Issue.6 , pp. 757-778
    • Das, T.K.1    Teng, B.-S.2
  • 6
  • 7
    • 0002953738 scopus 로고
    • The information environments of organisations
    • P.M. Javlin, et al. (Eds.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage
    • Huber, G.P., & Daft, R.L. 1987. The information environments of organisations. In P.M. Javlin, et al. (Eds.). Handbook of organizational communication. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
    • (1987) Handbook of Organizational Communication
    • Huber, G.P.1    Daft, R.L.2
  • 8
    • 11044221860 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Buckingham: Open University Press discusses the importance of rationality and intention
    • Hodgkinson, G.P., & Sparrow, P. 2002. The competent organization. Buckingham: Open University Press discusses the importance of rationality and intention.
    • (2002) The Competent Organization
    • Hodgkinson, G.P.1    Sparrow, P.2
  • 9
    • 11044234847 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Emotion and learning
    • S.R. Clegg, C. Hardy, & W.R. Nord (Eds). London: Sage
    • Fineman, S. 1996: Emotion and learning. In S.R. Clegg, C. Hardy, & W.R. Nord (Eds). Handbook of organization studies. London: Sage: 545.
    • (1996) Handbook of Organization Studies , pp. 545
    • Fineman, S.1
  • 10
    • 0011400739 scopus 로고
    • Effects of environmental uncertainty on information and decisions in banks
    • Leblebici, H., & Salancik, G.R. 1981. Effects of environmental uncertainty on information and decisions in banks. Administrative Science Quarterly, 26(4): 578-596; Narayanan, V.K., & Fahey, L. 1982. The micro-politics of strategy formulation. Academy of Management Review, 7(1): 25-34; Bourgeois, L.J., & Eisenhardt, K.M. 1988. Strategic decision processes in high velocity environments: Four cases in the micro-computer industry. Management Science, 34(7): 816-835.
    • (1981) Administrative Science Quarterly , vol.26 , Issue.4 , pp. 578-596
    • Leblebici, H.1    Salancik, G.R.2
  • 11
    • 0002495178 scopus 로고
    • The micro-politics of strategy formulation
    • Leblebici, H., & Salancik, G.R. 1981. Effects of environmental uncertainty on information and decisions in banks. Administrative Science Quarterly, 26(4): 578-596; Narayanan, V.K., & Fahey, L. 1982. The micro-politics of strategy formulation. Academy of Management Review, 7(1): 25-34; Bourgeois, L.J., & Eisenhardt, K.M. 1988. Strategic decision processes in high velocity environments: Four cases in the micro-computer industry. Management Science, 34(7): 816-835.
    • (1982) Academy of Management Review , vol.7 , Issue.1 , pp. 25-34
    • Narayanan, V.K.1    Fahey, L.2
  • 12
    • 0000634867 scopus 로고
    • Strategic decision processes in high velocity environments: Four cases in the micro-computer industry
    • Leblebici, H., & Salancik, G.R. 1981. Effects of environmental uncertainty on information and decisions in banks. Administrative Science Quarterly, 26(4): 578-596; Narayanan, V.K., & Fahey, L. 1982. The micro-politics of strategy formulation. Academy of Management Review, 7(1): 25-34; Bourgeois, L.J., & Eisenhardt, K.M. 1988. Strategic decision processes in high velocity environments: Four cases in the micro-computer industry. Management Science, 34(7): 816-835.
    • (1988) Management Science , vol.34 , Issue.7 , pp. 816-835
    • Bourgeois, L.J.1    Eisenhardt, K.M.2
  • 13
    • 0030304390 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Does decision-making matter: A study of strategic decision-making effectiveness
    • The relationship between procedural rationality and decision-making effectiveness (DME) was positive and statistically significant and when controlling for environmental instability (r = 0.35, p < 0.01; t = 2.30, p < 0.05). See: Dean, J.W., & Sharfman, M.P. 1996. Does decision-making matter: A study of strategic decision-making effectiveness. Academy of Management Journal, 39(2): 368-396.
    • (1996) Academy of Management Journal , vol.39 , Issue.2 , pp. 368-396
    • Dean, J.W.1    Sharfman, M.P.2
  • 14
    • 85025221771 scopus 로고
    • Procedural rationality in the strategic decision-making process
    • The correlation between rationality and uncertainty was negative and statistically significant (r = -0.38, p < 0.01). See: Dean, J.W., & Sharfman, M.P. 1993. Procedural rationality in the strategic decision-making process. Journal of Management Studies, 30(4): 587-620.
    • (1993) Journal of Management Studies , vol.30 , Issue.4 , pp. 587-620
    • Dean, J.W.1    Sharfman, M.P.2
  • 15
    • 0001229241 scopus 로고
    • Organizational information requirements, media richness and structural design
    • Daft, R.L., & Lengel, H.R. 1986. Organizational information requirements, media richness and structural design. Management Science, 32(5): 554-571. The research findings in support of these contrasting viewpoints are themselves somewhat complex. For example, Dean & Sharfman (1993) found that decision-makers engaged in less rational processes of strategic decision-making when faced with highly uncertain problems; whereas when there was little competitive threat, little perceived external control, and the issues being faced were well understood, executives used rational procedures. Fredrickson & Mitchell (1984) reported a negative relationship between rationality and organizational performance and used it as introductory evidence that rational models are not appropriate for some environments. Further research by Fredrickson (1984) established a positive relationship between rationality and performance in stable environments. See Fredrickson, J.W., & Mitchell, T.R. 1984. Strategic decision processes: Comprehensiveness and performance in an industry with an unstable environment. Academy of Management Journal, 27(2): 399-423; Fredrickson, J.W. 1984. The comprehensiveness of strategic decision processes: Extension, observations, future directions. Academy of Management Journal, 27(3): 445-466; Priem, R.L., Rasheed, A., & Kotulik, A. 1995. Rationality in strategic decision processes, environmental dynamism and firm performance. Journal of Management, 21 (5): 913-929.
    • (1986) Management Science , vol.32 , Issue.5 , pp. 554-571
    • Daft, R.L.1    Lengel, H.R.2
  • 16
    • 0000866269 scopus 로고
    • Strategic decision processes: Comprehensiveness and performance in an industry with an unstable environment
    • Daft, R.L., & Lengel, H.R. 1986. Organizational information requirements, media richness and structural design. Management Science, 32(5): 554-571. The research findings in support of these contrasting viewpoints are themselves somewhat complex. For example, Dean & Sharfman (1993) found that decision-makers engaged in less rational processes of strategic decision-making when faced with highly uncertain problems; whereas when there was little competitive threat, little perceived external control, and the issues being faced were well understood, executives used rational procedures. Fredrickson & Mitchell (1984) reported a negative relationship between rationality and organizational performance and used it as introductory evidence that rational models are not appropriate for some environments. Further research by Fredrickson (1984) established a positive relationship between rationality and performance in stable environments. See Fredrickson, J.W., & Mitchell, T.R. 1984. Strategic decision processes: Comprehensiveness and performance in an industry with an unstable environment. Academy of Management Journal, 27(2): 399-423; Fredrickson, J.W. 1984. The comprehensiveness of strategic decision processes: Extension, observations, future directions. Academy of Management Journal, 27(3): 445-466; Priem, R.L., Rasheed, A., & Kotulik, A. 1995. Rationality in strategic decision processes, environmental dynamism and firm performance. Journal of Management, 21 (5): 913-929.
    • (1984) Academy of Management Journal , vol.27 , Issue.2 , pp. 399-423
    • Fredrickson, J.W.1    Mitchell, T.R.2
  • 17
    • 0000341186 scopus 로고
    • The comprehensiveness of strategic decision processes: Extension, observations, future directions
    • Daft, R.L., & Lengel, H.R. 1986. Organizational information requirements, media richness and structural design. Management Science, 32(5): 554-571. The research findings in support of these contrasting viewpoints are themselves somewhat complex. For example, Dean & Sharfman (1993) found that decision-makers engaged in less rational processes of strategic decision-making when faced with highly uncertain problems; whereas when there was little competitive threat, little perceived external control, and the issues being faced were well understood, executives used rational procedures. Fredrickson & Mitchell (1984) reported a negative relationship between rationality and organizational performance and used it as introductory evidence that rational models are not appropriate for some environments. Further research by Fredrickson (1984) established a positive relationship between rationality and performance in stable environments. See Fredrickson, J.W., & Mitchell, T.R. 1984. Strategic decision processes: Comprehensiveness and performance in an industry with an unstable environment. Academy of Management Journal, 27(2): 399-423; Fredrickson, J.W. 1984. The comprehensiveness of strategic decision processes: Extension, observations, future directions. Academy of Management Journal, 27(3): 445-466; Priem, R.L., Rasheed, A., & Kotulik, A. 1995. Rationality in strategic decision processes, environmental dynamism and firm performance. Journal of Management, 21 (5): 913-929.
    • (1984) Academy of Management Journal , vol.27 , Issue.3 , pp. 445-466
    • Fredrickson, J.W.1
  • 18
    • 34249096931 scopus 로고
    • Rationality in strategic decision processes, environmental dynamism and firm performance
    • Daft, R.L., & Lengel, H.R. 1986. Organizational information requirements, media richness and structural design. Management Science, 32(5): 554-571. The research findings in support of these contrasting viewpoints are themselves somewhat complex. For example, Dean & Sharfman (1993) found that decision-makers engaged in less rational processes of strategic decision-making when faced with highly uncertain problems; whereas when there was little competitive threat, little perceived external control, and the issues being faced were well understood, executives used rational procedures. Fredrickson & Mitchell (1984) reported a negative relationship between rationality and organizational performance and used it as introductory evidence that rational models are not appropriate for some environments. Further research by Fredrickson (1984) established a positive relationship between rationality and performance in stable environments. See Fredrickson, J.W., & Mitchell, T.R. 1984. Strategic decision processes: Comprehensiveness and performance in an industry with an unstable environment. Academy of Management Journal, 27(2): 399-423; Fredrickson, J.W. 1984. The comprehensiveness of strategic decision processes: Extension, observations, future directions. Academy of Management Journal, 27(3): 445-466; Priem, R.L., Rasheed, A., & Kotulik, A. 1995. Rationality in strategic decision processes, environmental dynamism and firm performance. Journal of Management, 21 (5): 913-929.
    • (1995) Journal of Management , vol.21 , Issue.5 , pp. 913-929
    • Priem, R.L.1    Rasheed, A.2    Kotulik, A.3
  • 21
    • 0000974763 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The unbearable automaticity of being
    • Bargh, J.A., & Chartrand, T.L. 1999. The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54(7): 462-479.
    • (1999) American Psychologist , vol.54 , Issue.7 , pp. 462-479
    • Bargh, J.A.1    Chartrand, T.L.2
  • 23
    • 0011059493 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Taking the mystery out of intuitive decision-making
    • Burke, L.A., & Miller, M.K. 1999. Taking the mystery out of intuitive decision-making. The Academy of Management Executive. 13(4): 91-99.
    • (1999) The Academy of Management Executive , vol.13 , Issue.4 , pp. 91-99
    • Burke, L.A.1    Miller, M.K.2
  • 24
    • 0000214042 scopus 로고
    • How senior managers think
    • November/December
    • Isenberg, D.J. 1984. How senior managers think. Harvard Business Review, November/December: 81-90.
    • (1984) Harvard Business Review , pp. 81-90
    • Isenberg, D.J.1
  • 25
    • 11044230500 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Trust you gut
    • July
    • Overell, S. 2001. Trust you gut. Director, July: 28.
    • (2001) Director , pp. 28
    • Overell, S.1
  • 27
    • 0023109850 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • How expert nurses use intuition
    • January
    • Benner, P., & Tanner, C. 1987. How expert nurses use intuition. American Journal of Nursing, January: 23-31; Davis & Davis, op. cit.
    • (1987) American Journal of Nursing , pp. 23-31
    • Benner, P.1    Tanner, C.2
  • 29
    • 0029691631 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The Cognitive Style Index: A measure of intuition-analysis for organizational research
    • See Allinson, C.W., & Hayes, J. 1996. The Cognitive Style Index: A measure of intuition-analysis for organizational research. Journal of Management Studies, 33(1): 19-135; Allinson, C.W., Chell, E., & Hayes, J. 2000. Intuition and entrepreneurial behavior. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 9: 31-43; Sadler-Smith, E. 2004. Cognitive style and the management of small and medium sized enterprises. Organization Studies, 25: 155- 181; Clarke, I., & Mackaness, W. 2001. Management intuition: An interpretative account of structure and content of decision schemas using cognitive maps. Journal of Management Studies, 38(2): 147-172.
    • (1996) Journal of Management Studies , vol.33 , Issue.1 , pp. 19-135
    • Allinson, C.W.1    Hayes, J.2
  • 30
    • 0007150255 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Intuition and entrepreneurial behavior
    • See Allinson, C.W., & Hayes, J. 1996. The Cognitive Style Index: A measure of intuition-analysis for organizational research. Journal of Management Studies, 33(1): 19-135; Allinson, C.W., Chell, E., & Hayes, J. 2000. Intuition and entrepreneurial behavior. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 9: 31-43; Sadler-Smith, E. 2004. Cognitive style and the management of small and medium sized enterprises. Organization Studies, 25: 155- 181; Clarke, I., & Mackaness, W. 2001. Management intuition: An interpretative account of structure and content of decision schemas using cognitive maps. Journal of Management Studies, 38(2): 147-172.
    • (2000) European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology , vol.9 , pp. 31-43
    • Allinson, C.W.1    Chell, E.2    Hayes, J.3
  • 31
    • 1442280726 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Cognitive style and the management of small and medium sized enterprises
    • See Allinson, C.W., & Hayes, J. 1996. The Cognitive Style Index: A measure of intuition-analysis for organizational research. Journal of Management Studies, 33(1): 19-135; Allinson, C.W., Chell, E., & Hayes, J. 2000. Intuition and entrepreneurial behavior. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 9: 31-43; Sadler-Smith, E. 2004. Cognitive style and the management of small and medium sized enterprises. Organization Studies, 25: 155- 181; Clarke, I., & Mackaness, W. 2001. Management intuition: An interpretative account of structure and content of decision schemas using cognitive maps. Journal of Management Studies, 38(2): 147-172.
    • (2004) Organization Studies , vol.25 , pp. 155-181
    • Sadler-Smith, E.1
  • 32
    • 0035285417 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Management intuition: An interpretative account of structure and content of decision schemas using cognitive maps
    • See Allinson, C.W., & Hayes, J. 1996. The Cognitive Style Index: A measure of intuition-analysis for organizational research. Journal of Management Studies, 33(1): 19-135; Allinson, C.W., Chell, E., & Hayes, J. 2000. Intuition and entrepreneurial behavior. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 9: 31-43; Sadler-Smith, E. 2004. Cognitive style and the management of small and medium sized enterprises. Organization Studies, 25: 155- 181; Clarke, I., & Mackaness, W. 2001. Management intuition: An interpretative account of structure and content of decision schemas using cognitive maps. Journal of Management Studies, 38(2): 147-172.
    • (2001) Journal of Management Studies , vol.38 , Issue.2 , pp. 147-172
    • Clarke, I.1    Mackaness, W.2
  • 33
    • 0030529161 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Using the MBTI to study managers: A literature review and research agenda
    • Gardner, W.L., & Martinko, M.J. 1996. Using the MBTI to study managers: A literature review and research agenda. Journal of Management, 22(1): 45-83.
    • (1996) Journal of Management , vol.22 , Issue.1 , pp. 45-83
    • Gardner, W.L.1    Martinko, M.J.2
  • 35
    • 0000580380 scopus 로고
    • Individuals and information overload in organizations: Is more necessarily better?
    • O'Reilly, C.A. 1980. Individuals and information overload in organizations: Is more necessarily better? Academy of Management Journal, 23(4): 684-696.
    • (1980) Academy of Management Journal , vol.23 , Issue.4 , pp. 684-696
    • O'Reilly, C.A.1
  • 36
    • 7544237747 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Strategy and cognition: Understanding the role of management knowledge structures, organizational memory and information overload
    • Sparrow, P.R. 1999. Strategy and cognition: Understanding the role of management knowledge structures, organizational memory and information overload. Creativity and Innovation Management, 8(2): 140-148.
    • (1999) Creativity and Innovation Management , vol.8 , Issue.2 , pp. 140-148
    • Sparrow, P.R.1
  • 37
    • 0035434551 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Intuition: An important tool in the practice of nursing
    • McCutcheon, H.H.I., & Pincombe, J. 2001. Intuition: An important tool in the practice of nursing. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 35: 342-348; Atkinson, L. 2000. Trusting your own judgment. In Atkinson, T., & Claxton, G. (Eds). 2000. The intuitive practitioner. Buckingham: Open University Press: 53-66; Goldberg, P. 1989. The many faces of intuition. In W.H. Agor (Ed.), Intuition in organisations: Leading and managing productively. Newbury Park, CA.: Sage Publications.
    • (2001) Journal of Advanced Nursing , vol.35 , pp. 342-348
    • McCutcheon, H.H.I.1    Pincombe, J.2
  • 38
    • 0035434551 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Trusting your own judgment
    • Atkinson, T., & Claxton, G. (Eds). 2000. Buckingham: Open University Press
    • McCutcheon, H.H.I., & Pincombe, J. 2001. Intuition: An important tool in the practice of nursing. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 35: 342-348; Atkinson, L. 2000. Trusting your own judgment. In Atkinson, T., & Claxton, G. (Eds). 2000. The intuitive practitioner. Buckingham: Open University Press: 53-66; Goldberg, P. 1989. The many faces of intuition. In W.H. Agor (Ed.), Intuition in organisations: Leading and managing productively. Newbury Park, CA.: Sage Publications.
    • (2000) The Intuitive Practitioner , pp. 53-66
    • Atkinson, L.1
  • 39
    • 0035434551 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The many faces of intuition
    • W.H. Agor (Ed.). Newbury Park, CA.: Sage Publications
    • McCutcheon, H.H.I., & Pincombe, J. 2001. Intuition: An important tool in the practice of nursing. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 35: 342-348; Atkinson, L. 2000. Trusting your own judgment. In Atkinson, T., & Claxton, G. (Eds). 2000. The intuitive practitioner. Buckingham: Open University Press: 53-66; Goldberg, P. 1989. The many faces of intuition. In W.H. Agor (Ed.), Intuition in organisations: Leading and managing productively. Newbury Park, CA.: Sage Publications.
    • (1989) Intuition in Organisations: Leading and Managing Productively
    • Goldberg, P.1
  • 40
    • 11044225783 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Subjectivity and intuition in the scientific method
    • R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds), London: Routledge
    • The questions are useful only as a starting point for self-reflection and should not be used a basis for typecasting. For example, Einstein is seen by many as the archetypal rational mind, but he is widely reported as recognizing intuition as the 'sower of all true science.' See Dunne, B.J. 1997. Subjectivity and intuition in the scientific method. In R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds), Intuition: The inside story: Interdisciplinary perspectives. London: Routledge. For valid and reliable self-report measures of intuition and their psychometric evaluation, the following research may be consulted: Allinson & Hayes, 1996, op. cit.; Epstein, S., et al. 1996. Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-reasoning thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71: 390-405; Hodgkinson, G.P., & Sadler-Smith, E. 2003. Complex or unitary? A critique and empirical reassessment of the Allinson-Hayes Cognitive Style Index. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 76: 243-268; Scott, S.G., & Bruce, R.A. 1994. Determinants of innovative behavior: A path model of individual innovation in the workplace. Academy of Management Journal, 37: 580-607.
    • (1997) Intuition: The Inside Story: Interdisciplinary Perspectives
    • Dunne, B.J.1
  • 41
    • 11044224182 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • London: Routledge
    • The questions are useful only as a starting point for self-reflection and should not be used a basis for typecasting. For example, Einstein is seen by many as the archetypal rational mind, but he is widely reported as recognizing intuition as the 'sower of all true science.' See Dunne, B.J. 1997. Subjectivity and intuition in the scientific method. In R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds), Intuition: The inside story: Interdisciplinary perspectives. London: Routledge. For valid and reliable self-report measures of intuition and their psychometric evaluation, the following research may be consulted: Allinson & Hayes, 1996, op. cit.; Epstein, S., et al. 1996. Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-reasoning thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71: 390-405; Hodgkinson, G.P., & Sadler-Smith, E. 2003. Complex or unitary? A critique and empirical reassessment of the Allinson-Hayes Cognitive Style Index. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 76: 243-268; Scott, S.G., & Bruce, R.A. 1994. Determinants of innovative behavior: A path model of individual innovation in the workplace. Academy of Management Journal, 37: 580-607.
    • (1996) Intuition: The Inside Story: Interdisciplinary Perspectives
    • Allinson1    Hayes2
  • 42
    • 0030209604 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-reasoning thinking styles
    • The questions are useful only as a starting point for self-reflection and should not be used a basis for typecasting. For example, Einstein is seen by many as the archetypal rational mind, but he is widely reported as recognizing intuition as the 'sower of all true science.' See Dunne, B.J. 1997. Subjectivity and intuition in the scientific method. In R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds), Intuition: The inside story: Interdisciplinary perspectives. London: Routledge. For valid and reliable self-report measures of intuition and their psychometric evaluation, the following research may be consulted: Allinson & Hayes, 1996, op. cit.; Epstein, S., et al. 1996. Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-reasoning thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71: 390-405; Hodgkinson, G.P., & Sadler-Smith, E. 2003. Complex or unitary? A critique and empirical reassessment of the Allinson-Hayes Cognitive Style Index. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 76: 243-268; Scott, S.G., & Bruce, R.A. 1994. Determinants of innovative behavior: A path model of individual innovation in the workplace. Academy of Management Journal, 37: 580-607.
    • (1996) Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , vol.71 , pp. 390-405
    • Epstein, S.1
  • 43
    • 0038772264 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Complex or unitary? A critique and empirical reassessment of the Allinson-Hayes Cognitive Style Index
    • The questions are useful only as a starting point for self-reflection and should not be used a basis for typecasting. For example, Einstein is seen by many as the archetypal rational mind, but he is widely reported as recognizing intuition as the 'sower of all true science.' See Dunne, B.J. 1997. Subjectivity and intuition in the scientific method. In R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds), Intuition: The inside story: Interdisciplinary perspectives. London: Routledge. For valid and reliable self-report measures of intuition and their psychometric evaluation, the following research may be consulted: Allinson & Hayes, 1996, op. cit.; Epstein, S., et al. 1996. Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-reasoning thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71: 390-405; Hodgkinson, G.P., & Sadler-Smith, E. 2003. Complex or unitary? A critique and empirical reassessment of the Allinson-Hayes Cognitive Style Index. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 76: 243-268; Scott, S.G., & Bruce, R.A. 1994. Determinants of innovative behavior: A path model of individual innovation in the workplace. Academy of Management Journal, 37: 580-607.
    • (2003) Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology , vol.76 , pp. 243-268
    • Hodgkinson, G.P.1    Sadler-Smith, E.2
  • 44
    • 18944400441 scopus 로고
    • Determinants of innovative behavior: A path model of individual innovation in the workplace
    • The questions are useful only as a starting point for self-reflection and should not be used a basis for typecasting. For example, Einstein is seen by many as the archetypal rational mind, but he is widely reported as recognizing intuition as the 'sower of all true science.' See Dunne, B.J. 1997. Subjectivity and intuition in the scientific method. In R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds), Intuition: The inside story: Interdisciplinary perspectives. London: Routledge. For valid and reliable self-report measures of intuition and their psychometric evaluation, the following research may be consulted: Allinson & Hayes, 1996, op. cit.; Epstein, S., et al. 1996. Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-reasoning thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71: 390-405; Hodgkinson, G.P., & Sadler-Smith, E. 2003. Complex or unitary? A critique and empirical reassessment of the Allinson-Hayes Cognitive Style Index. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 76: 243-268; Scott, S.G., & Bruce, R.A. 1994. Determinants of innovative behavior: A path model of individual innovation in the workplace. Academy of Management Journal, 37: 580-607.
    • (1994) Academy of Management Journal , vol.37 , pp. 580-607
    • Scott, S.G.1    Bruce, R.A.2
  • 45
    • 0020109716 scopus 로고
    • On the study of statistical intuitions
    • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. 1982. On the study of statistical intuitions. Cognition, 11: 123-141.
    • (1982) Cognition , vol.11 , pp. 123-141
    • Kahneman, D.1    Tversky, A.2
  • 46
    • 0242674910 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The anatomy of intuition
    • Atkinson, T., & Claxton, G. (Eds). Buckingham: Open University Press
    • Claxton, G. 2001. The anatomy of intuition. In Atkinson, T., & Claxton, G. (Eds). The intuitive practitioner. Buckingham: Open University Press: 32-52.
    • (2001) The Intuitive Practitioner , pp. 32-52
    • Claxton, G.1
  • 47
    • 0004143473 scopus 로고
    • New York: Harcourt Brace
    • This is the way in which Wallas (1926) described intuition in the context of creativity and insight in his incubation-illumination theory of creativity. See Wallas, G. 1926. The art of thought. New York: Harcourt Brace.
    • (1926) The Art of Thought
    • Wallas, G.1
  • 48
    • 0003806577 scopus 로고
    • New York: Doubleday
    • Polanyi argued that there existed a tacit knowledge whose contents are not part of one's normal consciousness or open to introspection (in essence 'we know more than we can tell'). See Polanyi, M. 1966. The tacit dimension. New York: Doubleday.
    • (1966) The Tacit Dimension
    • Polanyi, M.1
  • 50
    • 84939812887 scopus 로고
    • New York: Oxford University Press
    • Reber, A.S. 1993. Implicit learning and tacit knowledge; An essay on the cognitive unconscious, New York: Oxford University Press. Recent advances in cognitive science and artificial intelligence suggest that intuition-as-expertise is an outcome of long periods of learning and expertise. See Tsang, E.W.K. 2004. Towards a scientific inquiry into superstitious business decision making. Organization Studies, 25: 923-946.
    • (1993) Implicit Learning and Tacit Knowledge; an Essay on the Cognitive Unconscious
    • Reber, A.S.1
  • 51
    • 3242757490 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Towards a scientific inquiry into superstitious business decision making
    • Reber, A.S. 1993. Implicit learning and tacit knowledge; An essay on the cognitive unconscious, New York: Oxford University Press. Recent advances in cognitive science and artificial intelligence suggest that intuition-as-expertise is an outcome of long periods of learning and expertise. See Tsang, E.W.K. 2004. Towards a scientific inquiry into superstitious business decision making. Organization Studies, 25: 923-946.
    • (2004) Organization Studies , vol.25 , pp. 923-946
    • Tsang, E.W.K.1
  • 52
    • 0010838484 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Chicago: The University of Chicago Press
    • Hogarth, R.M. 2001. Educating intuition. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
    • (2001) Educating Intuition
    • Hogarth, R.M.1
  • 53
    • 0002327196 scopus 로고
    • Making management decisions: The role of intuition and emotion
    • W.H. Agor (Ed.). Newbury Park, CA.: Sage
    • Simon, H.A. 1989. Making management decisions: The role of intuition and emotion. In W.H. Agor (Ed.). Intuition in organizations: Leading and managing productively. Newbury Park, CA.: Sage.
    • (1989) Intuition in Organizations: Leading and Managing Productively
    • Simon, H.A.1
  • 54
    • 0002114995 scopus 로고
    • Intuition in the context of discovery
    • Bowers, K.S., et al. 1990. Intuition in the context of discovery. Cognitive Psychology, 22: 72-110.
    • (1990) Cognitive Psychology , vol.22 , pp. 72-110
    • Bowers, K.S.1
  • 57
    • 0004146270 scopus 로고
    • Cambridge, MA.: Harvard University Press
    • Davis & Davis, op. cit., 59; Bruner, J.S. 1960. The process of education. Cambridge, MA.: Harvard University Press.
    • (1960) The Process of Education
    • Bruner, J.S.1
  • 58
    • 33750994721 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Smart people doing dumb things: A case of managerial incompetence
    • R.J. Sternberg (Ed.). New Haven & London: Yale University Press
    • Wagner, R.K. 2002. Smart people doing dumb things: A case of managerial incompetence. In R.J. Sternberg (Ed.). Why smart people can be so stupid. New Haven & London: Yale University Press. Related research also suggests that: (1) tacit knowledge is not related to IQ; (2) the relationship between tacit knowledge and performance is independent of relations between personality and performance.
    • (2002) Why Smart People can be so Stupid
    • Wagner, R.K.1
  • 59
    • 11044229365 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • http://www.ceoforum.com.au/200210_reflections.cfm.
  • 60
    • 0031280951 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Klein, op. cit., 124; Jenkins, M., & Johnson, G. 1997. Entrepre-neurial intentions and outcomes: A comparative causal mapping study. Journal of Management Studies, 34 (Special Issue): 895-920.
    • Klein1
  • 61
    • 0031280951 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Entrepre-neurial intentions and outcomes: A comparative causal mapping study
    • Klein, op. cit., 124; Jenkins, M., & Johnson, G. 1997. Entrepre-neurial intentions and outcomes: A comparative causal mapping study. Journal of Management Studies, 34 (Special Issue): 895-920.
    • (1997) Journal of Management Studies , vol.34 , Issue.SPEC. ISSUE , pp. 895-920
    • Jenkins, M.1    Johnson, G.2
  • 66
    • 0004060146 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • New York: Touch-stone/Simon &
    • LeDoux, I. 1996. The emotional brain. New York: Touch-stone/Simon & Schuster, Goleman uses the term 'emotional hijacking' in his popularization of LeDoux's research. See: Goleman, D., 1996, op. cit.
    • (1996) The Emotional Brain
    • LeDoux, I.1
  • 67
    • 0004060146 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • LeDoux, I. 1996. The emotional brain. New York: Touch-stone/Simon & Schuster, Goleman uses the term 'emotional hijacking' in his popularization of LeDoux's research. See: Goleman, D., 1996, op. cit.
    • (1996) The Emotional Brain
    • Goleman, D.1
  • 68
    • 0001847103 scopus 로고
    • Managing overconfidence
    • Winter
    • Russo, J.E., & Schoemaker, P.H.J. 1992. Managing overconfidence. Sloan Management Review, Winter: 7-17; Taggart, W. 1997. Discovering and understanding intuition. Exceptional Human Experience, 15(2): 174-188.
    • (1992) Sloan Management Review , pp. 7-17
    • Russo, J.E.1    Schoemaker, P.H.J.2
  • 69
    • 11044239247 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Discovering and understanding intuition
    • Russo, J.E., & Schoemaker, P.H.J. 1992. Managing overconfidence. Sloan Management Review, Winter: 7-17; Taggart, W. 1997. Discovering and understanding intuition. Exceptional Human Experience, 15(2): 174-188.
    • (1997) Exceptional Human Experience , vol.15 , Issue.2 , pp. 174-188
    • Taggart, W.1
  • 70
    • 11044230723 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • 'Batting average' is used in the English game of cricket; an alternative might be 'strike rate.'
  • 72
    • 0008609383 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The nature of intuition: A neuropsycho-logical approach
    • R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds). London: Routledge
    • Laughlin, C. 1997. The nature of intuition: A neuropsycho-logical approach. In R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds). Intuition: The inside story: Interdisciplinary perspectives. London: Routledge.
    • (1997) Intuition: The Inside Story: Interdisciplinary Perspectives
    • Laughlin, C.1
  • 73
    • 0002011180 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Creative cognition
    • R.J. Sternberg (Ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
    • Ward, T.B., Smith, S.M., & Finke, R.A. 1999. Creative cognition. In R.J. Sternberg (Ed.). The handbook of creativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. The quantum physicist Richard Feynman claimed that one of the reasons Einstein failed to develop a unified theory was that he stopped thinking in concrete physical images and turned to the manipulation of equations. See Monsay, E.H. 1997. Intuition in the development of scientific theory and practice. In R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds). Intuition: The inside story: Interdisciplinary perspectives. London: Routledge. Numerous examples exist in the history of thought in science, the arts, and many other fields of human endeavor which attest to the significant role played by imagery (visual, auditory, and kinesthetic).
    • (1999) The Handbook of Creativity
    • Ward, T.B.1    Smith, S.M.2    Finke, R.A.3
  • 74
    • 0040279425 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Intuition in the development of scientific theory and practice
    • R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds). London: Routledge
    • Ward, T.B., Smith, S.M., & Finke, R.A. 1999. Creative cognition. In R.J. Sternberg (Ed.). The handbook of creativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. The quantum physicist Richard Feynman claimed that one of the reasons Einstein failed to develop a unified theory was that he stopped thinking in concrete physical images and turned to the manipulation of equations. See Monsay, E.H. 1997. Intuition in the development of scientific theory and practice. In R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds). Intuition: The inside story: Interdisciplinary perspectives. London: Routledge. Numerous examples exist in the history of thought in science, the arts, and many other fields of human endeavor which attest to the significant role played by imagery (visual, auditory, and kinesthetic).
    • (1997) Intuition: The Inside Story: Interdisciplinary Perspectives
    • Monsay, E.H.1
  • 76
    • 11044222715 scopus 로고
    • Exercises for bringing head and heart together
    • November
    • Adapted from Watts, G.W. 1993. Exercises for bringing head and heart together. Training & Development, November: 17-19.
    • (1993) Training & Development , pp. 17-19
    • Watts, G.W.1
  • 79
    • 0001515156 scopus 로고
    • Hindsight bias and strategic choice: Some problems in learning from experience
    • Bukszar, E., & Connolly, T. 1988. Hindsight bias and strategic choice: Some problems in learning from experience. Academy of Management Journal, 31(3): 628-641.
    • (1988) Academy of Management Journal , vol.31 , Issue.3 , pp. 628-641
    • Bukszar, E.1    Connolly, T.2
  • 81
    • 0001847103 scopus 로고
    • Managing overconfidence
    • Winter
    • Russo, I.E., & Schoemaker, P.H.J. 1992. Managing overconfidence. Sloan Management Review, Winter: 7-17; Schwenk, C.R. 1984. Devil's advocacy in managerial decision-making. Journal of Management Studies, 21: 153-168.
    • (1992) Sloan Management Review , pp. 7-17
    • Russo, I.E.1    Schoemaker, P.H.J.2
  • 82
    • 84980226093 scopus 로고
    • Devil's advocacy in managerial decision-making
    • Russo, I.E., & Schoemaker, P.H.J. 1992. Managing overconfidence. Sloan Management Review, Winter: 7-17; Schwenk, C.R. 1984. Devil's advocacy in managerial decision-making. Journal of Management Studies, 21: 153-168.
    • (1984) Journal of Management Studies , vol.21 , pp. 153-168
    • Schwenk, C.R.1
  • 83
    • 0000997046 scopus 로고
    • Experiential effects of dialectical enquiry, devil's advocacy, and consensus approaches to strategic decision-making
    • Schweiger, D.M., Sandberg, W.R., & Rechner, P.L. 1989. Experiential effects of dialectical enquiry, devil's advocacy, and consensus approaches to strategic decision-making. Academy of Management Journal, 32: 745-772; Herbert, T.T., & Estes, R.W. 1977. Improving executive planning by formalising dissent: The corporate devil's advocate. Academy of Management Review, 2: 662-667; Rowan, op. cit., 196.
    • (1989) Academy of Management Journal , vol.32 , pp. 745-772
    • Schweiger, D.M.1    Sandberg, W.R.2    Rechner, P.L.3
  • 84
    • 0001258273 scopus 로고
    • Improving executive planning by formalising dissent: The corporate devil's advocate
    • Schweiger, D.M., Sandberg, W.R., & Rechner, P.L. 1989. Experiential effects of dialectical enquiry, devil's advocacy, and consensus approaches to strategic decision-making. Academy of Management Journal, 32: 745-772; Herbert, T.T., & Estes, R.W. 1977. Improving executive planning by formalising dissent: The corporate devil's advocate. Academy of Management Review, 2: 662-667; Rowan, op. cit., 196.
    • (1977) Academy of Management Review , vol.2 , pp. 662-667
    • Herbert, T.T.1    Estes, R.W.2
  • 85
    • 8744307155 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Schweiger, D.M., Sandberg, W.R., & Rechner, P.L. 1989. Experiential effects of dialectical enquiry, devil's advocacy, and consensus approaches to strategic decision-making. Academy of Management Journal, 32: 745-772; Herbert, T.T., & Estes, R.W. 1977. Improving executive planning by formalising dissent: The corporate devil's advocate. Academy of Management Review, 2: 662-667; Rowan, op. cit., 196.
    • Academy of Management Review , pp. 196
    • Rowan1
  • 86
    • 11044221719 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Decision-making in organizations
    • Clegg, S.R., Hardy, C., & Nord, W.R. (Eds). London: Sage
    • See, for example, Miller, S.J., Hickson, D.J., & Wilson, D.C. 1999. Decision-making in organizations. In Clegg, S.R., Hardy, C., & Nord, W.R. (Eds). Managing in organizations. Current issues. London: Sage; Klein, G. 2003. Intuition at work. New York: Century Doubleday; Davis & Davis, op. cit.
    • (1999) Managing in Organizations. Current Issues
    • Miller, S.J.1    Hickson, D.J.2    Wilson, D.C.3
  • 87
    • 0038250526 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • New York: Century Doubleday
    • See, for example, Miller, S.J., Hickson, D.J., & Wilson, D.C. 1999. Decision-making in organizations. In Clegg, S.R., Hardy, C., & Nord, W.R. (Eds). Managing in organizations. Current issues. London: Sage; Klein, G. 2003. Intuition at work. New York: Century Doubleday; Davis & Davis, op. cit.
    • (2003) Intuition at Work
    • Klein, G.1
  • 88
    • 11044226782 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • See, for example, Miller, S.J., Hickson, D.J., & Wilson, D.C. 1999. Decision-making in organizations. In Clegg, S.R., Hardy, C., & Nord, W.R. (Eds). Managing in organizations. Current issues. London: Sage; Klein, G. 2003. Intuition at work. New York: Century Doubleday; Davis & Davis, op. cit.
    • Intuition at Work
    • Davis1    Davis2
  • 89
    • 0040279425 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Intuition in the development of scientific theory and practice
    • R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds). London: Routledge
    • Monsay (1997) cites the views of scholars including Carl Rogers and Jonas Salk in support of the assertion that under the right conditions intuition can be developed. See Monsay, E.H. 1997. Intuition in the development of scientific theory and practice. In R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds). Intuition: The inside story: Interdisciplinary perspectives. London: Routledge.
    • (1997) Intuition: The Inside Story: Interdisciplinary Perspectives
    • Monsay, E.H.1
  • 90
    • 0041996882 scopus 로고
    • Minds and managers: On the dual nature of human information processing and management
    • Taggart, W., & Robey, D. 1981. Minds and managers: On the dual nature of human information processing and management. Academy of Management Review, 6: 187-195.
    • (1981) Academy of Management Review , vol.6 , pp. 187-195
    • Taggart, W.1    Robey, D.2
  • 91
    • 0004069083 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Cambridge, MA.: The MIT Press
    • For example, being mindful is an acute awareness of one's surroundings, thoughts, and behaviors and is engendered by pausing and examining our perceptions and feeling states as dispassionately as we can through a non-reactive awareness and openness to whatever the object of our attention is and our context, with the aim of avoiding premature cognitive commitment (see Austin, J.H. 1998. Zen and the brain. Cambridge, MA.: The MIT Press). Taggart described a quiet, relaxed mind that is focused upon the present moment and free of distracting fears and desires as the sine qua non of intuitive experience (1997, op. cit., 182). Intuitive flow experiences are discussed extensively in Csikszentmihalyi, M. 1992. Flow: The psychology of happiness. London: Rider.
    • (1998) Zen and the Brain
    • Austin, J.H.1
  • 92
    • 11044229366 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • For example, being mindful is an acute awareness of one's surroundings, thoughts, and behaviors and is engendered by pausing and examining our perceptions and feeling states as dispassionately as we can through a non-reactive awareness and openness to whatever the object of our attention is and our context, with the aim of avoiding premature cognitive commitment (see Austin, J.H. 1998. Zen and the brain. Cambridge, MA.: The MIT Press). Taggart described a quiet, relaxed mind that is focused upon the present moment and free of distracting fears and desires as the sine qua non of intuitive experience (1997, op. cit., 182). Intuitive flow experiences are discussed extensively in Csikszentmihalyi, M. 1992. Flow: The psychology of happiness. London: Rider.
    • (1997) Zen and the Brain , pp. 182
  • 93
    • 0003575559 scopus 로고
    • London: Rider
    • For example, being mindful is an acute awareness of one's surroundings, thoughts, and behaviors and is engendered by pausing and examining our perceptions and feeling states as dispassionately as we can through a non-reactive awareness and openness to whatever the object of our attention is and our context, with the aim of avoiding premature cognitive commitment (see Austin, J.H. 1998. Zen and the brain. Cambridge, MA.: The MIT Press). Taggart described a quiet, relaxed mind that is focused upon the present moment and free of distracting fears and desires as the sine qua non of intuitive experience (1997, op. cit., 182). Intuitive flow experiences are discussed extensively in Csikszentmihalyi, M. 1992. Flow: The psychology of happiness. London: Rider.
    • (1992) Flow: The Psychology of Happiness
    • Csikszentmihalyi, M.1
  • 96
    • 62649109947 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Developing intuition: Becoming smarter by thinking less
    • Academy of Management Annual Meeting, New Orleans, Louisiana, August 2004
    • Sadler-Smith, E, & Shefy, E. 2004. Developing intuition: Becoming smarter by thinking less. Academy of Management Best Paper Proceedings, Academy of Management Annual Meeting, New Orleans, Louisiana, August 2004.
    • (2004) Academy of Management Best Paper Proceedings
    • Sadler-Smith, E.1    Shefy, E.2
  • 97
    • 0030627470 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Managerial intuition: A conceptual and operational framework
    • January/February
    • Shapiro, S., & Spence, M.T. 1997. Managerial intuition: A conceptual and operational framework. Business Horizons, January/February: 63-68.
    • (1997) Business Horizons , pp. 63-68
    • Shapiro, S.1    Spence, M.T.2
  • 99
    • 0028489329 scopus 로고
    • Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious
    • See Epstein, S. 1994. Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, 49: 709-724. Stanovich and West make a similar distinction between System 1 processes that are fast, heuristic, associative, and automatic and System 2 processes that are rule-based, slow, load heavily on working memory, and are controlled. See Stanovich, K.E., & West, R.F. 2000. Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23: 645-726.
    • (1994) American Psychologist , vol.49 , pp. 709-724
    • Epstein, S.1
  • 100
    • 0034488449 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate?
    • See Epstein, S. 1994. Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, 49: 709-724. Stanovich and West make a similar distinction between System 1 processes that are fast, heuristic, associative, and automatic and System 2 processes that are rule-based, slow, load heavily on working memory, and are controlled. See Stanovich, K.E., & West, R.F. 2000. Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23: 645-726.
    • (2000) Behavioral and Brain Sciences , vol.23 , pp. 645-726
    • Stanovich, K.E.1    West, R.F.2
  • 101
    • 84909358155 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment
    • Haidt, J. 2001. The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4): 814-834.
    • (2001) Psychological Review , vol.108 , Issue.4 , pp. 814-834
    • Haidt, J.1
  • 102
    • 84993881884 scopus 로고
    • Psychological study of human judgment: Implications for investment decision-making
    • Slavic, P. 1972. Psychological study of human judgment: Implications for investment decision-making. The Journal of Finance, 27(4): 779-799.
    • (1972) The Journal of Finance , vol.27 , Issue.4 , pp. 779-799
    • Slavic, P.1
  • 103
    • 0008609383 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The nature of intuition: A neuro-psychological approach
    • R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds). London: Routledge
    • Writers such as the anthropologist Charles Laughlin attribute this dichotomy to the historical association of intuition with metaphysics and religion. In the Enlightenment and beyond, religion became the nemesis of science, and hence the 'intuitional baby' was thrown out with the 'metaphysical bath-water.' See Laughlin, C. 1997. The nature of intuition: A neuro-psychological approach. In R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds). Intuition the inside story: Interdisciplinary perspectives. London: Routledge.
    • (1997) Intuition the Inside Story: Interdisciplinary Perspectives
    • Laughlin, C.1


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.