-
1
-
-
11044226571
-
-
Robert Salmon, former vice-president of L'Oreal (http://www. refresher.com/lsignals); Lindsey Owen-Jones, chairman and chief executive officer of L'Oreal, in Business Week Online. 28 June 1999.
-
-
-
Salmon, R.1
Owen-Jones, L.2
-
2
-
-
11044221185
-
-
28 June
-
Robert Salmon, former vice-president of L'Oreal (http://www. refresher.com/lsignals); Lindsey Owen-Jones, chairman and chief executive officer of L'Oreal, in Business Week Online. 28 June 1999.
-
(1999)
Business Week Online
-
-
-
3
-
-
11044235446
-
-
Lanham: Scarecrow
-
Davis, S.H., & Davis, P.B. 2003. The intuitive dimensions of administrative decision-making. Lanham: Scarecrow; Eisen-hardt, K.M. 1999. Strategy as decision-making. Sloan Management Review, Spring: 65-72. For a summary of the dominant paradigms, see Das, T.K., & Teng, B-S. 1999. Cognitive biases and strategic decision processes: An integrative perspective. Journal of Management Studies, 36(6): 757-778. The question remains as to where an intuitive decision mode fits within the spectrum of approaches identified by Das and Teng, op. cit., 759.
-
(2003)
The Intuitive Dimensions of Administrative Decision-making
-
-
Davis, S.H.1
Davis, P.B.2
-
4
-
-
0002466471
-
Strategy as decision-making
-
Spring
-
Davis, S.H., & Davis, P.B. 2003. The intuitive dimensions of administrative decision-making. Lanham: Scarecrow; Eisen-hardt, K.M. 1999. Strategy as decision-making. Sloan Management Review, Spring: 65-72. For a summary of the dominant paradigms, see Das, T.K., & Teng, B-S. 1999. Cognitive biases and strategic decision processes: An integrative perspective. Journal of Management Studies, 36(6): 757-778. The question remains as to where an intuitive decision mode fits within the spectrum of approaches identified by Das and Teng, op. cit., 759.
-
(1999)
Sloan Management Review
, pp. 65-72
-
-
Eisen-Hardt, K.M.1
-
5
-
-
0033228946
-
Cognitive biases and strategic decision processes: An integrative perspective
-
Davis, S.H., & Davis, P.B. 2003. The intuitive dimensions of administrative decision-making. Lanham: Scarecrow; Eisen-hardt, K.M. 1999. Strategy as decision-making. Sloan Management Review, Spring: 65-72. For a summary of the dominant paradigms, see Das, T.K., & Teng, B-S. 1999. Cognitive biases and strategic decision processes: An integrative perspective. Journal of Management Studies, 36(6): 757-778. The question remains as to where an intuitive decision mode fits within the spectrum of approaches identified by Das and Teng, op. cit., 759.
-
(1999)
Journal of Management Studies
, vol.36
, Issue.6
, pp. 757-778
-
-
Das, T.K.1
Teng, B.-S.2
-
6
-
-
0035261170
-
When to trust your gut
-
February
-
Hayashi, A.M. 2001. When to trust your gut. Harvard Business Review, February: 59-65.
-
(2001)
Harvard Business Review
, pp. 59-65
-
-
Hayashi, A.M.1
-
7
-
-
0002953738
-
The information environments of organisations
-
P.M. Javlin, et al. (Eds.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage
-
Huber, G.P., & Daft, R.L. 1987. The information environments of organisations. In P.M. Javlin, et al. (Eds.). Handbook of organizational communication. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
-
(1987)
Handbook of Organizational Communication
-
-
Huber, G.P.1
Daft, R.L.2
-
8
-
-
11044221860
-
-
Buckingham: Open University Press discusses the importance of rationality and intention
-
Hodgkinson, G.P., & Sparrow, P. 2002. The competent organization. Buckingham: Open University Press discusses the importance of rationality and intention.
-
(2002)
The Competent Organization
-
-
Hodgkinson, G.P.1
Sparrow, P.2
-
9
-
-
11044234847
-
Emotion and learning
-
S.R. Clegg, C. Hardy, & W.R. Nord (Eds). London: Sage
-
Fineman, S. 1996: Emotion and learning. In S.R. Clegg, C. Hardy, & W.R. Nord (Eds). Handbook of organization studies. London: Sage: 545.
-
(1996)
Handbook of Organization Studies
, pp. 545
-
-
Fineman, S.1
-
10
-
-
0011400739
-
Effects of environmental uncertainty on information and decisions in banks
-
Leblebici, H., & Salancik, G.R. 1981. Effects of environmental uncertainty on information and decisions in banks. Administrative Science Quarterly, 26(4): 578-596; Narayanan, V.K., & Fahey, L. 1982. The micro-politics of strategy formulation. Academy of Management Review, 7(1): 25-34; Bourgeois, L.J., & Eisenhardt, K.M. 1988. Strategic decision processes in high velocity environments: Four cases in the micro-computer industry. Management Science, 34(7): 816-835.
-
(1981)
Administrative Science Quarterly
, vol.26
, Issue.4
, pp. 578-596
-
-
Leblebici, H.1
Salancik, G.R.2
-
11
-
-
0002495178
-
The micro-politics of strategy formulation
-
Leblebici, H., & Salancik, G.R. 1981. Effects of environmental uncertainty on information and decisions in banks. Administrative Science Quarterly, 26(4): 578-596; Narayanan, V.K., & Fahey, L. 1982. The micro-politics of strategy formulation. Academy of Management Review, 7(1): 25-34; Bourgeois, L.J., & Eisenhardt, K.M. 1988. Strategic decision processes in high velocity environments: Four cases in the micro-computer industry. Management Science, 34(7): 816-835.
-
(1982)
Academy of Management Review
, vol.7
, Issue.1
, pp. 25-34
-
-
Narayanan, V.K.1
Fahey, L.2
-
12
-
-
0000634867
-
Strategic decision processes in high velocity environments: Four cases in the micro-computer industry
-
Leblebici, H., & Salancik, G.R. 1981. Effects of environmental uncertainty on information and decisions in banks. Administrative Science Quarterly, 26(4): 578-596; Narayanan, V.K., & Fahey, L. 1982. The micro-politics of strategy formulation. Academy of Management Review, 7(1): 25-34; Bourgeois, L.J., & Eisenhardt, K.M. 1988. Strategic decision processes in high velocity environments: Four cases in the micro-computer industry. Management Science, 34(7): 816-835.
-
(1988)
Management Science
, vol.34
, Issue.7
, pp. 816-835
-
-
Bourgeois, L.J.1
Eisenhardt, K.M.2
-
13
-
-
0030304390
-
Does decision-making matter: A study of strategic decision-making effectiveness
-
The relationship between procedural rationality and decision-making effectiveness (DME) was positive and statistically significant and when controlling for environmental instability (r = 0.35, p < 0.01; t = 2.30, p < 0.05). See: Dean, J.W., & Sharfman, M.P. 1996. Does decision-making matter: A study of strategic decision-making effectiveness. Academy of Management Journal, 39(2): 368-396.
-
(1996)
Academy of Management Journal
, vol.39
, Issue.2
, pp. 368-396
-
-
Dean, J.W.1
Sharfman, M.P.2
-
14
-
-
85025221771
-
Procedural rationality in the strategic decision-making process
-
The correlation between rationality and uncertainty was negative and statistically significant (r = -0.38, p < 0.01). See: Dean, J.W., & Sharfman, M.P. 1993. Procedural rationality in the strategic decision-making process. Journal of Management Studies, 30(4): 587-620.
-
(1993)
Journal of Management Studies
, vol.30
, Issue.4
, pp. 587-620
-
-
Dean, J.W.1
Sharfman, M.P.2
-
15
-
-
0001229241
-
Organizational information requirements, media richness and structural design
-
Daft, R.L., & Lengel, H.R. 1986. Organizational information requirements, media richness and structural design. Management Science, 32(5): 554-571. The research findings in support of these contrasting viewpoints are themselves somewhat complex. For example, Dean & Sharfman (1993) found that decision-makers engaged in less rational processes of strategic decision-making when faced with highly uncertain problems; whereas when there was little competitive threat, little perceived external control, and the issues being faced were well understood, executives used rational procedures. Fredrickson & Mitchell (1984) reported a negative relationship between rationality and organizational performance and used it as introductory evidence that rational models are not appropriate for some environments. Further research by Fredrickson (1984) established a positive relationship between rationality and performance in stable environments. See Fredrickson, J.W., & Mitchell, T.R. 1984. Strategic decision processes: Comprehensiveness and performance in an industry with an unstable environment. Academy of Management Journal, 27(2): 399-423; Fredrickson, J.W. 1984. The comprehensiveness of strategic decision processes: Extension, observations, future directions. Academy of Management Journal, 27(3): 445-466; Priem, R.L., Rasheed, A., & Kotulik, A. 1995. Rationality in strategic decision processes, environmental dynamism and firm performance. Journal of Management, 21 (5): 913-929.
-
(1986)
Management Science
, vol.32
, Issue.5
, pp. 554-571
-
-
Daft, R.L.1
Lengel, H.R.2
-
16
-
-
0000866269
-
Strategic decision processes: Comprehensiveness and performance in an industry with an unstable environment
-
Daft, R.L., & Lengel, H.R. 1986. Organizational information requirements, media richness and structural design. Management Science, 32(5): 554-571. The research findings in support of these contrasting viewpoints are themselves somewhat complex. For example, Dean & Sharfman (1993) found that decision-makers engaged in less rational processes of strategic decision-making when faced with highly uncertain problems; whereas when there was little competitive threat, little perceived external control, and the issues being faced were well understood, executives used rational procedures. Fredrickson & Mitchell (1984) reported a negative relationship between rationality and organizational performance and used it as introductory evidence that rational models are not appropriate for some environments. Further research by Fredrickson (1984) established a positive relationship between rationality and performance in stable environments. See Fredrickson, J.W., & Mitchell, T.R. 1984. Strategic decision processes: Comprehensiveness and performance in an industry with an unstable environment. Academy of Management Journal, 27(2): 399-423; Fredrickson, J.W. 1984. The comprehensiveness of strategic decision processes: Extension, observations, future directions. Academy of Management Journal, 27(3): 445-466; Priem, R.L., Rasheed, A., & Kotulik, A. 1995. Rationality in strategic decision processes, environmental dynamism and firm performance. Journal of Management, 21 (5): 913-929.
-
(1984)
Academy of Management Journal
, vol.27
, Issue.2
, pp. 399-423
-
-
Fredrickson, J.W.1
Mitchell, T.R.2
-
17
-
-
0000341186
-
The comprehensiveness of strategic decision processes: Extension, observations, future directions
-
Daft, R.L., & Lengel, H.R. 1986. Organizational information requirements, media richness and structural design. Management Science, 32(5): 554-571. The research findings in support of these contrasting viewpoints are themselves somewhat complex. For example, Dean & Sharfman (1993) found that decision-makers engaged in less rational processes of strategic decision-making when faced with highly uncertain problems; whereas when there was little competitive threat, little perceived external control, and the issues being faced were well understood, executives used rational procedures. Fredrickson & Mitchell (1984) reported a negative relationship between rationality and organizational performance and used it as introductory evidence that rational models are not appropriate for some environments. Further research by Fredrickson (1984) established a positive relationship between rationality and performance in stable environments. See Fredrickson, J.W., & Mitchell, T.R. 1984. Strategic decision processes: Comprehensiveness and performance in an industry with an unstable environment. Academy of Management Journal, 27(2): 399-423; Fredrickson, J.W. 1984. The comprehensiveness of strategic decision processes: Extension, observations, future directions. Academy of Management Journal, 27(3): 445-466; Priem, R.L., Rasheed, A., & Kotulik, A. 1995. Rationality in strategic decision processes, environmental dynamism and firm performance. Journal of Management, 21 (5): 913-929.
-
(1984)
Academy of Management Journal
, vol.27
, Issue.3
, pp. 445-466
-
-
Fredrickson, J.W.1
-
18
-
-
34249096931
-
Rationality in strategic decision processes, environmental dynamism and firm performance
-
Daft, R.L., & Lengel, H.R. 1986. Organizational information requirements, media richness and structural design. Management Science, 32(5): 554-571. The research findings in support of these contrasting viewpoints are themselves somewhat complex. For example, Dean & Sharfman (1993) found that decision-makers engaged in less rational processes of strategic decision-making when faced with highly uncertain problems; whereas when there was little competitive threat, little perceived external control, and the issues being faced were well understood, executives used rational procedures. Fredrickson & Mitchell (1984) reported a negative relationship between rationality and organizational performance and used it as introductory evidence that rational models are not appropriate for some environments. Further research by Fredrickson (1984) established a positive relationship between rationality and performance in stable environments. See Fredrickson, J.W., & Mitchell, T.R. 1984. Strategic decision processes: Comprehensiveness and performance in an industry with an unstable environment. Academy of Management Journal, 27(2): 399-423; Fredrickson, J.W. 1984. The comprehensiveness of strategic decision processes: Extension, observations, future directions. Academy of Management Journal, 27(3): 445-466; Priem, R.L., Rasheed, A., & Kotulik, A. 1995. Rationality in strategic decision processes, environmental dynamism and firm performance. Journal of Management, 21 (5): 913-929.
-
(1995)
Journal of Management
, vol.21
, Issue.5
, pp. 913-929
-
-
Priem, R.L.1
Rasheed, A.2
Kotulik, A.3
-
21
-
-
0000974763
-
The unbearable automaticity of being
-
Bargh, J.A., & Chartrand, T.L. 1999. The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54(7): 462-479.
-
(1999)
American Psychologist
, vol.54
, Issue.7
, pp. 462-479
-
-
Bargh, J.A.1
Chartrand, T.L.2
-
23
-
-
0011059493
-
Taking the mystery out of intuitive decision-making
-
Burke, L.A., & Miller, M.K. 1999. Taking the mystery out of intuitive decision-making. The Academy of Management Executive. 13(4): 91-99.
-
(1999)
The Academy of Management Executive
, vol.13
, Issue.4
, pp. 91-99
-
-
Burke, L.A.1
Miller, M.K.2
-
24
-
-
0000214042
-
How senior managers think
-
November/December
-
Isenberg, D.J. 1984. How senior managers think. Harvard Business Review, November/December: 81-90.
-
(1984)
Harvard Business Review
, pp. 81-90
-
-
Isenberg, D.J.1
-
25
-
-
11044230500
-
Trust you gut
-
July
-
Overell, S. 2001. Trust you gut. Director, July: 28.
-
(2001)
Director
, pp. 28
-
-
Overell, S.1
-
27
-
-
0023109850
-
How expert nurses use intuition
-
January
-
Benner, P., & Tanner, C. 1987. How expert nurses use intuition. American Journal of Nursing, January: 23-31; Davis & Davis, op. cit.
-
(1987)
American Journal of Nursing
, pp. 23-31
-
-
Benner, P.1
Tanner, C.2
-
29
-
-
0029691631
-
The Cognitive Style Index: A measure of intuition-analysis for organizational research
-
See Allinson, C.W., & Hayes, J. 1996. The Cognitive Style Index: A measure of intuition-analysis for organizational research. Journal of Management Studies, 33(1): 19-135; Allinson, C.W., Chell, E., & Hayes, J. 2000. Intuition and entrepreneurial behavior. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 9: 31-43; Sadler-Smith, E. 2004. Cognitive style and the management of small and medium sized enterprises. Organization Studies, 25: 155- 181; Clarke, I., & Mackaness, W. 2001. Management intuition: An interpretative account of structure and content of decision schemas using cognitive maps. Journal of Management Studies, 38(2): 147-172.
-
(1996)
Journal of Management Studies
, vol.33
, Issue.1
, pp. 19-135
-
-
Allinson, C.W.1
Hayes, J.2
-
30
-
-
0007150255
-
Intuition and entrepreneurial behavior
-
See Allinson, C.W., & Hayes, J. 1996. The Cognitive Style Index: A measure of intuition-analysis for organizational research. Journal of Management Studies, 33(1): 19-135; Allinson, C.W., Chell, E., & Hayes, J. 2000. Intuition and entrepreneurial behavior. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 9: 31-43; Sadler-Smith, E. 2004. Cognitive style and the management of small and medium sized enterprises. Organization Studies, 25: 155- 181; Clarke, I., & Mackaness, W. 2001. Management intuition: An interpretative account of structure and content of decision schemas using cognitive maps. Journal of Management Studies, 38(2): 147-172.
-
(2000)
European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology
, vol.9
, pp. 31-43
-
-
Allinson, C.W.1
Chell, E.2
Hayes, J.3
-
31
-
-
1442280726
-
Cognitive style and the management of small and medium sized enterprises
-
See Allinson, C.W., & Hayes, J. 1996. The Cognitive Style Index: A measure of intuition-analysis for organizational research. Journal of Management Studies, 33(1): 19-135; Allinson, C.W., Chell, E., & Hayes, J. 2000. Intuition and entrepreneurial behavior. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 9: 31-43; Sadler-Smith, E. 2004. Cognitive style and the management of small and medium sized enterprises. Organization Studies, 25: 155- 181; Clarke, I., & Mackaness, W. 2001. Management intuition: An interpretative account of structure and content of decision schemas using cognitive maps. Journal of Management Studies, 38(2): 147-172.
-
(2004)
Organization Studies
, vol.25
, pp. 155-181
-
-
Sadler-Smith, E.1
-
32
-
-
0035285417
-
Management intuition: An interpretative account of structure and content of decision schemas using cognitive maps
-
See Allinson, C.W., & Hayes, J. 1996. The Cognitive Style Index: A measure of intuition-analysis for organizational research. Journal of Management Studies, 33(1): 19-135; Allinson, C.W., Chell, E., & Hayes, J. 2000. Intuition and entrepreneurial behavior. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 9: 31-43; Sadler-Smith, E. 2004. Cognitive style and the management of small and medium sized enterprises. Organization Studies, 25: 155- 181; Clarke, I., & Mackaness, W. 2001. Management intuition: An interpretative account of structure and content of decision schemas using cognitive maps. Journal of Management Studies, 38(2): 147-172.
-
(2001)
Journal of Management Studies
, vol.38
, Issue.2
, pp. 147-172
-
-
Clarke, I.1
Mackaness, W.2
-
33
-
-
0030529161
-
Using the MBTI to study managers: A literature review and research agenda
-
Gardner, W.L., & Martinko, M.J. 1996. Using the MBTI to study managers: A literature review and research agenda. Journal of Management, 22(1): 45-83.
-
(1996)
Journal of Management
, vol.22
, Issue.1
, pp. 45-83
-
-
Gardner, W.L.1
Martinko, M.J.2
-
35
-
-
0000580380
-
Individuals and information overload in organizations: Is more necessarily better?
-
O'Reilly, C.A. 1980. Individuals and information overload in organizations: Is more necessarily better? Academy of Management Journal, 23(4): 684-696.
-
(1980)
Academy of Management Journal
, vol.23
, Issue.4
, pp. 684-696
-
-
O'Reilly, C.A.1
-
36
-
-
7544237747
-
Strategy and cognition: Understanding the role of management knowledge structures, organizational memory and information overload
-
Sparrow, P.R. 1999. Strategy and cognition: Understanding the role of management knowledge structures, organizational memory and information overload. Creativity and Innovation Management, 8(2): 140-148.
-
(1999)
Creativity and Innovation Management
, vol.8
, Issue.2
, pp. 140-148
-
-
Sparrow, P.R.1
-
37
-
-
0035434551
-
Intuition: An important tool in the practice of nursing
-
McCutcheon, H.H.I., & Pincombe, J. 2001. Intuition: An important tool in the practice of nursing. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 35: 342-348; Atkinson, L. 2000. Trusting your own judgment. In Atkinson, T., & Claxton, G. (Eds). 2000. The intuitive practitioner. Buckingham: Open University Press: 53-66; Goldberg, P. 1989. The many faces of intuition. In W.H. Agor (Ed.), Intuition in organisations: Leading and managing productively. Newbury Park, CA.: Sage Publications.
-
(2001)
Journal of Advanced Nursing
, vol.35
, pp. 342-348
-
-
McCutcheon, H.H.I.1
Pincombe, J.2
-
38
-
-
0035434551
-
Trusting your own judgment
-
Atkinson, T., & Claxton, G. (Eds). 2000. Buckingham: Open University Press
-
McCutcheon, H.H.I., & Pincombe, J. 2001. Intuition: An important tool in the practice of nursing. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 35: 342-348; Atkinson, L. 2000. Trusting your own judgment. In Atkinson, T., & Claxton, G. (Eds). 2000. The intuitive practitioner. Buckingham: Open University Press: 53-66; Goldberg, P. 1989. The many faces of intuition. In W.H. Agor (Ed.), Intuition in organisations: Leading and managing productively. Newbury Park, CA.: Sage Publications.
-
(2000)
The Intuitive Practitioner
, pp. 53-66
-
-
Atkinson, L.1
-
39
-
-
0035434551
-
The many faces of intuition
-
W.H. Agor (Ed.). Newbury Park, CA.: Sage Publications
-
McCutcheon, H.H.I., & Pincombe, J. 2001. Intuition: An important tool in the practice of nursing. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 35: 342-348; Atkinson, L. 2000. Trusting your own judgment. In Atkinson, T., & Claxton, G. (Eds). 2000. The intuitive practitioner. Buckingham: Open University Press: 53-66; Goldberg, P. 1989. The many faces of intuition. In W.H. Agor (Ed.), Intuition in organisations: Leading and managing productively. Newbury Park, CA.: Sage Publications.
-
(1989)
Intuition in Organisations: Leading and Managing Productively
-
-
Goldberg, P.1
-
40
-
-
11044225783
-
Subjectivity and intuition in the scientific method
-
R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds), London: Routledge
-
The questions are useful only as a starting point for self-reflection and should not be used a basis for typecasting. For example, Einstein is seen by many as the archetypal rational mind, but he is widely reported as recognizing intuition as the 'sower of all true science.' See Dunne, B.J. 1997. Subjectivity and intuition in the scientific method. In R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds), Intuition: The inside story: Interdisciplinary perspectives. London: Routledge. For valid and reliable self-report measures of intuition and their psychometric evaluation, the following research may be consulted: Allinson & Hayes, 1996, op. cit.; Epstein, S., et al. 1996. Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-reasoning thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71: 390-405; Hodgkinson, G.P., & Sadler-Smith, E. 2003. Complex or unitary? A critique and empirical reassessment of the Allinson-Hayes Cognitive Style Index. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 76: 243-268; Scott, S.G., & Bruce, R.A. 1994. Determinants of innovative behavior: A path model of individual innovation in the workplace. Academy of Management Journal, 37: 580-607.
-
(1997)
Intuition: The Inside Story: Interdisciplinary Perspectives
-
-
Dunne, B.J.1
-
41
-
-
11044224182
-
-
London: Routledge
-
The questions are useful only as a starting point for self-reflection and should not be used a basis for typecasting. For example, Einstein is seen by many as the archetypal rational mind, but he is widely reported as recognizing intuition as the 'sower of all true science.' See Dunne, B.J. 1997. Subjectivity and intuition in the scientific method. In R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds), Intuition: The inside story: Interdisciplinary perspectives. London: Routledge. For valid and reliable self-report measures of intuition and their psychometric evaluation, the following research may be consulted: Allinson & Hayes, 1996, op. cit.; Epstein, S., et al. 1996. Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-reasoning thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71: 390-405; Hodgkinson, G.P., & Sadler-Smith, E. 2003. Complex or unitary? A critique and empirical reassessment of the Allinson-Hayes Cognitive Style Index. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 76: 243-268; Scott, S.G., & Bruce, R.A. 1994. Determinants of innovative behavior: A path model of individual innovation in the workplace. Academy of Management Journal, 37: 580-607.
-
(1996)
Intuition: The Inside Story: Interdisciplinary Perspectives
-
-
Allinson1
Hayes2
-
42
-
-
0030209604
-
Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-reasoning thinking styles
-
The questions are useful only as a starting point for self-reflection and should not be used a basis for typecasting. For example, Einstein is seen by many as the archetypal rational mind, but he is widely reported as recognizing intuition as the 'sower of all true science.' See Dunne, B.J. 1997. Subjectivity and intuition in the scientific method. In R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds), Intuition: The inside story: Interdisciplinary perspectives. London: Routledge. For valid and reliable self-report measures of intuition and their psychometric evaluation, the following research may be consulted: Allinson & Hayes, 1996, op. cit.; Epstein, S., et al. 1996. Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-reasoning thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71: 390-405; Hodgkinson, G.P., & Sadler-Smith, E. 2003. Complex or unitary? A critique and empirical reassessment of the Allinson-Hayes Cognitive Style Index. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 76: 243-268; Scott, S.G., & Bruce, R.A. 1994. Determinants of innovative behavior: A path model of individual innovation in the workplace. Academy of Management Journal, 37: 580-607.
-
(1996)
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
, vol.71
, pp. 390-405
-
-
Epstein, S.1
-
43
-
-
0038772264
-
Complex or unitary? A critique and empirical reassessment of the Allinson-Hayes Cognitive Style Index
-
The questions are useful only as a starting point for self-reflection and should not be used a basis for typecasting. For example, Einstein is seen by many as the archetypal rational mind, but he is widely reported as recognizing intuition as the 'sower of all true science.' See Dunne, B.J. 1997. Subjectivity and intuition in the scientific method. In R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds), Intuition: The inside story: Interdisciplinary perspectives. London: Routledge. For valid and reliable self-report measures of intuition and their psychometric evaluation, the following research may be consulted: Allinson & Hayes, 1996, op. cit.; Epstein, S., et al. 1996. Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-reasoning thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71: 390-405; Hodgkinson, G.P., & Sadler-Smith, E. 2003. Complex or unitary? A critique and empirical reassessment of the Allinson-Hayes Cognitive Style Index. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 76: 243-268; Scott, S.G., & Bruce, R.A. 1994. Determinants of innovative behavior: A path model of individual innovation in the workplace. Academy of Management Journal, 37: 580-607.
-
(2003)
Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology
, vol.76
, pp. 243-268
-
-
Hodgkinson, G.P.1
Sadler-Smith, E.2
-
44
-
-
18944400441
-
Determinants of innovative behavior: A path model of individual innovation in the workplace
-
The questions are useful only as a starting point for self-reflection and should not be used a basis for typecasting. For example, Einstein is seen by many as the archetypal rational mind, but he is widely reported as recognizing intuition as the 'sower of all true science.' See Dunne, B.J. 1997. Subjectivity and intuition in the scientific method. In R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds), Intuition: The inside story: Interdisciplinary perspectives. London: Routledge. For valid and reliable self-report measures of intuition and their psychometric evaluation, the following research may be consulted: Allinson & Hayes, 1996, op. cit.; Epstein, S., et al. 1996. Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-reasoning thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71: 390-405; Hodgkinson, G.P., & Sadler-Smith, E. 2003. Complex or unitary? A critique and empirical reassessment of the Allinson-Hayes Cognitive Style Index. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 76: 243-268; Scott, S.G., & Bruce, R.A. 1994. Determinants of innovative behavior: A path model of individual innovation in the workplace. Academy of Management Journal, 37: 580-607.
-
(1994)
Academy of Management Journal
, vol.37
, pp. 580-607
-
-
Scott, S.G.1
Bruce, R.A.2
-
45
-
-
0020109716
-
On the study of statistical intuitions
-
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. 1982. On the study of statistical intuitions. Cognition, 11: 123-141.
-
(1982)
Cognition
, vol.11
, pp. 123-141
-
-
Kahneman, D.1
Tversky, A.2
-
46
-
-
0242674910
-
The anatomy of intuition
-
Atkinson, T., & Claxton, G. (Eds). Buckingham: Open University Press
-
Claxton, G. 2001. The anatomy of intuition. In Atkinson, T., & Claxton, G. (Eds). The intuitive practitioner. Buckingham: Open University Press: 32-52.
-
(2001)
The Intuitive Practitioner
, pp. 32-52
-
-
Claxton, G.1
-
47
-
-
0004143473
-
-
New York: Harcourt Brace
-
This is the way in which Wallas (1926) described intuition in the context of creativity and insight in his incubation-illumination theory of creativity. See Wallas, G. 1926. The art of thought. New York: Harcourt Brace.
-
(1926)
The Art of Thought
-
-
Wallas, G.1
-
48
-
-
0003806577
-
-
New York: Doubleday
-
Polanyi argued that there existed a tacit knowledge whose contents are not part of one's normal consciousness or open to introspection (in essence 'we know more than we can tell'). See Polanyi, M. 1966. The tacit dimension. New York: Doubleday.
-
(1966)
The Tacit Dimension
-
-
Polanyi, M.1
-
50
-
-
84939812887
-
-
New York: Oxford University Press
-
Reber, A.S. 1993. Implicit learning and tacit knowledge; An essay on the cognitive unconscious, New York: Oxford University Press. Recent advances in cognitive science and artificial intelligence suggest that intuition-as-expertise is an outcome of long periods of learning and expertise. See Tsang, E.W.K. 2004. Towards a scientific inquiry into superstitious business decision making. Organization Studies, 25: 923-946.
-
(1993)
Implicit Learning and Tacit Knowledge; an Essay on the Cognitive Unconscious
-
-
Reber, A.S.1
-
51
-
-
3242757490
-
Towards a scientific inquiry into superstitious business decision making
-
Reber, A.S. 1993. Implicit learning and tacit knowledge; An essay on the cognitive unconscious, New York: Oxford University Press. Recent advances in cognitive science and artificial intelligence suggest that intuition-as-expertise is an outcome of long periods of learning and expertise. See Tsang, E.W.K. 2004. Towards a scientific inquiry into superstitious business decision making. Organization Studies, 25: 923-946.
-
(2004)
Organization Studies
, vol.25
, pp. 923-946
-
-
Tsang, E.W.K.1
-
52
-
-
0010838484
-
-
Chicago: The University of Chicago Press
-
Hogarth, R.M. 2001. Educating intuition. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
-
(2001)
Educating Intuition
-
-
Hogarth, R.M.1
-
53
-
-
0002327196
-
Making management decisions: The role of intuition and emotion
-
W.H. Agor (Ed.). Newbury Park, CA.: Sage
-
Simon, H.A. 1989. Making management decisions: The role of intuition and emotion. In W.H. Agor (Ed.). Intuition in organizations: Leading and managing productively. Newbury Park, CA.: Sage.
-
(1989)
Intuition in Organizations: Leading and Managing Productively
-
-
Simon, H.A.1
-
54
-
-
0002114995
-
Intuition in the context of discovery
-
Bowers, K.S., et al. 1990. Intuition in the context of discovery. Cognitive Psychology, 22: 72-110.
-
(1990)
Cognitive Psychology
, vol.22
, pp. 72-110
-
-
Bowers, K.S.1
-
57
-
-
0004146270
-
-
Cambridge, MA.: Harvard University Press
-
Davis & Davis, op. cit., 59; Bruner, J.S. 1960. The process of education. Cambridge, MA.: Harvard University Press.
-
(1960)
The Process of Education
-
-
Bruner, J.S.1
-
58
-
-
33750994721
-
Smart people doing dumb things: A case of managerial incompetence
-
R.J. Sternberg (Ed.). New Haven & London: Yale University Press
-
Wagner, R.K. 2002. Smart people doing dumb things: A case of managerial incompetence. In R.J. Sternberg (Ed.). Why smart people can be so stupid. New Haven & London: Yale University Press. Related research also suggests that: (1) tacit knowledge is not related to IQ; (2) the relationship between tacit knowledge and performance is independent of relations between personality and performance.
-
(2002)
Why Smart People can be so Stupid
-
-
Wagner, R.K.1
-
59
-
-
11044229365
-
-
http://www.ceoforum.com.au/200210_reflections.cfm.
-
-
-
-
60
-
-
0031280951
-
-
Klein, op. cit., 124; Jenkins, M., & Johnson, G. 1997. Entrepre-neurial intentions and outcomes: A comparative causal mapping study. Journal of Management Studies, 34 (Special Issue): 895-920.
-
-
-
Klein1
-
61
-
-
0031280951
-
Entrepre-neurial intentions and outcomes: A comparative causal mapping study
-
Klein, op. cit., 124; Jenkins, M., & Johnson, G. 1997. Entrepre-neurial intentions and outcomes: A comparative causal mapping study. Journal of Management Studies, 34 (Special Issue): 895-920.
-
(1997)
Journal of Management Studies
, vol.34
, Issue.SPEC. ISSUE
, pp. 895-920
-
-
Jenkins, M.1
Johnson, G.2
-
66
-
-
0004060146
-
-
New York: Touch-stone/Simon &
-
LeDoux, I. 1996. The emotional brain. New York: Touch-stone/Simon & Schuster, Goleman uses the term 'emotional hijacking' in his popularization of LeDoux's research. See: Goleman, D., 1996, op. cit.
-
(1996)
The Emotional Brain
-
-
LeDoux, I.1
-
67
-
-
0004060146
-
-
LeDoux, I. 1996. The emotional brain. New York: Touch-stone/Simon & Schuster, Goleman uses the term 'emotional hijacking' in his popularization of LeDoux's research. See: Goleman, D., 1996, op. cit.
-
(1996)
The Emotional Brain
-
-
Goleman, D.1
-
68
-
-
0001847103
-
Managing overconfidence
-
Winter
-
Russo, J.E., & Schoemaker, P.H.J. 1992. Managing overconfidence. Sloan Management Review, Winter: 7-17; Taggart, W. 1997. Discovering and understanding intuition. Exceptional Human Experience, 15(2): 174-188.
-
(1992)
Sloan Management Review
, pp. 7-17
-
-
Russo, J.E.1
Schoemaker, P.H.J.2
-
69
-
-
11044239247
-
Discovering and understanding intuition
-
Russo, J.E., & Schoemaker, P.H.J. 1992. Managing overconfidence. Sloan Management Review, Winter: 7-17; Taggart, W. 1997. Discovering and understanding intuition. Exceptional Human Experience, 15(2): 174-188.
-
(1997)
Exceptional Human Experience
, vol.15
, Issue.2
, pp. 174-188
-
-
Taggart, W.1
-
70
-
-
11044230723
-
-
note
-
'Batting average' is used in the English game of cricket; an alternative might be 'strike rate.'
-
-
-
-
72
-
-
0008609383
-
The nature of intuition: A neuropsycho-logical approach
-
R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds). London: Routledge
-
Laughlin, C. 1997. The nature of intuition: A neuropsycho-logical approach. In R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds). Intuition: The inside story: Interdisciplinary perspectives. London: Routledge.
-
(1997)
Intuition: The Inside Story: Interdisciplinary Perspectives
-
-
Laughlin, C.1
-
73
-
-
0002011180
-
Creative cognition
-
R.J. Sternberg (Ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
-
Ward, T.B., Smith, S.M., & Finke, R.A. 1999. Creative cognition. In R.J. Sternberg (Ed.). The handbook of creativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. The quantum physicist Richard Feynman claimed that one of the reasons Einstein failed to develop a unified theory was that he stopped thinking in concrete physical images and turned to the manipulation of equations. See Monsay, E.H. 1997. Intuition in the development of scientific theory and practice. In R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds). Intuition: The inside story: Interdisciplinary perspectives. London: Routledge. Numerous examples exist in the history of thought in science, the arts, and many other fields of human endeavor which attest to the significant role played by imagery (visual, auditory, and kinesthetic).
-
(1999)
The Handbook of Creativity
-
-
Ward, T.B.1
Smith, S.M.2
Finke, R.A.3
-
74
-
-
0040279425
-
Intuition in the development of scientific theory and practice
-
R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds). London: Routledge
-
Ward, T.B., Smith, S.M., & Finke, R.A. 1999. Creative cognition. In R.J. Sternberg (Ed.). The handbook of creativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. The quantum physicist Richard Feynman claimed that one of the reasons Einstein failed to develop a unified theory was that he stopped thinking in concrete physical images and turned to the manipulation of equations. See Monsay, E.H. 1997. Intuition in the development of scientific theory and practice. In R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds). Intuition: The inside story: Interdisciplinary perspectives. London: Routledge. Numerous examples exist in the history of thought in science, the arts, and many other fields of human endeavor which attest to the significant role played by imagery (visual, auditory, and kinesthetic).
-
(1997)
Intuition: The Inside Story: Interdisciplinary Perspectives
-
-
Monsay, E.H.1
-
76
-
-
11044222715
-
Exercises for bringing head and heart together
-
November
-
Adapted from Watts, G.W. 1993. Exercises for bringing head and heart together. Training & Development, November: 17-19.
-
(1993)
Training & Development
, pp. 17-19
-
-
Watts, G.W.1
-
79
-
-
0001515156
-
Hindsight bias and strategic choice: Some problems in learning from experience
-
Bukszar, E., & Connolly, T. 1988. Hindsight bias and strategic choice: Some problems in learning from experience. Academy of Management Journal, 31(3): 628-641.
-
(1988)
Academy of Management Journal
, vol.31
, Issue.3
, pp. 628-641
-
-
Bukszar, E.1
Connolly, T.2
-
81
-
-
0001847103
-
Managing overconfidence
-
Winter
-
Russo, I.E., & Schoemaker, P.H.J. 1992. Managing overconfidence. Sloan Management Review, Winter: 7-17; Schwenk, C.R. 1984. Devil's advocacy in managerial decision-making. Journal of Management Studies, 21: 153-168.
-
(1992)
Sloan Management Review
, pp. 7-17
-
-
Russo, I.E.1
Schoemaker, P.H.J.2
-
82
-
-
84980226093
-
Devil's advocacy in managerial decision-making
-
Russo, I.E., & Schoemaker, P.H.J. 1992. Managing overconfidence. Sloan Management Review, Winter: 7-17; Schwenk, C.R. 1984. Devil's advocacy in managerial decision-making. Journal of Management Studies, 21: 153-168.
-
(1984)
Journal of Management Studies
, vol.21
, pp. 153-168
-
-
Schwenk, C.R.1
-
83
-
-
0000997046
-
Experiential effects of dialectical enquiry, devil's advocacy, and consensus approaches to strategic decision-making
-
Schweiger, D.M., Sandberg, W.R., & Rechner, P.L. 1989. Experiential effects of dialectical enquiry, devil's advocacy, and consensus approaches to strategic decision-making. Academy of Management Journal, 32: 745-772; Herbert, T.T., & Estes, R.W. 1977. Improving executive planning by formalising dissent: The corporate devil's advocate. Academy of Management Review, 2: 662-667; Rowan, op. cit., 196.
-
(1989)
Academy of Management Journal
, vol.32
, pp. 745-772
-
-
Schweiger, D.M.1
Sandberg, W.R.2
Rechner, P.L.3
-
84
-
-
0001258273
-
Improving executive planning by formalising dissent: The corporate devil's advocate
-
Schweiger, D.M., Sandberg, W.R., & Rechner, P.L. 1989. Experiential effects of dialectical enquiry, devil's advocacy, and consensus approaches to strategic decision-making. Academy of Management Journal, 32: 745-772; Herbert, T.T., & Estes, R.W. 1977. Improving executive planning by formalising dissent: The corporate devil's advocate. Academy of Management Review, 2: 662-667; Rowan, op. cit., 196.
-
(1977)
Academy of Management Review
, vol.2
, pp. 662-667
-
-
Herbert, T.T.1
Estes, R.W.2
-
85
-
-
8744307155
-
-
Schweiger, D.M., Sandberg, W.R., & Rechner, P.L. 1989. Experiential effects of dialectical enquiry, devil's advocacy, and consensus approaches to strategic decision-making. Academy of Management Journal, 32: 745-772; Herbert, T.T., & Estes, R.W. 1977. Improving executive planning by formalising dissent: The corporate devil's advocate. Academy of Management Review, 2: 662-667; Rowan, op. cit., 196.
-
Academy of Management Review
, pp. 196
-
-
Rowan1
-
86
-
-
11044221719
-
Decision-making in organizations
-
Clegg, S.R., Hardy, C., & Nord, W.R. (Eds). London: Sage
-
See, for example, Miller, S.J., Hickson, D.J., & Wilson, D.C. 1999. Decision-making in organizations. In Clegg, S.R., Hardy, C., & Nord, W.R. (Eds). Managing in organizations. Current issues. London: Sage; Klein, G. 2003. Intuition at work. New York: Century Doubleday; Davis & Davis, op. cit.
-
(1999)
Managing in Organizations. Current Issues
-
-
Miller, S.J.1
Hickson, D.J.2
Wilson, D.C.3
-
87
-
-
0038250526
-
-
New York: Century Doubleday
-
See, for example, Miller, S.J., Hickson, D.J., & Wilson, D.C. 1999. Decision-making in organizations. In Clegg, S.R., Hardy, C., & Nord, W.R. (Eds). Managing in organizations. Current issues. London: Sage; Klein, G. 2003. Intuition at work. New York: Century Doubleday; Davis & Davis, op. cit.
-
(2003)
Intuition at Work
-
-
Klein, G.1
-
88
-
-
11044226782
-
-
See, for example, Miller, S.J., Hickson, D.J., & Wilson, D.C. 1999. Decision-making in organizations. In Clegg, S.R., Hardy, C., & Nord, W.R. (Eds). Managing in organizations. Current issues. London: Sage; Klein, G. 2003. Intuition at work. New York: Century Doubleday; Davis & Davis, op. cit.
-
Intuition at Work
-
-
Davis1
Davis2
-
89
-
-
0040279425
-
Intuition in the development of scientific theory and practice
-
R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds). London: Routledge
-
Monsay (1997) cites the views of scholars including Carl Rogers and Jonas Salk in support of the assertion that under the right conditions intuition can be developed. See Monsay, E.H. 1997. Intuition in the development of scientific theory and practice. In R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds). Intuition: The inside story: Interdisciplinary perspectives. London: Routledge.
-
(1997)
Intuition: The Inside Story: Interdisciplinary Perspectives
-
-
Monsay, E.H.1
-
90
-
-
0041996882
-
Minds and managers: On the dual nature of human information processing and management
-
Taggart, W., & Robey, D. 1981. Minds and managers: On the dual nature of human information processing and management. Academy of Management Review, 6: 187-195.
-
(1981)
Academy of Management Review
, vol.6
, pp. 187-195
-
-
Taggart, W.1
Robey, D.2
-
91
-
-
0004069083
-
-
Cambridge, MA.: The MIT Press
-
For example, being mindful is an acute awareness of one's surroundings, thoughts, and behaviors and is engendered by pausing and examining our perceptions and feeling states as dispassionately as we can through a non-reactive awareness and openness to whatever the object of our attention is and our context, with the aim of avoiding premature cognitive commitment (see Austin, J.H. 1998. Zen and the brain. Cambridge, MA.: The MIT Press). Taggart described a quiet, relaxed mind that is focused upon the present moment and free of distracting fears and desires as the sine qua non of intuitive experience (1997, op. cit., 182). Intuitive flow experiences are discussed extensively in Csikszentmihalyi, M. 1992. Flow: The psychology of happiness. London: Rider.
-
(1998)
Zen and the Brain
-
-
Austin, J.H.1
-
92
-
-
11044229366
-
-
For example, being mindful is an acute awareness of one's surroundings, thoughts, and behaviors and is engendered by pausing and examining our perceptions and feeling states as dispassionately as we can through a non-reactive awareness and openness to whatever the object of our attention is and our context, with the aim of avoiding premature cognitive commitment (see Austin, J.H. 1998. Zen and the brain. Cambridge, MA.: The MIT Press). Taggart described a quiet, relaxed mind that is focused upon the present moment and free of distracting fears and desires as the sine qua non of intuitive experience (1997, op. cit., 182). Intuitive flow experiences are discussed extensively in Csikszentmihalyi, M. 1992. Flow: The psychology of happiness. London: Rider.
-
(1997)
Zen and the Brain
, pp. 182
-
-
-
93
-
-
0003575559
-
-
London: Rider
-
For example, being mindful is an acute awareness of one's surroundings, thoughts, and behaviors and is engendered by pausing and examining our perceptions and feeling states as dispassionately as we can through a non-reactive awareness and openness to whatever the object of our attention is and our context, with the aim of avoiding premature cognitive commitment (see Austin, J.H. 1998. Zen and the brain. Cambridge, MA.: The MIT Press). Taggart described a quiet, relaxed mind that is focused upon the present moment and free of distracting fears and desires as the sine qua non of intuitive experience (1997, op. cit., 182). Intuitive flow experiences are discussed extensively in Csikszentmihalyi, M. 1992. Flow: The psychology of happiness. London: Rider.
-
(1992)
Flow: The Psychology of Happiness
-
-
Csikszentmihalyi, M.1
-
96
-
-
62649109947
-
Developing intuition: Becoming smarter by thinking less
-
Academy of Management Annual Meeting, New Orleans, Louisiana, August 2004
-
Sadler-Smith, E, & Shefy, E. 2004. Developing intuition: Becoming smarter by thinking less. Academy of Management Best Paper Proceedings, Academy of Management Annual Meeting, New Orleans, Louisiana, August 2004.
-
(2004)
Academy of Management Best Paper Proceedings
-
-
Sadler-Smith, E.1
Shefy, E.2
-
97
-
-
0030627470
-
Managerial intuition: A conceptual and operational framework
-
January/February
-
Shapiro, S., & Spence, M.T. 1997. Managerial intuition: A conceptual and operational framework. Business Horizons, January/February: 63-68.
-
(1997)
Business Horizons
, pp. 63-68
-
-
Shapiro, S.1
Spence, M.T.2
-
99
-
-
0028489329
-
Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious
-
See Epstein, S. 1994. Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, 49: 709-724. Stanovich and West make a similar distinction between System 1 processes that are fast, heuristic, associative, and automatic and System 2 processes that are rule-based, slow, load heavily on working memory, and are controlled. See Stanovich, K.E., & West, R.F. 2000. Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23: 645-726.
-
(1994)
American Psychologist
, vol.49
, pp. 709-724
-
-
Epstein, S.1
-
100
-
-
0034488449
-
Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate?
-
See Epstein, S. 1994. Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, 49: 709-724. Stanovich and West make a similar distinction between System 1 processes that are fast, heuristic, associative, and automatic and System 2 processes that are rule-based, slow, load heavily on working memory, and are controlled. See Stanovich, K.E., & West, R.F. 2000. Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23: 645-726.
-
(2000)
Behavioral and Brain Sciences
, vol.23
, pp. 645-726
-
-
Stanovich, K.E.1
West, R.F.2
-
101
-
-
84909358155
-
The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment
-
Haidt, J. 2001. The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4): 814-834.
-
(2001)
Psychological Review
, vol.108
, Issue.4
, pp. 814-834
-
-
Haidt, J.1
-
102
-
-
84993881884
-
Psychological study of human judgment: Implications for investment decision-making
-
Slavic, P. 1972. Psychological study of human judgment: Implications for investment decision-making. The Journal of Finance, 27(4): 779-799.
-
(1972)
The Journal of Finance
, vol.27
, Issue.4
, pp. 779-799
-
-
Slavic, P.1
-
103
-
-
0008609383
-
The nature of intuition: A neuro-psychological approach
-
R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds). London: Routledge
-
Writers such as the anthropologist Charles Laughlin attribute this dichotomy to the historical association of intuition with metaphysics and religion. In the Enlightenment and beyond, religion became the nemesis of science, and hence the 'intuitional baby' was thrown out with the 'metaphysical bath-water.' See Laughlin, C. 1997. The nature of intuition: A neuro-psychological approach. In R. Davis-Floyd & P. Sven Arvidson (Eds). Intuition the inside story: Interdisciplinary perspectives. London: Routledge.
-
(1997)
Intuition the Inside Story: Interdisciplinary Perspectives
-
-
Laughlin, C.1
|