메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 18, Issue 9, 2001, Pages 2255-2266

Inefficient visual search for second-order motion

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords

ARTICLE; CONTRAST SENSITIVITY; HUMAN; LIGHT; METHODOLOGY; MOVEMENT PERCEPTION; PHOTOSTIMULATION; PHYSIOLOGY; PSYCHOLOGICAL MODEL; VISION;

EID: 0035458646     PISSN: 10847529     EISSN: 15208532     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1364/JOSAA.18.002255     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (35)

References (58)
  • 1
    • 0022521836 scopus 로고
    • Serial and parallel processing of visual feature conjunctions
    • K. Nakayama and G. H. Silverman, “Serial and parallel processing of visual feature conjunctions,” Nature 320, 264-265 (1986).
    • (1986) Nature , vol.320 , pp. 264-265
    • Nakayama, K.1    Silverman, G.H.2
  • 2
    • 0026461955 scopus 로고
    • Are direction and speed coded independently by the visual system? Evi-dence from visual search
    • J. Driver, P. McLeod, and Z. Dienes, “Are direction and speed coded independently by the visual system? Evi-dence from visual search,” Spatial Vision 6, 133-147 (1992).
    • (1992) Spatial Vision , vol.6 , pp. 133-147
    • Driver, J.1    McLeod, P.2    Dienes, Z.3
  • 3
    • 0027719840 scopus 로고
    • The conspicuousness of orientation and motion contrast
    • H. C. Nothdurft, “The conspicuousness of orientation and motion contrast,” Spatial Vision 7, 341-363 (1993).
    • (1993) Spatial Vision , vol.7 , pp. 341-363
    • Nothdurft, H.C.1
  • 4
    • 0018878142 scopus 로고
    • A feature-integration theory of attention
    • A. M. Treisman and G. Gelade, “A feature-integration theory of attention,” Cogn. Psychol. 12, 97-136 (1980).
    • (1980) Cogn. Psychol , vol.12 , pp. 97-136
    • Treisman, A.M.1    Gelade, G.2
  • 5
    • 0026210630 scopus 로고
    • Search, similarity, and integration of features between and within dimensions
    • A. Treisman, “Search, similarity, and integration of features between and within dimensions,” J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percep. Perform. 17, 652-676 (1991).
    • (1991) J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percep. Perform , vol.17 , pp. 652-676
    • Treisman, A.1
  • 6
    • 84864888849 scopus 로고
    • Guided search 2.0: A revised model of visual search
    • J. M. Wolfe, “Guided search 2.0: A revised model of visual search,” Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1, 202-238 (1994).
    • (1994) Psychon. Bull. Rev , vol.1 , pp. 202-238
    • Wolfe, J.M.1
  • 7
    • 76549154317 scopus 로고
    • The mechanism of directionally selective units in rabbits retina
    • H. B. Barlow and W. R. Levick, “The mechanism of directionally selective units in rabbit’s retina,” J. Physiol. (London) 178, 477-504 (1965).
    • (1965) J. Physiol. (London) , vol.178 , pp. 477-504
    • Barlow, H.B.1    Levick, W.R.2
  • 8
    • 33645410496 scopus 로고
    • Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cats visual cortex
    • D. H. Hubel and T. N. Wiesel, “Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat’s visual cortex,” J. Physiol. (London) 160, 106-154 (1962).
    • (1962) J. Physiol. (London) , vol.160 , pp. 106-154
    • Hubel, D.H.1    Wiesel, T.N.2
  • 9
    • 0022014331 scopus 로고
    • Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion
    • E. H. Adelson and J. R. Bergen, “Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 2, 284-299 (1985).
    • (1985) J. Opt. Soc. Am. A , vol.2 , pp. 284-299
    • Adelson, E.H.1    Bergen, J.R.2
  • 11
    • 0022014327 scopus 로고
    • Model of human visual-motion sensing
    • A. B. Watson and A. J. Ahumada, “Model of human visual-motion sensing,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 2, 322-342 (1985).
    • (1985) J. Opt. Soc. Am. A , vol.2 , pp. 322-342
    • Watson, A.B.1    Ahumada, A.J.2
  • 12
    • 0024893590 scopus 로고
    • Motion: The long and short of it
    • P. Cavanagh and G. Mather, “Motion: The long and short of it,” Spatial Vision 4, 103-129 1989).
    • (1989) Spatial Vision , vol.4 , pp. 103-129
    • Cavanagh, P.1    Mather, G.2
  • 13
    • 0024117651 scopus 로고
    • Drift-balanced random stimuli: A general basis for studying non-Fourier motion perception
    • C. Chubb and G. Sperling, “Drift-balanced random stimuli: A general basis for studying non-Fourier motion perception,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 5, 1986-2007 (1988).
    • (1988) J. Opt. Soc. Am. A , vol.5 , pp. 1986-2007
    • Chubb, C.1    Sperling, G.2
  • 14
    • 0027066880 scopus 로고
    • A computa-tional model of the analysis of some first-order and second-order motion patterns by simple and complex cells
    • A. Johnston, P. W. McOwan, and H. Buxton, “A computa-tional model of the analysis of some first-order and second-order motion patterns by simple and complex cells,” Proc. R. Soc. London Ser. B 250, 297-306 (1992).
    • (1992) Proc. R. Soc. London Ser. B , vol.250 , pp. 297-306
    • Johnston, A.1    McOwan, P.W.2    Buxton, H.3
  • 15
    • 0027290796 scopus 로고
    • Discriminating the direction of second-order motion at short stimulus durations
    • A. M. Derrington, D. R. Badcock, and G. B. Henning, “Discriminating the direction of second-order motion at short stimulus durations,” Vision Res. 33, 1785-1794 (1993).
    • (1993) Vision Res , vol.33 , pp. 1785-1794
    • Derrington, A.M.1    Badcock, D.R.2    Henning, G.B.3
  • 16
    • 0026875299 scopus 로고
    • Immobility of some second-order stimuli in human peripheral vision
    • A. Pantle, “Immobility of some second-order stimuli in human peripheral vision,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 9, 863-867 (1992).
    • (1992) J. Opt. Soc. Am. A , vol.9 , pp. 863-867
    • Pantle, A.1
  • 17
    • 0031178550 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Second-order motion perception in the peripheral visual field
    • J. M. Zanker, “Second-order motion perception in the peripheral visual field,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 14, 1385-1392 (1997).
    • (1997) J. Opt. Soc. Am. A , vol.14 , pp. 1385-1392
    • Zanker, J.M.1
  • 18
    • 0028372385 scopus 로고
    • Direction identification thresholds for second-order motion in central and peripheral vision
    • A. T. Smith, R. F. Hess, and C. L. Baker, “Direction identification thresholds for second-order motion in central and peripheral vision,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 11, 506-514 (1994).
    • (1994) J. Opt. Soc. Am. A , vol.11 , pp. 506-514
    • Smith, A.T.1    Hess, R.F.2    Baker, C.L.3
  • 19
    • 0028970441 scopus 로고
    • 1st-and 2nd-order motion and texture resolution in central and peripheral vision
    • J. A. Solomon and G. Sperling, “1st-and 2nd-order motion and texture resolution in central and peripheral vision,” Vision Res. 35, 59-64 (1995).
    • (1995) Vision Res , vol.35 , pp. 59-64
    • Solomon, J.A.1    Sperling, G.2
  • 20
    • 0031697921 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Position displacement, not velocity, is the cue to motion detection of second-order stimuli
    • A. E. Seiffert and P. Cavanagh, “Position displacement, not velocity, is the cue to motion detection of second-order stimuli,” Vision Res. 38, 3569-3582 (1998).
    • (1998) Vision Res , vol.38 , pp. 3569-3582
    • Seiffert, A.E.1    Cavanagh, P.2
  • 21
    • 0032859403 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Position-based motion perception for color and texture stimuli: Effects of contrast and speed
    • A. E. Seiffert and P. Cavanagh, “Position-based motion perception for color and texture stimuli: Effects of contrast and speed,” Vision Res. 39, 4172-4185 (1999).
    • (1999) Vision Res , vol.39 , pp. 4172-4185
    • Seiffert, A.E.1    Cavanagh, P.2
  • 22
    • 0033120320 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Second-order motion discrimination by feature-tracking
    • A. M. Derrington and O. I. Ukkonen, “Second-order motion discrimination by feature-tracking,” Vision Res. 39, 1465-1475 (1999).
    • (1999) Vision Res , vol.39 , pp. 1465-1475
    • Derrington, A.M.1    Ukkonen, O.I.2
  • 23
    • 0033777209 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Motion of contrast-modulated gratings is analysed by different mechanisms at low and at high contrasts
    • O. I. Ukkonen and A. M. Derrington, “Motion of contrast-modulated gratings is analysed by different mechanisms at low and at high contrasts,” Vision Res. 40, 3359-3371 (2000).
    • (2000) Vision Res , vol.40 , pp. 3359-3371
    • Ukkonen, O.I.1    Derrington, A.M.2
  • 24
    • 0023199398 scopus 로고
    • Detecting conjunctions of color and form: Reassessing the serial search hypothesis
    • H. Pashler, “Detecting conjunctions of color and form: Reassessing the serial search hypothesis,” Percept. Psycho-phys. 41, 191-201 (1987).
    • (1987) Percept. Psycho-Phys , vol.41 , pp. 191-201
    • Pashler, H.1
  • 25
    • 0000865969 scopus 로고
    • The iconic bottleneck and the tenuous link between early visual processing and perception
    • C. Blakemore, ed. (Cambridge U. Press, Cambridge, UK
    • K. Nakayama, “The iconic bottleneck and the tenuous link between early visual processing and perception,” in Vision: Coding and Efficiency, C. Blakemore, ed. (Cambridge U. Press, Cambridge, UK, 1990), pp. 411-422.
    • (1990) Vision: Coding and Efficiency , pp. 411-422
    • Nakayama, K.1
  • 26
    • 0018055314 scopus 로고
    • Attention demands of visual search
    • G. D. Logan, “Attention demands of visual search,” Memory Cogn. 6, 446-453 (1978).
    • (1978) Memory Cogn , vol.6 , pp. 446-453
    • Logan, G.D.1
  • 27
    • 84970101059 scopus 로고
    • Serial vs. Parallel processing: Sometimes they look like Tweedledum and Tweedledee, but they can (and should) be distinguished
    • J. T. Townsend, “Serial vs. parallel processing: Sometimes they look like Tweedledum and Tweedledee, but they can (and should) be distinguished,” Psychol. Sci. 1, 46-54 (1990).
    • (1990) Psychol. Sci , vol.1 , pp. 46-54
    • Townsend, J.T.1
  • 28
    • 0001044213 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Attention, pattern recognition, and pop-out in visual search
    • R. Parasuraman, ed. (MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass
    • K. Nakayama and J. S. Joseph, “Attention, pattern recognition, and pop-out in visual search,” in The Attentive Brain, R. Parasuraman, ed. (MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass., 1998), pp. 279-298.
    • (1998) The Attentive Brain , pp. 279-298
    • Nakayama, K.1    Joseph, J.S.2
  • 29
    • 0026706721 scopus 로고
    • Attention-based motion perception
    • P. Cavanagh, “Attention-based motion perception,” Science 257, 1563-1565 (1992).
    • (1992) Science , vol.257 , pp. 1563-1565
    • Cavanagh, P.1
  • 30
    • 0023395885 scopus 로고
    • Equiluminance: Spatial and temporal factors and the contribution of blue-sensitive cones
    • P. Cavanagh, D. I. MacLeod, and S. M. Anstis, “Equiluminance: Spatial and temporal factors and the contribution of blue-sensitive cones,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 4, 1428-1438 (1987).
    • (1987) J. Opt. Soc. Am. A , vol.4 , pp. 1428-1438
    • Cavanagh, P.1    Macleod, D.I.2    Anstis, S.M.3
  • 31
    • 0030075387 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Just say no: How are visual searches terminated when there is no target present?
    • M. M. Chun and J. M. Wolfe, “Just say no: How are visual searches terminated when there is no target present?” Cogn. Psychol. 30, 39-78 (1996).
    • (1996) Cogn. Psychol , vol.30 , pp. 39-78
    • Chun, M.M.1    Wolfe, J.M.2
  • 32
    • 0029112977 scopus 로고
    • Combining speed information across space
    • P. Verghese and L. S. Stone, “Combining speed information across space,” Vision Res. 35, 2811-2823 (1995).
    • (1995) Vision Res , vol.35 , pp. 2811-2823
    • Verghese, P.1    Stone, L.S.2
  • 33
    • 0031040473 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Spatial layout affects speed discrimination
    • P. Verghese and L. S. Stone, “Spatial layout affects speed discrimination,” Vision Res. 37, 397-406 (1997).
    • (1997) Vision Res , vol.37 , pp. 397-406
    • Verghese, P.1    Stone, L.S.2
  • 34
    • 0034679969 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Attention is fast but volition is slow
    • G. M. Wolfe, G. A. Alvarez, and T. S. Horowitz, “Attention is fast but volition is slow,” Nature 406, 691 (2000).
    • (2000) Nature , vol.406 , pp. 691
    • Wolfe, G.M.1    Alvarez, G.A.2    Horowitz, T.S.3
  • 35
    • 0033606943 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Electrophysiological measurements of rapid shifts of attention during visual search
    • G. F. Woodman and S. J. Luck, “Electrophysiological measurements of rapid shifts of attention during visual search,” Nature 400, 867-869 (1999).
    • (1999) Nature , vol.400 , pp. 867-869
    • Woodman, G.F.1    Luck, S.J.2
  • 36
    • 0001487015 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Are attentional dwell times inconsistent with serial visual search?
    • C. M. Moore, H. Egeth, L. R. Berglan, and S. J. Luck, “Are attentional dwell times inconsistent with serial visual search?” Psychon. Bull. Rev. 3, 360-365 (1996).
    • (1996) Psychon. Bull. Rev , vol.3 , pp. 360-365
    • Moore, C.M.1    Egeth, H.2    Berglan, L.R.3    Luck, S.J.4
  • 37
    • 0347669501 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • What can 1 million trials tell us about visual search?
    • J. M. Wolfe, “What can 1 million trials tell us about visual search?” Psychol. Sci. 9, 33-39 (1998).
    • (1998) Psychol. Sci , vol.9 , pp. 33-39
    • Wolfe, J.M.1
  • 38
    • 0025084195 scopus 로고
    • Motion at isoluminance: Discrimination/detection ratios for moving isoluminant gratings
    • D. T. Lindsey and D. Y. Teller, “Motion at isoluminance: Discrimination/detection ratios for moving isoluminant gratings,” Vision Res. 30, 1751-1761 (1990).
    • (1990) Vision Res , vol.30 , pp. 1751-1761
    • Lindsey, D.T.1    Teller, D.Y.2
  • 39
    • 0027538237 scopus 로고
    • Detecting and dis-criminating the direction of motion of luminance and colour gratings
    • A. M. Derrington and G. B. Henning, “Detecting and dis-criminating the direction of motion of luminance and colour gratings,” Vision Res. 33, 799-811 (1993).
    • (1993) Vision Res , vol.33 , pp. 799-811
    • Derrington, A.M.1    Henning, G.B.2
  • 40
    • 0025013008 scopus 로고
    • Motion mechanisms have only limited access to form information
    • J. D. Victor and M. M. Conte, “Motion mechanisms have only limited access to form information,” Vision Res. 30, 289-301 (1990).
    • (1990) Vision Res , vol.30 , pp. 289-301
    • Victor, J.D.1    Conte, M.M.2
  • 41
    • 0033990477 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Attention mecha-nisms for multi-location first-and second-order motion per-ception
    • Z.-L. Lu, C. Q. Liu, and B. A. Dosher, “Attention mecha-nisms for multi-location first-and second-order motion per-ception,” Vision Res. 40, 173-186 (2000).
    • (2000) Vision Res , vol.40 , pp. 173-186
    • Lu, Z.-L.1    Liu, C.Q.2    Dosher, B.A.3
  • 42
    • 0030265621 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Motion transparency arises from perceptual grouping: Evidence from luminance and contrast modulation motion displays
    • P. W. McOwan and A. Johnston, “Motion transparency arises from perceptual grouping: Evidence from luminance and contrast modulation motion displays,” Curr. Biol. 6, 1343-1346 (1996).
    • (1996) Curr. Biol , vol.6 , pp. 1343-1346
    • McOwan, P.W.1    Johnston, A.2
  • 43
    • 0024834511 scopus 로고
    • Kinetic depth effect and optic flow: I. 3D shape from Fourier motion
    • B. A. Dosher, M. S. Landy, and G. Sperling, “Kinetic depth effect and optic flow: I. 3D shape from Fourier motion,” Vision Res. 29, 1789-1813 (1989).
    • (1989) Vision Res , vol.29 , pp. 1789-1813
    • Dosher, B.A.1    Landy, M.S.2    Sperling, G.3
  • 44
    • 0033981454 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Slow discrimination of contrast-defined expansion patterns
    • H. A. Allen and A. M. Derrington, “Slow discrimination of contrast-defined expansion patterns,” Vision Res. 40, 735-744 (2000).
    • (2000) Vision Res , vol.40 , pp. 735-744
    • Allen, H.A.1    Derrington, A.M.2
  • 45
    • 0027207549 scopus 로고
    • Segmentation versus integration in visual motion processing
    • O. J. Braddick, “Segmentation versus integration in visual motion processing,” Trends Neurosci. 16, 263-268 (1993).
    • (1993) Trends Neurosci , vol.16 , pp. 263-268
    • Braddick, O.J.1
  • 46
    • 0026086791 scopus 로고
    • The kinetic depth effect and optic flow: II. First-and second-order motion
    • M. S. Landy, B. A. Dosher, G. Sperling, and M. E. Perkins, “The kinetic depth effect and optic flow: II. First-and second-order motion,” Vision Res. 31, 859-876 (1991).
    • (1991) Vision Res , vol.31 , pp. 859-876
    • Landy, M.S.1    Dosher, B.A.2    Sperling, G.3    Perkins, M.E.4
  • 48
    • 0031855360 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Second-order motions contribute to vection
    • R. Gurnsey, D. Fleet, and C. Potechin, “Second-order motions contribute to vection,” Vision Res. 38, 2801-2816 (1998).
    • (1998) Vision Res , vol.38 , pp. 2801-2816
    • Gurnsey, R.1    Fleet, D.2    Potechin, C.3
  • 49
    • 0027023547 scopus 로고
    • Motion defined exclusively by second-order characteristics does not evoke optokinetic nystagmus
    • L. R. Harris and A. T. Smith, “Motion defined exclusively by second-order characteristics does not evoke optokinetic nystagmus,” Visual Neurosci. 9, 565-570 (1992).
    • (1992) Visual Neurosci , vol.9 , pp. 565-570
    • Harris, L.R.1    Smith, A.T.2
  • 51
    • 0029090094 scopus 로고
    • The functional architecture of human visual motion perception
    • Z. L. Lu and G. Sperling, “The functional architecture of human visual motion perception,” Vision Res. 35, 2697-2722 (1995).
    • (1995) Vision Res , vol.35 , pp. 2697-2722
    • Lu, Z.L.1    Sperling, G.2
  • 52
    • 0025427025 scopus 로고
    • Dissociation of short-and long-range apparent motion in visual search
    • R. B. Ivry and A. Cohen, “Dissociation of short-and long-range apparent motion in visual search,” J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 16, 317-331 (1990).
    • (1990) J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform , vol.16 , pp. 317-331
    • Ivry, R.B.1    Cohen, A.2
  • 53
    • 0028672064 scopus 로고
    • Attention and apparent motion
    • T. Horowitz and A. Treisman, “Attention and apparent motion,” Spatial Vision 8, 193-219 (1994).
    • (1994) Spatial Vision , vol.8 , pp. 193-219
    • Horowitz, T.1    Treisman, A.2
  • 54
    • 0019319473 scopus 로고
    • Low-level and high-level processes in apparent motion
    • O. J. Braddick, “Low-level and high-level processes in apparent motion,” Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London Ser. B 290, 137-151 (1980).
    • (1980) Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London Ser. B , vol.290 , pp. 137-151
    • Braddick, O.J.1
  • 55
    • 0028068986 scopus 로고
    • Different processes underlie the detection of second-order motion at low and high temporal frequencies
    • I. E. Holliday and S. J. Anderson, “Different processes underlie the detection of second-order motion at low and high temporal frequencies,” Proc. R. Soc. London Ser. B 257, 165-173 (1994).
    • (1994) Proc. R. Soc. London Ser. B , vol.257 , pp. 165-173
    • Holliday, I.E.1    Anderson, S.J.2
  • 56
    • 0033046172 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Does early nonlinearity account for second-order motion?
    • N. E. Scott-Samuel and M. A. Georgeson, “Does early nonlinearity account for second-order motion?,” Vision Res. 39, 2853-2865 (1999).
    • (1999) Vision Res , vol.39 , pp. 2853-2865
    • Scott-Samuel, N.E.1    Georgeson, M.A.2
  • 57
    • 0026211606 scopus 로고
    • Spatial summation properties of directionally selective mechanisms in human vision
    • S. J. Anderson and D. C. Burr, “Spatial summation properties of directionally selective mechanisms in human vision,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 8, 1330-1339 (1991).
    • (1991) J. Opt. Soc. Am. A , vol.8 , pp. 1330-1339
    • Anderson, S.J.1    Burr, D.C.2
  • 58
    • 0033136355 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Neuronal responses to orientation and motion contrast in cat striate cortex
    • S. Kastner, H. C. Nothdurft, and I. N. Pigarev, “Neuronal responses to orientation and motion contrast in cat striate cortex,” Visual Neurosci. 16, 587-600 (1999).
    • (1999) Visual Neurosci , vol.16 , pp. 587-600
    • Kastner, S.1    Nothdurft, H.C.2    Pigarev, I.N.3


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.