메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 281, Issue 5380, 1998, Pages 1185-1187

Making memories: Brain activity that predicts how well visual experience will be remembered

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords

ARTICLE; BRAIN DEPTH STIMULATION; CONTROLLED STUDY; EVENT RELATED POTENTIAL; EXPERIENCE; FRONTAL LOBE; HIPPOCAMPUS; HUMAN; HUMAN EXPERIMENT; NUCLEAR MAGNETIC RESONANCE IMAGING; PHOTOGRAPHY; PREFRONTAL CORTEX; PRIORITY JOURNAL; TEMPORAL LOBE; VISUAL MEMORY;

EID: 0032555613     PISSN: 00368075     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1126/science.281.5380.1185     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (939)

References (40)
  • 2
    • 0019163156 scopus 로고
    • W. B. Scoville and B. Milner, J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 20, 11 (1957); N. J. Cohen and L. R. Squire, Science 210, 207 (1980).
    • (1980) Science , vol.210 , pp. 207
    • Cohen, N.J.1    Squire, L.R.2
  • 8
    • 0026512326 scopus 로고
    • L. R. Squire et al., ibid. 89, 1837 (1992); M. E. Raichle et al., Cereb. Cortex 4, 8 (1994); D. L. Schacter, N. M Alpert, C. R. Savage, S. L. Rauch, M. S. Albert, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 321 (1996); J. D. E. Gabrieli, J. E. Desmond, J. B. Demb, A. D. Wagner, Psychol. Sci. 7, 278, (1996); R. L. Buckner et al., Neuron 20, 285 (1998).
    • (1992) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. , vol.89 , pp. 1837
    • Squire, L.R.1
  • 9
    • 0028040173 scopus 로고
    • L. R. Squire et al., ibid. 89, 1837 (1992); M. E. Raichle et al., Cereb. Cortex 4, 8 (1994); D. L. Schacter, N. M Alpert, C. R. Savage, S. L. Rauch, M. S. Albert, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 321 (1996); J. D. E. Gabrieli, J. E. Desmond, J. B. Demb, A. D. Wagner, Psychol. Sci. 7, 278, (1996); R. L. Buckner et al., Neuron 20, 285 (1998).
    • (1994) Cereb. Cortex , vol.4 , pp. 8
    • Raichle, M.E.1
  • 10
    • 0030030851 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • L. R. Squire et al., ibid. 89, 1837 (1992); M. E. Raichle et al., Cereb. Cortex 4, 8 (1994); D. L. Schacter, N. M Alpert, C. R. Savage, S. L. Rauch, M. S. Albert, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 321 (1996); J. D. E. Gabrieli, J. E. Desmond, J. B. Demb, A. D. Wagner, Psychol. Sci. 7, 278, (1996); R. L. Buckner et al., Neuron 20, 285 (1998).
    • (1996) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. , vol.93 , pp. 321
    • Schacter, D.L.1    Alpert, N.M.2    Savage, C.R.3    Rauch, S.L.4    Albert, M.S.5
  • 11
    • 0006053794 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • L. R. Squire et al., ibid. 89, 1837 (1992); M. E. Raichle et al., Cereb. Cortex 4, 8 (1994); D. L. Schacter, N. M Alpert, C. R. Savage, S. L. Rauch, M. S. Albert, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 321 (1996); J. D. E. Gabrieli, J. E. Desmond, J. B. Demb, A. D. Wagner, Psychol. Sci. 7, 278, (1996); R. L. Buckner et al., Neuron 20, 285 (1998).
    • (1996) Psychol. Sci. , vol.7 , pp. 278
    • Gabrieli, J.D.E.1    Desmond, J.E.2    Demb, J.B.3    Wagner, A.D.4
  • 12
    • 0032005464 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • L. R. Squire et al., ibid. 89, 1837 (1992); M. E. Raichle et al., Cereb. Cortex 4, 8 (1994); D. L. Schacter, N. M Alpert, C. R. Savage, S. L. Rauch, M. S. Albert, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 321 (1996); J. D. E. Gabrieli, J. E. Desmond, J. B. Demb, A. D. Wagner, Psychol. Sci. 7, 278, (1996); R. L. Buckner et al., Neuron 20, 285 (1998).
    • (1998) Neuron , vol.20 , pp. 285
    • Buckner, R.L.1
  • 13
    • 3542997551 scopus 로고
    • T. F. Sanquist et al., Psychophysiology 23, 298 (1984); K. A. Paller, M. Klutas, A. R. Mayes, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 67, 360 (1987); M. D. Rugg and M. G. H. Coles, Eds. Electrophysiology of Mind: Event-Related Brain Potentials and Cognition (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 1995).
    • (1984) Psychophysiology , vol.23 , pp. 298
    • Sanquist, T.F.1
  • 14
    • 0023461067 scopus 로고
    • T. F. Sanquist et al., Psychophysiology 23, 298 (1984); K. A. Paller, M. Klutas, A. R. Mayes, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 67, 360 (1987); M. D. Rugg and M. G. H. Coles, Eds. Electrophysiology of Mind: Event-Related Brain Potentials and Cognition (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 1995).
    • (1987) Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. , vol.67 , pp. 360
    • Paller, K.A.1    Klutas, M.2    Mayes, A.R.3
  • 17
    • 0031932352 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • 2 in-plane and 7.0-mm through-plane resolution. The anterior slice was positioned 14 mm posterior to the head of the hippocampal formation. Structural images were acquired with a standard T1-weighted pulse sequence. Head movement was minimized by means of a bite-bar formed with each subject's dental impression, and motion artifact was examined and corrected automatically for all scans with Air 3.0 [R. P. Woods, S. R. Cherry, J. C. Mazziota, J. Comput. Assist. Tomogr. 16, 620 (1992)].
    • (1998) Magn. Reson. Med. , vol.39 , pp. 361
    • Glover, G.H.1    Lai, S.2
  • 18
    • 0026776405 scopus 로고
    • 2 in-plane and 7.0-mm through-plane resolution. The anterior slice was positioned 14 mm posterior to the head of the hippocampal formation. Structural images were acquired with a standard T1-weighted pulse sequence. Head movement was minimized by means of a bite-bar formed with each subject's dental impression, and motion artifact was examined and corrected automatically for all scans with Air 3.0 [R. P. Woods, S. R. Cherry, J. C. Mazziota, J. Comput. Assist. Tomogr. 16, 620 (1992)].
    • (1992) J. Comput. Assist. Tomogr. , vol.16 , pp. 620
    • Woods, R.P.1    Cherry, S.R.2    Mazziota, J.C.3
  • 19
    • 3543046827 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Pictures were presented for a period of 2.88 s with an intertrial interval of 12.96 s during which a fixation cross was presented. This interval allowed the fMRI signal to rise in response to each picture presentation and to return to baseline before the next picture presentation. Thus, a separate fMRI response could be measured for each picture. Subjects (three women and three men, aged 22 to 32 years) were instructed to respond as quickly and accurately as possible and to focus on the fixation cross between picture presentations. Responses and reaction times were recorded by means of a button box with optic switches. Subjects pressed one button for indoor scenes and another for outdoor scenes.
  • 20
    • 3543031119 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Participants were instructed to view pictures presented on a computer screen and to judge if each picture had been seen during scanning. Each picture remained on the screen until the participant made a response. For pictures judged as seen during scanning, a question would appear on the screen asking the basis of that recognition. Subjects were to respond "remember" if the recognition of the picture was based on a conscious awareness of some aspect or aspects of what was experienced at the time the item was presented (for example, aspects of the physical appearance of the scene, or what one was thinking when encountering the item). Subjects were to respond "know" when recognition was made without conscious recollection of any particular aspects of its previous occurrence. As an example, participants were told that "know" was similar to what they would sense upon recognizing someone in the street without being able to recollect anything about the person.
  • 21
    • 0029997609 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • There were 22 scans of brain activity per trial. Individual scans in each trial were assigned to baseline fixation or to picture classification phases on the basis of a lag in peak hemodynamic response of ∼4 s after the presentation of the stimulus [D. Malonek and A. Grinvald, Science 272, 551 (1996)]. Scans 1 through 6 of each trial measured activation in response to baseline fixation. Scans 10 through 15 measured activation in response to picture classification. The first statistical map, which revealed areas responding to picture presentation, compared signal from baseline scans to that of picture-classification scans for each voxel by means of a nonparametric Kolmogorov-Smirnov statistic. The second statistical map, which revealed areas predicting subsequent memory, was created as follows: For each trial, the average value from baseline scans was subtracted from scans 10 through 15. Subtracted values were integrated to measure each voxel's event-related response. A Kendall's rank-order correlation was calculated for each voxel between event-related responses and subsequent memory classification of remembered, familiar, or forgotten. Correlation coefficients were transformed into z scores. For the composite maps, the structural MRI scans were normalized into the same space to allow for the superimposition of statistical maps averaged across subjects onto an averaged structural image. The averaged activation maps were intensity thresholded at P < 0.01, one-tailed, and each slice was subjected to a cluster analysis procedure [J. Xiong, J.-H. Gao, J. L. Lancaster, P. T. Fox Hum. Brain Mapping 3, 287 (1995)] to correct for multiple statistical comparisons by means of a spatial extent threshold that yielded a P < 0.01, one-tailed, significance level over the entire image. Significant voxels were colored according to their level of significance and were overlaid on the averaged structural image. Regions where activity correlated positively with subsequent memory were examined on brain images normalized to the stereotactic space of Talairach and Tournoux [J. Talairach and P. Tournoux, Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain (Thieme, New York, 1988)]. Coordinates for the activations are as follows: right frontal (40, 15, 32); right parahippocampal in slice 2 (22, -35, -15) and slice 4 (24, -48, -8); and left parahippocampal in slice 1 (-34, -29, -18), slice 2 (-33, -34, -13), slice 3 (-27, -40, -9), and slice 4 (-29, -46, -6).
    • (1996) Science , vol.272 , pp. 551
    • Malonek, D.1    Grinvald, A.2
  • 22
    • 0005153275 scopus 로고
    • There were 22 scans of brain activity per trial. Individual scans in each trial were assigned to baseline fixation or to picture classification phases on the basis of a lag in peak hemodynamic response of ∼4 s after the presentation of the stimulus [D. Malonek and A. Grinvald, Science 272, 551 (1996)]. Scans 1 through 6 of each trial measured activation in response to baseline fixation. Scans 10 through 15 measured activation in response to picture classification. The first statistical map, which revealed areas responding to picture presentation, compared signal from baseline scans to that of picture-classification scans for each voxel by means of a nonparametric Kolmogorov-Smirnov statistic. The second statistical map, which revealed areas predicting subsequent memory, was created as follows: For each trial, the average value from baseline scans was subtracted from scans 10 through 15. Subtracted values were integrated to measure each voxel's event-related response. A Kendall's rank-order correlation was calculated for each voxel between event-related responses and subsequent memory classification of remembered, familiar, or forgotten. Correlation coefficients were transformed into z scores. For the composite maps, the structural MRI scans were normalized into the same space to allow for the superimposition of statistical maps averaged across subjects onto an averaged structural image. The averaged activation maps were intensity thresholded at P < 0.01, one-tailed, and each slice was subjected to a cluster analysis procedure [J. Xiong, J.-H. Gao, J. L. Lancaster, P. T. Fox Hum. Brain Mapping 3, 287 (1995)] to correct for multiple statistical comparisons by means of a spatial extent threshold that yielded a P < 0.01, one-tailed, significance level over the entire image. Significant voxels were colored according to their level of significance and were overlaid on the averaged structural image. Regions where activity correlated positively with subsequent memory were examined on brain images normalized to the stereotactic space of Talairach and Tournoux [J. Talairach and P. Tournoux, Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain (Thieme, New York, 1988)]. Coordinates for the activations are as follows: right frontal (40, 15, 32); right parahippocampal in slice 2 (22, -35, -15) and slice 4 (24, -48, -8); and left parahippocampal in slice 1 (-34, -29, -18), slice 2 (-33, -34, -13), slice 3 (-27, -40, -9), and slice 4 (-29, -46, -6).
    • (1995) Hum. Brain Mapping , vol.3 , pp. 287
    • Xiong, J.1    Gao, J.-H.2    Lancaster, J.L.3    Fox, P.T.4
  • 23
    • 0003415335 scopus 로고
    • Thieme, New York
    • There were 22 scans of brain activity per trial. Individual scans in each trial were assigned to baseline fixation or to picture classification phases on the basis of a lag in peak hemodynamic response of ∼4 s after the presentation of the stimulus [D. Malonek and A. Grinvald, Science 272, 551 (1996)]. Scans 1 through 6 of each trial measured activation in response to baseline fixation. Scans 10 through 15 measured activation in response to picture classification. The first statistical map, which revealed areas responding to picture presentation, compared signal from baseline scans to that of picture-classification scans for each voxel by means of a nonparametric Kolmogorov-Smirnov statistic. The second statistical map, which revealed areas predicting subsequent memory, was created as follows: For each trial, the average value from baseline scans was subtracted from scans 10 through 15. Subtracted values were integrated to measure each voxel's event-related response. A Kendall's rank-order correlation was calculated for each voxel between event-related responses and subsequent memory classification of remembered, familiar, or forgotten. Correlation coefficients were transformed into z scores. For the composite maps, the structural MRI scans were normalized into the same space to allow for the superimposition of statistical maps averaged across subjects onto an averaged structural image. The averaged activation maps were intensity thresholded at P < 0.01, one-tailed, and each slice was subjected to a cluster analysis procedure [J. Xiong, J.-H. Gao, J. L. Lancaster, P. T. Fox Hum. Brain Mapping 3, 287 (1995)] to correct for multiple statistical comparisons by means of a spatial extent threshold that yielded a P < 0.01, one-tailed, significance level over the entire image. Significant voxels were colored according to their level of significance and were overlaid on the averaged structural image. Regions where activity correlated positively with subsequent memory were examined on brain images normalized to the stereotactic space of Talairach and Tournoux [J. Talairach and P. Tournoux, Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain (Thieme, New York, 1988)]. Coordinates for the activations are as follows: right frontal (40, 15, 32); right parahippocampal in slice 2 (22, -35, -15) and slice 4 (24, -48, -8); and left parahippocampal in slice 1 (-34, -29, -18), slice 2 (-33, -34, -13), slice 3 (-27, -40, -9), and slice 4 (-29, -46, -6).
    • (1988) Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain
    • Talairach, J.1    Tournoux, P.2
  • 24
    • 3543028151 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • For each subject, regions of interest were drawn around parahippocampal activations showing significant positive correlation with subsequent memory. Event-related responses (calculated for each voxel as described above) were averaged in the activated regions for pictures that were subsequently remembered, familiar, or forgotten.
  • 25
    • 0023780253 scopus 로고
    • J. A. Saint-Cyr, A. E. Taylor, A. E. Lang, Brain 111, 941 (1988); B. J. Knowlton, J. A. Mangels, L. R. Squire, Science 273, 1399 (1996).
    • (1988) Brain , vol.111 , pp. 941
    • Saint-Cyr, J.A.1    Taylor, A.E.2    Lang, A.E.3
  • 28
    • 0032076255 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • S. E. Petersen, P. T. Fox, M. I. Posner, M. Mintun, M. E. Raichle, Nature 331, 585 (1988); C. S. Carter et al., Science 280, 747 (1998).
    • (1998) Science , vol.280 , pp. 747
    • Carter, C.S.1
  • 30
    • 0032499196 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • G. K. Aguirre, J. A. Detre, D. C. Alsop, M. D'Esposito, Cereb. Cortex 6, 823 (1996); R. Epstein and N. Kanwisher, Nature 392, 598 (1998); E. A. Maguire et al., Science 280, 921 (1998).
    • (1998) Nature , vol.392 , pp. 598
    • Epstein, R.1    Kanwisher, N.2
  • 31
    • 0032496356 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • G. K. Aguirre, J. A. Detre, D. C. Alsop, M. D'Esposito, Cereb. Cortex 6, 823 (1996); R. Epstein and N. Kanwisher, Nature 392, 598 (1998); E. A. Maguire et al., Science 280, 921 (1998).
    • (1998) Science , vol.280 , pp. 921
    • Maguire, E.A.1
  • 34
    • 0030061766 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • J. V. Haxby et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 922 (1996); R. J. Dolan and P. C. Fletcher, Nature 388, 582 (1997); A. Martin, C. L. Wiggs, J. Weisberg, Hippocampus 7, 587 (1997); S. A. Rombouts et al., ibid., p. 594; W. M. Kelly et al., Neuron 20, 927 (1998); A. D. Wagner et al., Science 281, 1188 (1998).
    • (1996) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. , vol.93 , pp. 922
    • Haxby, J.V.1
  • 35
    • 0030806919 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • J. V. Haxby et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 922 (1996); R. J. Dolan and P. C. Fletcher, Nature 388, 582 (1997); A. Martin, C. L. Wiggs, J. Weisberg, Hippocampus 7, 587 (1997); S. A. Rombouts et al., ibid., p. 594; W. M. Kelly et al., Neuron 20, 927 (1998); A. D. Wagner et al., Science 281, 1188 (1998).
    • (1997) Nature , vol.388 , pp. 582
    • Dolan, R.J.1    Fletcher, P.C.2
  • 36
    • 0031437871 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • J. V. Haxby et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 922 (1996); R. J. Dolan and P. C. Fletcher, Nature 388, 582 (1997); A. Martin, C. L. Wiggs, J. Weisberg, Hippocampus 7, 587 (1997); S. A. Rombouts et al., ibid., p. 594; W. M. Kelly et al., Neuron 20, 927 (1998); A. D. Wagner et al., Science 281, 1188 (1998).
    • (1997) Hippocampus , vol.7 , pp. 587
    • Martin, A.1    Wiggs, C.L.2    Weisberg, J.3
  • 37
    • 0030061766 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • J. V. Haxby et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 922 (1996); R. J. Dolan and P. C. Fletcher, Nature 388, 582 (1997); A. Martin, C. L. Wiggs, J. Weisberg, Hippocampus 7, 587 (1997); S. A. Rombouts et al., ibid., p. 594; W. M. Kelly et al., Neuron 20, 927 (1998); A. D. Wagner et al., Science 281, 1188 (1998).
    • Hippocampus , pp. 594
    • Rombouts, S.A.1
  • 38
    • 17344395327 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • J. V. Haxby et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 922 (1996); R. J. Dolan and P. C. Fletcher, Nature 388, 582 (1997); A. Martin, C. L. Wiggs, J. Weisberg, Hippocampus 7, 587 (1997); S. A. Rombouts et al., ibid., p. 594; W. M. Kelly et al., Neuron 20, 927 (1998); A. D. Wagner et al., Science 281, 1188 (1998).
    • (1998) Neuron , vol.20 , pp. 927
    • Kelly, W.M.1
  • 39
    • 0032555511 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • J. V. Haxby et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 922 (1996); R. J. Dolan and P. C. Fletcher, Nature 388, 582 (1997); A. Martin, C. L. Wiggs, J. Weisberg, Hippocampus 7, 587 (1997); S. A. Rombouts et al., ibid., p. 594; W. M. Kelly et al., Neuron 20, 927 (1998); A. D. Wagner et al., Science 281, 1188 (1998).
    • (1998) Science , vol.281 , pp. 1188
    • Wagner, A.D.1
  • 40
    • 3543015640 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Supported by grants from the National Institute on Aging and the National Center for Research Resources. J.B.B. was supported by a Medical Scientist Training Program grant awarded by the National Institute of General Medical Sciences. We thank two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments, R. Poldrack and G. Fernandez for stimulating discussions, and E. Thomas, W. Francis, and C. Vaidya for advice on analysis and this manuscript.


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.