메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 278, Issue 5335, 1997, Pages 52-58

Drug abuse: Hedonic homeostatic dysregulation

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords

ARTICLE; BRAIN DYSFUNCTION; DISEASE PREDISPOSITION; DISTRESS SYNDROME; DOPAMINERGIC SYSTEM; DRUG ABUSE; HOMEOSTASIS; HUMAN; LIMBIC SYSTEM; NEUROBIOLOGY; PRIORITY JOURNAL; PSYCHIATRY; REWARD; SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY;

EID: 0030756351     PISSN: 00368075     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1126/science.278.5335.52     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (2084)

References (101)
  • 4
    • 0004055731 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • National Academy Press, Washington, DC
    • A recent Institute of Medicine report [Institute of Medicine, Pathways of Addiction (National Academy Press, Washington, DC, 1996)] used a three-stage conceptualization of drug-taking behavior that applies to all psychoactive drugs, whether licit or illicit: use, abuse, and dependence. "Use" of drugs is the taking of drugs, in the narrow sense, to distinguish it from a more intensified pattern of use. "Abuse" refers to any harmful use, regardless of whether the behavior constitutes a disorder in the DSM-IV of the American Psychiatric Association. "Dependence" refers to "substance dependence" as defined by DSM-IV or "addiction" as defined by International Classification of Diseases (ICD 10).
    • (1996) Pathways of Addiction
  • 6
    • 0030758555 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • in press
    • G. F. Koob and E. J. Nestler, J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 9, 482 (1997); P. V. Piazza and M. Le Moal, Brain Res. Rev., in press.
    • Brain Res. Rev.
    • Piazza, P.V.1    Le Moal, M.2
  • 7
    • 0003950824 scopus 로고
    • Academic Press, San Diego
    • Underregulation can be defined as a "failure to exert control over one's self." Conflicting or inadequate standards would be a breakdown in the basis for self-regulation. Reduction in monitoring is a failure of a person to evaluate one's self and actions against relevant standards. Inadequate strength is analogous to the common-sense concept of willpower and is a conflict between the power of impulse/tendency to act and the self-regulatory mechanism to interrupt that response and prevent action. Misregulation can be defined as "exerting control in a way that fails to bring about the desired result or leads to some alternative result." Misregulation probably most often involves some kind of deficiency in knowledge, especially self-knowledge. These knowledge deficiencies include false beliefs, distorted beliefs, overgeneralizations, and misdirected control efforts. Lapse-activated causal patterns are the patterns of behavior that translate an initial lapse (break in self-regulation) into a large-scale indulgence or major binge. Many factors contribute to these patterns of behavior, including underregulation, emotional responses, stress, zero-tolerance beliefs, spiralling distress, and others [R. F. Baumeister, T. F. Heatherton, D. M. Tice, Eds., Losing Control: How and Why People Fail at Self-Regulation (Academic Press, San Diego, 1994)].
    • (1994) Losing Control: How and Why People Fail at Self-Regulation
    • Baumeister, R.F.1    Heatherton, T.F.2    Tice, D.M.3
  • 8
    • 0027237106 scopus 로고
    • The use of animal models to characterize the neurobiology of specific aspects of human disorders is a reorientation to the "top-down" approach. Here, specific behaviors are explored at the system level, the cellular level, and ultimately the molecular level, with hypothesis testing based on an understanding of the mechanism of the behavioral response [A. Markou et al., Psychopharmacology 112, 163 (1993); G. F. Koob, in Psychopharmacology: The Fourth Generation of Progress, F. E. Bloom and D. J. Kupfer, Eds. (Raven Press, New York, 1995), pp. 759-772; G. F. Koob et al., J. Psychopharmacol., in press].
    • (1993) Psychopharmacology , vol.112 , pp. 163
    • Markou, A.1
  • 9
    • 0001809695 scopus 로고
    • F. E. Bloom and D. J. Kupfer, Eds. Raven Press, New York
    • The use of animal models to characterize the neurobiology of specific aspects of human disorders is a reorientation to the "top-down" approach. Here, specific behaviors are explored at the system level, the cellular level, and ultimately the molecular level, with hypothesis testing based on an understanding of the mechanism of the behavioral response [A. Markou et al., Psychopharmacology 112, 163 (1993); G. F. Koob, in Psychopharmacology: The Fourth Generation of Progress, F. E. Bloom and D. J. Kupfer, Eds. (Raven Press, New York, 1995), pp. 759-772; G. F. Koob et al., J. Psychopharmacol., in press].
    • (1995) Psychopharmacology: The Fourth Generation of Progress , pp. 759-772
    • Koob, G.F.1
  • 10
    • 1842273508 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • in press
    • The use of animal models to characterize the neurobiology of specific aspects of human disorders is a reorientation to the "top-down" approach. Here, specific behaviors are explored at the system level, the cellular level, and ultimately the molecular level, with hypothesis testing based on an understanding of the mechanism of the behavioral response [A. Markou et al., Psychopharmacology 112, 163 (1993); G. F. Koob, in Psychopharmacology: The Fourth Generation of Progress, F. E. Bloom and D. J. Kupfer, Eds. (Raven Press, New York, 1995), pp. 759-772; G. F. Koob et al., J. Psychopharmacol., in press].
    • J. Psychopharmacol.
    • Koob, G.F.1
  • 12
    • 0024553362 scopus 로고
    • R. A. Wise and P.-P. Rompre, Annu. Rev. Physiol. 40, 191 (1989); M. Le Moal and H. Simon, Physiol. Rev. 71, 155 (1991); G. F. Koob, Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 13, 177 (1992); F. E. Pontieri, G. Tanda, F. Orzi, G. Di Chiara, Nature 382, 255 (1996).
    • (1989) Annu. Rev. Physiol. , vol.40 , pp. 191
    • Wise, R.A.1    Rompre, P.-P.2
  • 13
    • 0026053705 scopus 로고
    • R. A. Wise and P.-P. Rompre, Annu. Rev. Physiol. 40, 191 (1989); M. Le Moal and H. Simon, Physiol. Rev. 71, 155 (1991); G. F. Koob, Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 13, 177 (1992); F. E. Pontieri, G. Tanda, F. Orzi, G. Di Chiara, Nature 382, 255 (1996).
    • (1991) Physiol. Rev. , vol.71 , pp. 155
    • Le Moal, M.1    Simon, H.2
  • 14
    • 0026507628 scopus 로고
    • R. A. Wise and P.-P. Rompre, Annu. Rev. Physiol. 40, 191 (1989); M. Le Moal and H. Simon, Physiol. Rev. 71, 155 (1991); G. F. Koob, Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 13, 177 (1992); F. E. Pontieri, G. Tanda, F. Orzi, G. Di Chiara, Nature 382, 255 (1996).
    • (1992) Trends Pharmacol. Sci. , vol.13 , pp. 177
    • Koob, G.F.1
  • 15
    • 0029943635 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • R. A. Wise and P.-P. Rompre, Annu. Rev. Physiol. 40, 191 (1989); M. Le Moal and H. Simon, Physiol. Rev. 71, 155 (1991); G. F. Koob, Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 13, 177 (1992); F. E. Pontieri, G. Tanda, F. Orzi, G. Di Chiara, Nature 382, 255 (1996).
    • (1996) Nature , vol.382 , pp. 255
    • Pontieri, F.E.1    Tanda, G.2    Orzi, F.3    Di Chiara, G.4
  • 16
    • 0022560106 scopus 로고
    • W. L. Woolverton, Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 24, 531 (1986); G. F. Koob, H. T. Le, I. Creese, Neurosci. Lett. 79, 315 (1987); J. Bergman, J. B. Kamien, R. D. Spealman, Behav. Pharmacol. 1, 355 (1990); S. B. Caine and G. F. Koob, Science 260, 1814 (1993).
    • (1986) Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. , vol.24 , pp. 531
    • Woolverton, W.L.1
  • 17
    • 0023162527 scopus 로고
    • W. L. Woolverton, Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 24, 531 (1986); G. F. Koob, H. T. Le, I. Creese, Neurosci. Lett. 79, 315 (1987); J. Bergman, J. B. Kamien, R. D. Spealman, Behav. Pharmacol. 1, 355 (1990); S. B. Caine and G. F. Koob, Science 260, 1814 (1993).
    • (1987) Neurosci. Lett. , vol.79 , pp. 315
    • Koob, G.F.1    Le, H.T.2    Creese, I.3
  • 18
    • 0022560106 scopus 로고
    • W. L. Woolverton, Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 24, 531 (1986); G. F. Koob, H. T. Le, I. Creese, Neurosci. Lett. 79, 315 (1987); J. Bergman, J. B. Kamien, R. D. Spealman, Behav. Pharmacol. 1, 355 (1990); S. B. Caine and G. F. Koob, Science 260, 1814 (1993).
    • (1990) Behav. Pharmacol. , vol.1 , pp. 355
    • Bergman, J.1    Kamien, J.B.2    Spealman, R.D.3
  • 19
    • 0027194404 scopus 로고
    • W. L. Woolverton, Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 24, 531 (1986); G. F. Koob, H. T. Le, I. Creese, Neurosci. Lett. 79, 315 (1987); J. Bergman, J. B. Kamien, R. D. Spealman, Behav. Pharmacol. 1, 355 (1990); S. B. Caine and G. F. Koob, Science 260, 1814 (1993).
    • (1993) Science , vol.260 , pp. 1814
    • Caine, S.B.1    Koob, G.F.2
  • 21
    • 0026598192 scopus 로고
    • G. Di Chiara and R. A. North, Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 13, 185 (1992); T. S. Shippenberg, A. Herz, R. Spanagel, R. Bals-Kubik, C. Stein, Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 654, 347 (1992).
    • (1992) Trends Pharmacol. Sci. , vol.13 , pp. 185
    • Di Chiara, G.1    North, R.A.2
  • 23
    • 0002209379 scopus 로고
    • J. Engel and L. Oreland, Eds. Raven Press, New York
    • G. F. Koob, F. J. Vaccarino, M. Amalric, F. E. Bloom, in Brain Reward Systems and Abuse, J. Engel and L. Oreland, Eds. (Raven Press, New York, 1987), pp. 35-50; J. A. Engel et al., in Novel Pharmacological Interventions for Alcoholism, C. A. Naranjo and E. M. Sellers, Eds. (Springer, New York, 1992), pp. 68-82; E. M. Sellers, G. A. Higgins, M. B. Sobell, Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 13, 69 (1992); B. Tabakoff and P. L. Hoffman, in Substance Abuse: A Comprehensive Textbook, J. H. Lowinson, P. Ruiz, R. B. Millman, Eds. (Williams & Wilkins, Baltimore, 1992), pp. 152-185.
    • (1987) Brain Reward Systems and Abuse , pp. 35-50
    • Koob, G.F.1    Vaccarino, F.J.2    Amalric, M.3    Bloom, F.E.4
  • 24
    • 0001858481 scopus 로고
    • C. A. Naranjo and E. M. Sellers, Eds. Springer, New York
    • G. F. Koob, F. J. Vaccarino, M. Amalric, F. E. Bloom, in Brain Reward Systems and Abuse, J. Engel and L. Oreland, Eds. (Raven Press, New York, 1987), pp. 35-50; J. A. Engel et al., in Novel Pharmacological Interventions for Alcoholism, C. A. Naranjo and E. M. Sellers, Eds. (Springer, New York, 1992), pp. 68-82; E. M. Sellers, G. A. Higgins, M. B. Sobell, Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 13, 69 (1992); B. Tabakoff and P. L. Hoffman, in Substance Abuse: A Comprehensive Textbook, J. H. Lowinson, P. Ruiz, R. B. Millman, Eds. (Williams & Wilkins, Baltimore, 1992), pp. 152-185.
    • (1992) Novel Pharmacological Interventions for Alcoholism , pp. 68-82
    • Engel, J.A.1
  • 25
    • 0026536784 scopus 로고
    • G. F. Koob, F. J. Vaccarino, M. Amalric, F. E. Bloom, in Brain Reward Systems and Abuse, J. Engel and L. Oreland, Eds. (Raven Press, New York, 1987), pp. 35-50; J. A. Engel et al., in Novel Pharmacological Interventions for Alcoholism, C. A. Naranjo and E. M. Sellers, Eds. (Springer, New York, 1992), pp. 68-82; E. M. Sellers, G. A. Higgins, M. B. Sobell, Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 13, 69 (1992); B. Tabakoff and P. L. Hoffman, in Substance Abuse: A Comprehensive Textbook, J. H. Lowinson, P. Ruiz, R. B. Millman, Eds. (Williams & Wilkins, Baltimore, 1992), pp. 152-185.
    • (1992) Trends Pharmacol. Sci. , vol.13 , pp. 69
    • Sellers, E.M.1    Higgins, G.A.2    Sobell, M.B.3
  • 26
    • 0001966840 scopus 로고
    • J. H. Lowinson, P. Ruiz, R. B. Millman, Eds. Williams & Wilkins, Baltimore
    • G. F. Koob, F. J. Vaccarino, M. Amalric, F. E. Bloom, in Brain Reward Systems and Abuse, J. Engel and L. Oreland, Eds. (Raven Press, New York, 1987), pp. 35-50; J. A. Engel et al., in Novel Pharmacological Interventions for Alcoholism, C. A. Naranjo and E. M. Sellers, Eds. (Springer, New York, 1992), pp. 68-82; E. M. Sellers, G. A. Higgins, M. B. Sobell, Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 13, 69 (1992); B. Tabakoff and P. L. Hoffman, in Substance Abuse: A Comprehensive Textbook, J. H. Lowinson, P. Ruiz, R. B. Millman, Eds. (Williams & Wilkins, Baltimore, 1992), pp. 152-185.
    • (1992) Substance Abuse: A Comprehensive Textbook , pp. 152-185
    • Tabakoff, B.1    Hoffman, P.L.2
  • 28
    • 0030761521 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • J. P. Chen, W. Paredes, J. H. Lowinson, E. L. Gardner, Neurosci. Lett. 129, 136 (1991); G. Tanda, F. E. Pontieri, G. Di Chiara, Science 276, 2048 (1997).
    • (1997) Science , vol.276 , pp. 2048
    • Tanda, G.1    Pontieri, F.E.2    Di Chiara, G.3
  • 31
    • 0029905264 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • G. F. Koob, Neuron 16, 893 (1996).
    • (1996) Neuron , vol.16 , pp. 893
    • Koob, G.F.1
  • 32
    • 0001950226 scopus 로고
    • G. Edwards, M. A. H. Russell, D. Hawks, M. MacCafferty, Eds. Saxon House/Lexington Books, Westmead, UK
    • Russell argued, "The notion of dependence on a drug, object, role, activity, or any other stimulus-source requires the crucial feature of negative affect experienced in its absence. The degree of dependence can be equated with the amount of this negative affect, which may range from mild discomfort to extreme distress, or it may be equated with the amount of difficulty or effort required to do without the drug, object, etc." This appearance of negative affect could have motivational significance or simply be a signal of a change in set point [M. A. H. Russell, in Drugs and Drug Dependence, G. Edwards, M. A. H. Russell, D. Hawks, M. MacCafferty, Eds. (Saxon House/Lexington Books, Westmead, UK, 1976), pp. 182-187].
    • (1976) Drugs and Drug Dependence , pp. 182-187
    • Russell, M.A.H.1
  • 37
    • 0016045280 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • In reference to opponent process theory, the a-processes (positive hedonic effects) were proposed to occur shortly after presentation of the reinforcer and to show tolerance. In contrast, the b-processes (negative hedonic effects) appear after the a-process has terminated, are slow to decay, and get larger with repeated exposure. More recent data and conceptualizations suggest that the b-process actually may appear shortly after the beginning of the a-process and may ultimately result in a change in hedonic set point (Fig. 4). In reference to sensitization theory, sensitization has been defined as the increased response to a drug that follows its repeated intermittent presentation and may be a potential source of significant differential vulnerability to certain concepts of the addiction cycle. Sensitization theory invokes a shift to a state of incentive salience (defined as a hypersensitive neural state that produces the experience of "wanting"). This state of incentive salience is hypothesized to be produced by drug-induced sensitization of the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system. Thus, the pathologically strong craving or "wanting" encountered during protracted abstinence would be hypothesized to be due, in large part, to an overactive mesocorticolimbic dopamine system. Sensitization has largely been characterized as an enhanced motor response to drugs of abuse with repeated administration, though recent work has extended these effects to a progressive increase in the reinforcing value of the drugs, at least during initial acquisition of drug-seeking or conditioned drug effects (Figs. 1 and 4). Sensitization is also more likely to be evident with intermittent, rather than continuous, administration of drugs, which favors the development of tolerance, and sensitization is long-lasting (up to weeks after injection) [R. L. Solomon and J. D. Corbit, Psychol. Rev. 81, 119 (1974); R. L. Solomon, in Psychopathology: Experimental Models, J. D. Maser and M. E. P. Seligman, Eds. (Freeman, San Francisco, 1977), pp. 124-145; T. E. Robinson and K. C. Berridge, Brain Res. Rev. 18, 247 (1993); G. Schulteis and G. F. Koob, Neurochem. Res. 21, 1437 (1996); J.-P. Laulin, A. Larcher, E. Celerier, M. Le Moal, G. Simonnet, Eur. J. Pharmacol., in press].
    • (1974) Psychol. Rev. , vol.81 , pp. 119
    • Solomon, R.L.1    Corbit, J.D.2
  • 38
    • 0016045280 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • J. D. Maser and M. E. P. Seligman, Eds. Freeman, San Francisco
    • In reference to opponent process theory, the a-processes (positive hedonic effects) were proposed to occur shortly after presentation of the reinforcer and to show tolerance. In contrast, the b-processes (negative hedonic effects) appear after the a-process has terminated, are slow to decay, and get larger with repeated exposure. More recent data and conceptualizations suggest that the b-process actually may appear shortly after the beginning of the a-process and may ultimately result in a change in hedonic set point (Fig. 4). In reference to sensitization theory, sensitization has been defined as the increased response to a drug that follows its repeated intermittent presentation and may be a potential source of significant differential vulnerability to certain concepts of the addiction cycle. Sensitization theory invokes a shift to a state of incentive salience (defined as a hypersensitive neural state that produces the experience of "wanting"). This state of incentive salience is hypothesized to be produced by drug-induced sensitization of the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system. Thus, the pathologically strong craving or "wanting" encountered during protracted abstinence would be hypothesized to be due, in large part, to an overactive mesocorticolimbic dopamine system. Sensitization has largely been characterized as an enhanced motor response to drugs of abuse with repeated administration, though recent work has extended these effects to a progressive increase in the reinforcing value of the drugs, at least during initial acquisition of drug-seeking or conditioned drug effects (Figs. 1 and 4). Sensitization is also more likely to be evident with intermittent, rather than continuous, administration of drugs, which favors the development of tolerance, and sensitization is long-lasting (up to weeks after injection) [R. L. Solomon and J. D. Corbit, Psychol. Rev. 81, 119 (1974); R. L. Solomon, in Psychopathology: Experimental Models, J. D. Maser and M. E. P. Seligman, Eds. (Freeman, San Francisco, 1977), pp. 124-145; T. E. Robinson and K. C. Berridge, Brain Res. Rev. 18, 247 (1993); G. Schulteis and G. F. Koob, Neurochem. Res. 21, 1437 (1996); J.-P. Laulin, A. Larcher, E. Celerier, M. Le Moal, G. Simonnet, Eur. J. Pharmacol., in press].
    • (1977) Psychopathology: Experimental Models , pp. 124-145
    • Solomon, R.L.1
  • 39
    • 0027305458 scopus 로고
    • In reference to opponent process theory, the a-processes (positive hedonic effects) were proposed to occur shortly after presentation of the reinforcer and to show tolerance. In contrast, the b-processes (negative hedonic effects) appear after the a-process has terminated, are slow to decay, and get larger with repeated exposure. More recent data and conceptualizations suggest that the b-process actually may appear shortly after the beginning of the a-process and may ultimately result in a change in hedonic set point (Fig. 4). In reference to sensitization theory, sensitization has been defined as the increased response to a drug that follows its repeated intermittent presentation and may be a potential source of significant differential vulnerability to certain concepts of the addiction cycle. Sensitization theory invokes a shift to a state of incentive salience (defined as a hypersensitive neural state that produces the experience of "wanting"). This state of incentive salience is hypothesized to be produced by drug-induced sensitization of the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system. Thus, the pathologically strong craving or "wanting" encountered during protracted abstinence would be hypothesized to be due, in large part, to an overactive mesocorticolimbic dopamine system. Sensitization has largely been characterized as an enhanced motor response to drugs of abuse with repeated administration, though recent work has extended these effects to a progressive increase in the reinforcing value of the drugs, at least during initial acquisition of drug-seeking or conditioned drug effects (Figs. 1 and 4). Sensitization is also more likely to be evident with intermittent, rather than continuous, administration of drugs, which favors the development of tolerance, and sensitization is long-lasting (up to weeks after injection) [R. L. Solomon and J. D. Corbit, Psychol. Rev. 81, 119 (1974); R. L. Solomon, in Psychopathology: Experimental Models, J. D. Maser and M. E. P. Seligman, Eds. (Freeman, San Francisco, 1977), pp. 124-145; T. E. Robinson and K. C. Berridge, Brain Res. Rev. 18, 247 (1993); G. Schulteis and G. F. Koob, Neurochem. Res. 21, 1437 (1996); J.-P. Laulin, A. Larcher, E. Celerier, M. Le Moal, G. Simonnet, Eur. J. Pharmacol., in press].
    • (1993) Brain Res. Rev. , vol.18 , pp. 247
    • Robinson, T.E.1    Berridge, K.C.2
  • 40
    • 0029857333 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • In reference to opponent process theory, the a-processes (positive hedonic effects) were proposed to occur shortly after presentation of the reinforcer and to show tolerance. In contrast, the b-processes (negative hedonic effects) appear after the a-process has terminated, are slow to decay, and get larger with repeated exposure. More recent data and conceptualizations suggest that the b-process actually may appear shortly after the beginning of the a-process and may ultimately result in a change in hedonic set point (Fig. 4). In reference to sensitization theory, sensitization has been defined as the increased response to a drug that follows its repeated intermittent presentation and may be a potential source of significant differential vulnerability to certain concepts of the addiction cycle. Sensitization theory invokes a shift to a state of incentive salience (defined as a hypersensitive neural state that produces the experience of "wanting"). This state of incentive salience is hypothesized to be produced by drug-induced sensitization of the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system. Thus, the pathologically strong craving or "wanting" encountered during protracted abstinence would be hypothesized to be due, in large part, to an overactive mesocorticolimbic dopamine system. Sensitization has largely been characterized as an enhanced motor response to drugs of abuse with repeated administration, though recent work has extended these effects to a progressive increase in the reinforcing value of the drugs, at least during initial acquisition of drug-seeking or conditioned drug effects (Figs. 1 and 4). Sensitization is also more likely to be evident with intermittent, rather than continuous, administration of drugs, which favors the development of tolerance, and sensitization is long-lasting (up to weeks after injection) [R. L. Solomon and J. D. Corbit, Psychol. Rev. 81, 119 (1974); R. L. Solomon, in Psychopathology: Experimental Models, J. D. Maser and M. E. P. Seligman, Eds. (Freeman, San Francisco, 1977), pp. 124-145; T. E. Robinson and K. C. Berridge, Brain Res. Rev. 18, 247 (1993); G. Schulteis and G. F. Koob, Neurochem. Res. 21, 1437 (1996); J.-P. Laulin, A. Larcher, E. Celerier, M. Le Moal, G. Simonnet, Eur. J. Pharmacol., in press].
    • (1996) Neurochem. Res. , vol.21 , pp. 1437
    • Schulteis, G.1    Koob, G.F.2
  • 41
    • 0016045280 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • in press
    • In reference to opponent process theory, the a-processes (positive hedonic effects) were proposed to occur shortly after presentation of the reinforcer and to show tolerance. In contrast, the b-processes (negative hedonic effects) appear after the a-process has terminated, are slow to decay, and get larger with repeated exposure. More recent data and conceptualizations suggest that the b-process actually may appear shortly after the beginning of the a-process and may ultimately result in a change in hedonic set point (Fig. 4). In reference to sensitization theory, sensitization has been defined as the increased response to a drug that follows its repeated intermittent presentation and may be a potential source of significant differential vulnerability to certain concepts of the addiction cycle. Sensitization theory invokes a shift to a state of incentive salience (defined as a hypersensitive neural state that produces the experience of "wanting"). This state of incentive salience is hypothesized to be produced by drug-induced sensitization of the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system. Thus, the pathologically strong craving or "wanting" encountered during protracted abstinence would be hypothesized to be due, in large part, to an overactive mesocorticolimbic dopamine system. Sensitization has largely been characterized as an enhanced motor response to drugs of abuse with repeated administration, though recent work has extended these effects to a progressive increase in the reinforcing value of the drugs, at least during initial acquisition of drug-seeking or conditioned drug effects (Figs. 1 and 4). Sensitization is also more likely to be evident with intermittent, rather than continuous, administration of drugs, which favors the development of tolerance, and sensitization is long-lasting (up to weeks after injection) [R. L. Solomon and J. D. Corbit, Psychol. Rev. 81, 119 (1974); R. L. Solomon, in Psychopathology: Experimental Models, J. D. Maser and M. E. P. Seligman, Eds. (Freeman, San Francisco, 1977), pp. 124-145; T. E. Robinson and K. C. Berridge, Brain Res. Rev. 18, 247 (1993); G. Schulteis and G. F. Koob, Neurochem. Res. 21, 1437 (1996); J.-P. Laulin, A. Larcher, E. Celerier, M. Le Moal, G. Simonnet, Eur. J. Pharmacol., in press].
    • Eur. J. Pharmacol.
    • Laulin, J.-P.1    Larcher, A.2    Celerier, E.3    Le Moal, M.4    Simonnet, G.5
  • 43
    • 0029990389 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • F. Weiss, A. Markou, M. T. Lorang, G. F. Koob, Brain Res. 593, 314 (1992); F. Weiss et al., J. Neurosci. 16, 3474 (1996).
    • (1996) J. Neurosci. , vol.16 , pp. 3474
    • Weiss, F.1
  • 46
    • 0025751604 scopus 로고
    • P. W. Kalivas and J. Stewart, Brain Res. Rev. 16, 223 (1991); F. J. White and M. E. Wolf, in The Biological Bases of Drug Tolerance and Dependence, J. A. Pratt, Ed. (Academic Press, London, 1991), pp. 153-197; D. J. Henry and F. J. White, Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 654, 88 (1992).
    • (1991) Brain Res. Rev. , vol.16 , pp. 223
    • Kalivas, P.W.1    Stewart, J.2
  • 47
    • 0025751604 scopus 로고
    • J. A. Pratt, Ed. Academic Press, London
    • P. W. Kalivas and J. Stewart, Brain Res. Rev. 16, 223 (1991); F. J. White and M. E. Wolf, in The Biological Bases of Drug Tolerance and Dependence, J. A. Pratt, Ed. (Academic Press, London, 1991), pp. 153-197; D. J. Henry and F. J. White, Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 654, 88 (1992).
    • (1991) The Biological Bases of Drug Tolerance and Dependence , pp. 153-197
    • White, F.J.1    Wolf, M.E.2
  • 48
    • 0026674530 scopus 로고
    • P. W. Kalivas and J. Stewart, Brain Res. Rev. 16, 223 (1991); F. J. White and M. E. Wolf, in The Biological Bases of Drug Tolerance and Dependence, J. A. Pratt, Ed. (Academic Press, London, 1991), pp. 153-197; D. J. Henry and F. J. White, Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 654, 88 (1992).
    • (1992) Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. , vol.654 , pp. 88
    • Henry, D.J.1    White, F.J.2
  • 49
    • 0025195866 scopus 로고
    • D. H. Malin et al., Peptides 11, 277 (1990); T. S. Shippenberg, A. LeFevour, C. Heidbreder, J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 276, 545 (1996).
    • (1990) Peptides , vol.11 , pp. 277
    • Malin, D.H.1
  • 51
    • 84909890030 scopus 로고
    • The extended amygdala includes the medial (shell) part of the nucleus accumbens, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, the central medial amygdala, and the sublenticular substantia innominata region. The afferent and efferent connections of the extended amygdala make it well situated for mediating not only basic drug reward, but also the neuroadaptive changes associated with sensitization and counter-adaptation. Afferent connections include projections from the limbic cortices, hippocampus, basolateral amygdala, midbrain, and lateral hypothalamus. Efferent connections include the ventral pallidum, ventral tegmental area, various brainstem projections, and a large projection to the lateral hypothalamus [J. B. Johnston, J. Comp. Neurol. 35, 337 (1923); G. F. Alheid and L. Heimer, Neuroscience 27, 1 (1988); L. Heimer and G. Alheid, in The Basal Forebrain: Anatomy to Function, T. C. Napier, P. W. Kalivas, I. Hanin, Eds. (Plenum, New York, 1991), pp. 1-42].
    • (1923) J. Comp. Neurol. , vol.35 , pp. 337
    • Johnston, J.B.1
  • 52
    • 0023697444 scopus 로고
    • The extended amygdala includes the medial (shell) part of the nucleus accumbens, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, the central medial amygdala, and the sublenticular substantia innominata region. The afferent and efferent connections of the extended amygdala make it well situated for mediating not only basic drug reward, but also the neuroadaptive changes associated with sensitization and counter-adaptation. Afferent connections include projections from the limbic cortices, hippocampus, basolateral amygdala, midbrain, and lateral hypothalamus. Efferent connections include the ventral pallidum, ventral tegmental area, various brainstem projections, and a large projection to the lateral hypothalamus [J. B. Johnston, J. Comp. Neurol. 35, 337 (1923); G. F. Alheid and L. Heimer, Neuroscience 27, 1 (1988); L. Heimer and G. Alheid, in The Basal Forebrain: Anatomy to Function, T. C. Napier, P. W. Kalivas, I. Hanin, Eds. (Plenum, New York, 1991), pp. 1-42].
    • (1988) Neuroscience , vol.27 , pp. 1
    • Alheid, G.F.1    Heimer, L.2
  • 53
    • 0026345518 scopus 로고
    • T. C. Napier, P. W. Kalivas, I. Hanin, Eds. Plenum, New York
    • The extended amygdala includes the medial (shell) part of the nucleus accumbens, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, the central medial amygdala, and the sublenticular substantia innominata region. The afferent and efferent connections of the extended amygdala make it well situated for mediating not only basic drug reward, but also the neuroadaptive changes associated with sensitization and counter-adaptation. Afferent connections include projections from the limbic cortices, hippocampus, basolateral amygdala, midbrain, and lateral hypothalamus. Efferent connections include the ventral pallidum, ventral tegmental area, various brainstem projections, and a large projection to the lateral hypothalamus [J. B. Johnston, J. Comp. Neurol. 35, 337 (1923); G. F. Alheid and L. Heimer, Neuroscience 27, 1 (1988); L. Heimer and G. Alheid, in The Basal Forebrain: Anatomy to Function, T. C. Napier, P. W. Kalivas, I. Hanin, Eds. (Plenum, New York, 1991), pp. 1-42].
    • (1991) The Basal Forebrain: Anatomy to Function , pp. 1-42
    • Heimer, L.1    Alheid, G.2
  • 54
    • 0029027973 scopus 로고
    • S. B. Caine, S. C. Heinrichs, V. L. Coffin, G. F. Koob, Brain Res. 692, 47 (1995); M. P. Epping-Jordan, A. Markou, G. F. Koob, Neurosci. Abstr. 21, 719 (1995); F. E. Pontieri, G. Tanda, G. Di Chiara, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 92, 12304 (1995);
    • (1995) Brain Res. , vol.692 , pp. 47
    • Caine, S.B.1    Heinrichs, S.C.2    Coffin, V.L.3    Koob, G.F.4
  • 55
    • 0029027973 scopus 로고
    • S. B. Caine, S. C. Heinrichs, V. L. Coffin, G. F. Koob, Brain Res. 692, 47 (1995); M. P. Epping-Jordan, A. Markou, G. F. Koob, Neurosci. Abstr. 21, 719 (1995); F. E. Pontieri, G. Tanda, G. Di Chiara, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 92, 12304 (1995);
    • (1995) Neurosci. Abstr. , vol.21 , pp. 719
    • Epping-Jordan, M.P.1    Markou, A.2    Koob, G.F.3
  • 56
    • 0029583656 scopus 로고
    • S. B. Caine, S. C. Heinrichs, V. L. Coffin, G. F. Koob, Brain Res. 692, 47 (1995); M. P. Epping-Jordan, A. Markou, G. F. Koob, Neurosci. Abstr. 21, 719 (1995); F. E. Pontieri, G. Tanda, G. Di Chiara, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 92, 12304 (1995);
    • (1995) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. , vol.92 , pp. 12304
    • Pontieri, F.E.1    Tanda, G.2    Di Chiara, G.3
  • 57
    • 0028981524 scopus 로고
    • P. Hyytia and G. F. Koob, Eur. J. Pharmacol. 283, 151 (1995); C. J.Heyser, A. J. Roberts, G. Schulteis, P. Hyytia, G. F. Koob, Neurosci. Abstr. 21, 1698 (1995).
    • (1995) Eur. J. Pharmacol. , vol.283 , pp. 151
    • Hyytia, P.1    Koob, G.F.2
  • 59
    • 0030574235 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Y. Zhou et al., Eur. J. Pharmacol. 315, 31 (1996); Y. Zhou et al., J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 279, 351 (1996).
    • (1996) Eur. J. Pharmacol. , vol.315 , pp. 31
    • Zhou, Y.1
  • 60
    • 0030431223 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Y. Zhou et al., Eur. J. Pharmacol. 315, 31 (1996); Y. Zhou et al., J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 279, 351 (1996).
    • (1996) J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. , vol.279 , pp. 351
    • Zhou, Y.1
  • 61
    • 0027415929 scopus 로고
    • S. Rassnick, S. C. Heinrichs, K. T. Britton, G. F. Koob, Brain Res. 605, 25 (1993); S. C. Heinrichs, F. Menzaghi, G. Schulteis, G. F. Koob, L. Stinus, Behav. Pharmacol. 6, 74 (1995); F. Rodríguez de Fonseca, M. R. A. Carrera, M. Navarro, G. F. Koob, F. Weiss, Science 276, 2050 (1997); E. Merlo Pich et al., J. Neurosci. 15, 5439 (1995).
    • (1993) Brain Res. , vol.605 , pp. 25
    • Rassnick, S.1    Heinrichs, S.C.2    Britton, K.T.3    Koob, G.F.4
  • 62
    • 0028860024 scopus 로고
    • S. Rassnick, S. C. Heinrichs, K. T. Britton, G. F. Koob, Brain Res. 605, 25 (1993); S. C. Heinrichs, F. Menzaghi, G. Schulteis, G. F. Koob, L. Stinus, Behav. Pharmacol. 6, 74 (1995); F. Rodríguez de Fonseca, M. R. A. Carrera, M. Navarro, G. F. Koob, F. Weiss, Science 276, 2050 (1997); E. Merlo Pich et al., J. Neurosci. 15, 5439 (1995).
    • (1995) Behav. Pharmacol. , vol.6 , pp. 74
    • Heinrichs, S.C.1    Menzaghi, F.2    Schulteis, G.3    Koob, G.F.4    Stinus, L.5
  • 63
    • 0039534405 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • S. Rassnick, S. C. Heinrichs, K. T. Britton, G. F. Koob, Brain Res. 605, 25 (1993); S. C. Heinrichs, F. Menzaghi, G. Schulteis, G. F. Koob, L. Stinus, Behav. Pharmacol. 6, 74 (1995); F. Rodríguez de Fonseca, M. R. A. Carrera, M. Navarro, G. F. Koob, F. Weiss, Science 276, 2050 (1997); E. Merlo Pich et al., J. Neurosci. 15, 5439 (1995).
    • (1997) Science , vol.276 , pp. 2050
    • Rodríguez De Fonseca, F.1    Carrera, M.R.A.2    Navarro, M.3    Koob, G.F.4    Weiss, F.5
  • 64
    • 0029117641 scopus 로고
    • S. Rassnick, S. C. Heinrichs, K. T. Britton, G. F. Koob, Brain Res. 605, 25 (1993); S. C. Heinrichs, F. Menzaghi, G. Schulteis, G. F. Koob, L. Stinus, Behav. Pharmacol. 6, 74 (1995); F. Rodríguez de Fonseca, M. R. A. Carrera, M. Navarro, G. F. Koob, F. Weiss, Science 276, 2050 (1997); E. Merlo Pich et al., J. Neurosci. 15, 5439 (1995).
    • (1995) J. Neurosci. , vol.15 , pp. 5439
    • Merlo Pich, E.1
  • 66
    • 0019780124 scopus 로고
    • H. deWit and J. Stewart, Psychopharmacology 75, 134 (1981); J. Stewart and H. deWit, in Assessing the Reinforcing Properties of Abused Drugs, M. A. Bozarth, Ed. (Springer-Verlag, New York, 1987), pp. 211-227; Y. Shaham, H. Rajabi, J. Stewart, J. Neurosci. 16, 1957 (1996).
    • (1981) Psychopharmacology , vol.75 , pp. 134
    • DeWit, H.1    Stewart, J.2
  • 67
    • 0019780124 scopus 로고
    • M. A. Bozarth, Ed. Springer-Verlag, New York
    • H. deWit and J. Stewart, Psychopharmacology 75, 134 (1981); J. Stewart and H. deWit, in Assessing the Reinforcing Properties of Abused Drugs, M. A. Bozarth, Ed. (Springer-Verlag, New York, 1987), pp. 211-227; Y. Shaham, H. Rajabi, J. Stewart, J. Neurosci. 16, 1957 (1996).
    • (1987) Assessing the Reinforcing Properties of Abused Drugs , pp. 211-227
    • Stewart, J.1    DeWit, H.2
  • 68
    • 0029867146 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • H. deWit and J. Stewart, Psychopharmacology 75, 134 (1981); J. Stewart and H. deWit, in Assessing the Reinforcing Properties of Abused Drugs, M. A. Bozarth, Ed. (Springer-Verlag, New York, 1987), pp. 211-227; Y. Shaham, H. Rajabi, J. Stewart, J. Neurosci. 16, 1957 (1996).
    • (1996) J. Neurosci. , vol.16 , pp. 1957
    • Shaham, Y.1    Rajabi, H.2    Stewart, J.3
  • 69
    • 0026498986 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • S. S. O'Malley et al., Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 49, 881 (1992); J. R. Volpicelli, A. I. Alterman, M. Hayashida, C. P. O'Brien, ibid., p. 876; H. Sass, M. Soyka, K. Mann, W. Zieglgansberger, ibid. 53, 673 (1996).
    • (1992) Arch. Gen. Psychiatry , vol.49 , pp. 881
    • O'Malley, S.S.1
  • 72
    • 0022639001 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • J. R. Volpicelli, M. A. Davis, J. E. Olgin, Life Sci. 38, 841 (1986); R. Spanagel, S. M. Hoelter, K. Allingham, R. Landgraf, W. Zieglgansberger, Eur. J. Pharmacol. 305, 39 (1996); C. J. Heyser, G. Schulteis, P. Durbin, G. F. Koob, Neuropsychopharmacology, in press.
    • (1986) Life Sci. , vol.38 , pp. 841
    • Volpicelli, J.R.1    Davis, M.A.2    Olgin, J.E.3
  • 74
    • 0022639001 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • in press
    • J. R. Volpicelli, M. A. Davis, J. E. Olgin, Life Sci. 38, 841 (1986); R. Spanagel, S. M. Hoelter, K. Allingham, R. Landgraf, W. Zieglgansberger, Eur. J. Pharmacol. 305, 39 (1996); C. J. Heyser, G. Schulteis, P. Durbin, G. F. Koob, Neuropsychopharmacology, in press.
    • Neuropsychopharmacology
    • Heyser, C.J.1    Schulteis, G.2    Durbin, P.3    Koob, G.F.4
  • 75
    • 1842394019 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • personal communication
    • D. Smith, personal communication.
    • Smith, D.1
  • 77
    • 0026509148 scopus 로고
    • J. C. Crabbe and J. K. Belknap, Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 13, 212 (1992); L. H. Gold, Behav. Pharmacol. 7, 589 (1996); H. W. D. Matthes et al., Nature 383, 819 (1996).
    • (1992) Trends Pharmacol. Sci. , vol.13 , pp. 212
    • Crabbe, J.C.1    Belknap, J.K.2
  • 78
    • 0030431522 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • J. C. Crabbe and J. K. Belknap, Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 13, 212 (1992); L. H. Gold, Behav. Pharmacol. 7, 589 (1996); H. W. D. Matthes et al., Nature 383, 819 (1996).
    • (1996) Behav. Pharmacol. , vol.7 , pp. 589
    • Gold, L.H.1
  • 79
    • 0029852678 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • J. C. Crabbe and J. K. Belknap, Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 13, 212 (1992); L. H. Gold, Behav. Pharmacol. 7, 589 (1996); H. W. D. Matthes et al., Nature 383, 819 (1996).
    • (1996) Nature , vol.383 , pp. 819
    • Matthes, H.W.D.1
  • 82
    • 0027057356 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • V. Deroche et al., Brain Res. 598, 343 (1992); P. V. Piazza and M. Le Moal, Brain Res. Rev., in press.
    • (1992) Brain Res. , vol.598 , pp. 343
    • Deroche, V.1
  • 89
    • 0000310973 scopus 로고
    • S. Fisher and J. Reason, Eds. Wiley, New York
    • P. Sterling and J. Eyer, in Handbook of Life Stress Cognition and Health, S. Fisher and J. Reason, Eds. (Wiley, New York, 1988), pp. 629-649; B. S. McEwen and E. Stellar, Arch. Intern. Med. 153, 2093 (1993).
    • (1988) Handbook of Life Stress Cognition and Health , pp. 629-649
    • Sterling, P.1    Eyer, J.2
  • 90
    • 0027515623 scopus 로고
    • P. Sterling and J. Eyer, in Handbook of Life Stress Cognition and Health, S. Fisher and J. Reason, Eds. (Wiley, New York, 1988), pp. 629-649; B. S. McEwen and E. Stellar, Arch. Intern. Med. 153, 2093 (1993).
    • (1993) Arch. Intern. Med. , vol.153 , pp. 2093
    • McEwen, B.S.1    Stellar, E.2
  • 95
    • 1842320564 scopus 로고
    • The Structure and Dynamics of the Psyche translation from Über die Energetik der Seele [On the Driving Force of the Soul]
    • Rascher, Zurich, Princeton Univ. Press, Princeton, NJ, ed. 2
    • The concept of limited energy within a hedonic system can be traced at least as far back as Carl Jung, where the psyche was regarded as a relatively closed system. This limited energy was expressed by the term "libido," which basically described a general life instinct or psychic energy. Jung wrote, "Since our experience is confined to relatively closed systems, we are never in a position to observe an absolute psychological entropy, but the more the psychological system is closed off, the more clearly is the phenomenon of entropy manifested (a system is absolutely closed when no energy from outside can be fed into it)" [C. G. Jung, The Structure and Dynamics of the Psyche (translation from Über die Energetik der Seele [On the Driving Force of the Soul], vol. 8 of Über psychische Energetik und das Wasen der Traume [On Psychological Energy and the Meaning of Dreams] (Rascher, Zurich, 1948)} (Princeton Univ. Press, Princeton, NJ, ed. 2, 1969)].
    • (1948) Über Psychische Energetik und das Wasen der Traume [On Psychological Energy and the Meaning of Dreams] , vol.8
    • Jung, C.G.1
  • 96
    • 0003530805 scopus 로고
    • Yale Univ. Press, New Haven and London
    • The theological system of Calvin and his followers is marked by a strong emphasis on the sovereignty of God, the depravity of humankind, and the doctrine of predestination. Calvinism is characterized by a strict, disciplined lifestyle where morality is tantamount and there is a strong sense of church unity. Calvinists, and later Puritans, with regard to personal life, demanded of themselves a reformation of character, the rejection of idle recreations and vain display, and sober, obedient godliness [S. E. Ahlstrom, A Religious History of the American People (Yale Univ. Press, New Haven and London, 1972].
    • (1972) A Religious History of the American People
    • Ahlstrom, S.E.1
  • 101
    • 1842279383 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Supported in part by NIH grants AA06420 and AA08459 (G.F.K.) from the National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism; NIH grants DA04043, DA04398, and DA08467 (G.F.K.) from the National Institute on Drug Abuse; and INSERM grants (M.L.M.). We thank the following individuals for their comments and discussions of the data and concepts discussed herein: S. Ahmed, M. Cador, V. Deroche, M. Heilig, C. Heyser, P. Karli, M. Lewis, A. Markou, P. Piazza, A. Roberts, G. Schulteis, G. Simonnet, T. Wall, and F. Weiss. We also thank P. Brennan, M. Arends, and the Molecular and Experimental Medicine Word Processing Unit (L. Miller and J. Robertson) for their help with manuscript preparation. This is manuscript number 11020-NP from The Scripps Research Institute.


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.