메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn , Issue , 2008, Pages 369-381

Human Cognitive Architecture

Author keywords

Cognitive load theory: An instructional design theory based on our knowledge of human cognitive architecture.; Human cognitive architecture: The manner in which structures and functions required for human cognitive processes are organized.; Long term memory: The store holding all knowledge acquired during the processes of learning.; Natural information processing system: The procedures by which natural systems such as human cognition and evolution by natural selection process information.; Working memory: The structure that processes information coming from either the environment or long term memory and that transfers learned information for storage in long term memory.

Indexed keywords


EID: 85144953984     PISSN: None     EISSN: None     Source Type: Book    
DOI: 10.4324/9780203880869-35     Document Type: Chapter
Times cited : (53)

References (47)
  • 1
    • 33646069390 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The split-attention principle
    • edited by R. E. Mayer, Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.*
    • Ayres, P. and Sweller, J. (2005). The split-attention principle. In Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning, edited by R. E. Mayer, pp. 135-136. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.*
    • (2005) Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning , pp. 135-136
    • Ayres, P.1    Sweller, J.2
  • 2
    • 0026554483 scopus 로고
    • Working memory
    • Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255, 556-559.*
    • (1992) Science , vol.255 , pp. 556-559
    • Baddeley, A.1
  • 4
    • 84948512218 scopus 로고
    • Cognitive load theory and the format of instruction
    • Chandler, P. and Sweller, J. (1991). Cognitive load theory and the format of instruction. Cogn. Instruct., 8, 293-332.
    • (1991) Cogn. Instruct. , vol.8 , pp. 293-332
    • Chandler, P.1    Sweller, J.2
  • 6
    • 0002816556 scopus 로고
    • Expertise in problem solving
    • edited by R. Sternberg, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
    • Chi, M., Glaser, R., and Rees, E. (1982). Expertise in problem solving. In Advances in the Psychology of Human Intelligence, edited by R. Sternberg, pp. 7-75. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    • (1982) Advances in the Psychology of Human Intelligence , pp. 7-75
    • Chi, M.1    Glaser, R.2    Rees, E.3
  • 8
    • 0001399281 scopus 로고
    • The effects of schema acquisition and rule automation on mathematical problem-solving transfer
    • Cooper, G. and Sweller, J. (1987). The effects of schema acquisition and rule automation on mathematical problem-solving transfer. J. Educ. Psychol., 79, 347-362.
    • (1987) J. Educ. Psychol. , vol.79 , pp. 347-362
    • Cooper, G.1    Sweller, J.2
  • 12
    • 0018450527 scopus 로고
    • Chunking in recall of symbolic drawings
    • Egan, D. E. and Schwartz B. J. (1979). Chunking in recall of symbolic drawings. Memory Cogn., 7, 149-158.
    • (1979) Memory Cogn. , vol.7 , pp. 149-158
    • Egan, D.E.1    Schwartz, B.J.2
  • 13
    • 0029287202 scopus 로고
    • Long-term working memory
    • Ericsson, K. A. and Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychol. Rev., 102, 211-245.*
    • (1995) Psychol. Rev. , vol.102 , pp. 211-245
    • Ericsson, K.A.1    Kintsch, W.2
  • 14
    • 0002402819 scopus 로고
    • Processes involved in designing software
    • edited by J. R. Anderson, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
    • Jeffries, R., Turner, A., Polson, P., and Atwood, M. (1981). Processes involved in designing software. In Cognitive Skills and Their Acquisition, edited by J. R. Anderson, pp. 255-283. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    • (1981) Cognitive Skills and Their Acquisition , pp. 255-283
    • Jeffries, R.1    Turner, A.2    Polson, P.3    Atwood, M.4
  • 15
    • 4544257209 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Measuring knowledge to optimize cognitive load factors during instruction
    • Kalyuga, S. and Sweller, J. (2004). Measuring knowledge to optimize cognitive load factors during instruction. J. Educ. Psychol., 96, 558-568.
    • (2004) J. Educ. Psychol. , vol.96 , pp. 558-568
    • Kalyuga, S.1    Sweller, J.2
  • 16
    • 0032032804 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Levels of expertise and instructional design
    • Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., and Sweller, J. (1998). Levels of expertise and instructional design. Hum. Factors, 40, 1-17.
    • (1998) Hum. Factors , vol.40 , pp. 1-17
    • Kalyuga, S.1    Chandler, P.2    Sweller, J.3
  • 17
    • 0034144894 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Incorporating learner experience into the design of multimedia instruction
    • Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., and Sweller, J. (2000). Incorporating learner experience into the design of multimedia instruction. J. Educ. Psychol., 92, 126-136.
    • (2000) J. Educ. Psychol. , vol.92 , pp. 126-136
    • Kalyuga, S.1    Chandler, P.2    Sweller, J.3
  • 18
    • 0035438174 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • When problem solving is superior to studying worked examples
    • Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., Tuovinen, J. E., and Sweller, J. (2001). When problem solving is superior to studying worked examples. J. Educ. Psychol., 93, 579-588.
    • (2001) J. Educ. Psychol. , vol.93 , pp. 579-588
    • Kalyuga, S.1    Chandler, P.2    Tuovinen, J.E.3    Sweller, J.4
  • 20
    • 33744807434 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential and inquiry-based teaching
    • Kirschner, P., Sweller, J., and Clark, R. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: an analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential and inquiry-based teaching. Educ. Psychol., 41, 75-86.
    • (2006) Educ. Psychol. , vol.41 , pp. 75-86
    • Kirschner, P.1    Sweller, J.2    Clark, R.3
  • 21
    • 7444269930 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The equivalence of learning paths in early science instruction: Effects of direct instruction and discovery learning
    • Klahr, D. and Nigam, M. (2004). The equivalence of learning paths in early science instruction: effects of direct instruction and discovery learning. Psychol. Sci., 15, 661-667.
    • (2004) Psychol. Sci. , vol.15 , pp. 661-667
    • Klahr, D.1    Nigam, M.2
  • 22
    • 30644472618 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Interactions among the imagination, expertise reversal and element interactivity effects
    • Leahy, W. and Sweller, J. (2005). Interactions among the imagination, expertise reversal and element interactivity effects. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl., 11, 266-276. *
    • (2005) J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. , vol.11 , pp. 266-276
    • Leahy, W.1    Sweller, J.2
  • 23
    • 33847031441 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The modality principle
    • edited by R. E. Mayer, Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press
    • Low, R. and Sweller, J. (2005). The modality principle. In Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning, edited by R. E. Mayer, pp. 147-158. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.
    • (2005) Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning , pp. 147-158
    • Low, R.1    Sweller, J.2
  • 24
    • 0742269292 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Should there be a three-strikes rule against pure discovery learning? The case for guided methods of instruction
    • Mayer, R. E. (2004). Should there be a three-strikes rule against pure discovery learning? The case for guided methods of instruction. Am. Psychol., 59, 14-19.
    • (2004) Am. Psychol. , vol.59 , pp. 14-19
    • Mayer, R.E.1
  • 25
    • 30644477627 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Cognitive theory of multimedia learning
    • edited by R. E. Mayer, Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.*
    • Mayer, R. E. (2005). Cognitive theory of multimedia learning. In Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning, edited by R. E. Mayer, pp. 31-48. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.*
    • (2005) Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning , pp. 31-48
    • Mayer, R.E.1
  • 26
    • 39749093168 scopus 로고
    • The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information
    • Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychol. Rev., 63, 81-97.
    • (1956) Psychol. Rev. , vol.63 , pp. 81-97
    • Miller, G.A.1
  • 27
    • 33846580222 scopus 로고
    • Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem solving skills: A cognitive-load approach
    • Paas, F. and van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (1994). Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem solving skills: a cognitive-load approach. J. Educ. Psychol., 86, 122-133.
    • (1994) J. Educ. Psychol. , vol.86 , pp. 122-133
    • Paas, F.1    van Merriënboer, J.J.G.2
  • 28
    • 0037412682 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Cognitive load theory and instructional design: Recent developments
    • Paas, F., Renkl, A., and Sweller, J. (2003). Cognitive load theory and instructional design: recent developments. Educ. Psychol., 38, 1-4.*
    • (2003) Educ. Psychol. , vol.38 , pp. 1-4
    • Paas, F.1    Renkl, A.2    Sweller, J.3
  • 29
    • 3843076391 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Cognitive load theory: Instructional implications of the interaction between information structures and cognitive architecture
    • Paas, F., Renkl, A., and Sweller, J. (2004). Cognitive load theory: instructional implications of the interaction between information structures and cognitive architecture. Instruct. Sci., 32, 1-8.
    • (2004) Instruct. Sci. , vol.32 , pp. 1-8
    • Paas, F.1    Renkl, A.2    Sweller, J.3
  • 31
    • 0037412702 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Structuring the transition from example study to problem solving in cognitive skill acquisition: A cognitive load perspective
    • Renkl, A. and Atkinson, R. (2003). Structuring the transition from example study to problem solving in cognitive skill acquisition: a cognitive load perspective. Educ. Psychol., 38, 15-22.
    • (2003) Educ. Psychol. , vol.38 , pp. 15-22
    • Renkl, A.1    Atkinson, R.2
  • 32
    • 3843105827 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • How fading worked solution steps works: A cognitive load perspective
    • Renkl, A., Atkinson, R., and Große, C. (2004). How fading worked solution steps works: a cognitive load perspective. Instruct. Sci., 32, 59-82.
    • (2004) Instruct. Sci. , vol.32 , pp. 59-82
    • Renkl, A.1    Atkinson, R.2    Große, C.3
  • 33
    • 0002788226 scopus 로고
    • A simulation of memory for chess positions
    • Simon, H. A. and Gilmartin, K. (1973). A simulation of memory for chess positions. Cogn. Psychol., 5, 29-46.
    • (1973) Cogn. Psychol. , vol.5 , pp. 29-46
    • Simon, H.A.1    Gilmartin, K.2
  • 34
    • 0000819688 scopus 로고
    • Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning
    • Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: effects on learning. Cogn. Sci., 12, 257-285.*
    • (1988) Cogn. Sci. , vol.12 , pp. 257-285
    • Sweller, J.1
  • 35
    • 43949152992 scopus 로고
    • Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty and instructional design
    • Sweller, J. (1994). Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty and instructional design. Learn. Instruct., 4, 295-312.*
    • (1994) Learn. Instruct. , vol.4 , pp. 295-312
    • Sweller, J.1
  • 36
    • 36148969361 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Evolution of human cognitive architecture
    • edited by B. Ross, San Diego, CA: Academic Press.*
    • Sweller, J. (2003). Evolution of human cognitive architecture. In The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 43, edited by B. Ross, pp. 215-266. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.*
    • (2003) The Psychology of Learning and Motivation , vol.43 , pp. 215-266
    • Sweller, J.1
  • 37
    • 3843137289 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Instructional design consequences of an analogy between evolution by natural selection and human cognitive architecture
    • Sweller, J. (2004). Instructional design consequences of an analogy between evolution by natural selection and human cognitive architecture. Instruct. Sci., 32, 9-31.
    • (2004) Instruct. Sci. , vol.32 , pp. 9-31
    • Sweller, J.1
  • 38
    • 30644477627 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Implications of cognitive load theory for multimedia learning
    • edited by R. E. Mayer, Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press
    • Sweller, J. (2005a). Implications of cognitive load theory for multimedia learning. In Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning, edited by R. E. Mayer, pp. 19-30. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.
    • (2005) Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning , pp. 19-30
    • Sweller, J.1
  • 39
    • 33847031441 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The redundancy principle
    • edited by R. E. Mayer, Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press
    • Sweller, J. (2005b). The redundancy principle. In Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning, edited by R. E. Mayer, pp. 159-167. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.
    • (2005) Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning , pp. 159-167
    • Sweller, J.1
  • 40
    • 84963474629 scopus 로고
    • The use of worked examples as a substitute for problem solving in learning algebra
    • Sweller, J. and Cooper, G. A. (1985). The use of worked examples as a substitute for problem solving in learning algebra. Cogn. Instruct., 2, 59-89.
    • (1985) Cogn. Instruct. , vol.2 , pp. 59-89
    • Sweller, J.1    Cooper, G.A.2
  • 41
    • 33847074719 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Natural information processing systems
    • Sweller, J. and Sweller, S. (2006). Natural information processing systems. Evol. Psychol., 4, 434-458.
    • (2006) Evol. Psychol. , vol.4 , pp. 434-458
    • Sweller, J.1    Sweller, S.2
  • 42
    • 0000584898 scopus 로고
    • Cognitive load and selective attention as factors in the structuring of technical material
    • Sweller, J., Chandler, P., Tierney, P., and Cooper, M. (1990). Cognitive load and selective attention as factors in the structuring of technical material. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., 119, 176-192.
    • (1990) J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. , vol.119 , pp. 176-192
    • Sweller, J.1    Chandler, P.2    Tierney, P.3    Cooper, M.4
  • 46
    • 18844441224 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Cognitive load theory and complex learning: Recent developments and future directions
    • Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. and Sweller, J. (2005). Cognitive load theory and complex learning: recent developments and future directions. Educ. Psychol. Rev., 17, 147-177.*
    • (2005) Educ. Psychol. Rev. , vol.17 , pp. 147-177
    • Van Merriënboer, J.J.G.1    Sweller, J.2
  • 47
    • 0002926013 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Redirecting learner’s attention during training: Effects on cognitive load, transfer test performance and training efficiency
    • Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., Schuurman, J., de Croock, M., and Paas, F. (2002). Redirecting learner’s attention during training: effects on cognitive load, transfer test performance and training efficiency. Learn. Instruct., 12, 11-37.
    • (2002) Learn. Instruct. , vol.12 , pp. 11-37
    • Van Merriënboer, J.J.G.1    Schuurman, J.2    de Croock, M.3    Paas, F.4


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.