메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 108, Issue 4, 2011, Pages 188-204

A mosquito bite against the enactive approach to bodily experiences

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords


EID: 80053480477     PISSN: 0022362X     EISSN: 19398549     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.5840/jphil2011108411     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (18)

References (22)
  • 1
    • 80053556390 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • This view has been defended by, among others
    • Cambridge: Harvard
    • This view has been defended by, among others, Susan Hurley in Consciousness in Action (Cambridge: Harvard, 1998)
    • (1998) Susan Hurley in Consciousness in Action
  • 2
    • 0035495009 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness
    • October
    • Kevin O'Regan and Alva Noë, "A Sensorimotor Account of Vision and Visual Consciousness," Behavioral and Brain Sciences, xxiv, 5 (October 2001): 939-1031
    • (2001) Behavioral and Brain Sciences , vol.24 , Issue.5 , pp. 939-1031
    • O'regan, K.1    Noë, A.2
  • 4
    • 33744772591 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Sensorimotor subjectivity and the enactive approach to experience
    • Evan Thompson, "Sensorimotor Subjectivity and the Enactive Approach to Experience," Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, iv, 4 (2005): 407-27.
    • (2005) Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences , vol.4 , Issue.4 , pp. 407-427
    • Thompson, E.1
  • 5
    • 60449092835 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Enactivism and the unity of perception and action
    • For a review of the differences between versions of the enactive theory of vision, see Nivedita Gangopadhyay and Julian Kiverstein, "Enactivism and the Unity of Perception and Action," Topoi, xxviii, 1 (2009): 63-73.
    • (2009) Topoi , vol.28 , Issue.1 , pp. 63-73
    • Gangopadhyay, N.1    Kiverstein, J.2
  • 6
    • 43249126108 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The scope and limits of the enactive approaches to visual experiences
    • February
    • Pierre Jacob, "The Scope and Limits of the Enactive Approaches to Visual Experiences," Perception, xxxvii, 3 (February 2008): 446-61.
    • (2008) Perception , vol.37 , Issue.3 , pp. 446-461
    • Jacob, P.1
  • 7
    • 77955564581 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Attention and sensorimotor intentionality
    • David Woodruff Smith and Amie L. Thomasson, eds (New York: Oxford)
    • For a detailed account of the notion of bodily know-how, see Charles Siewert, "Attention and Sensorimotor Intentionality," in David Woodruff Smith and Amie L. Thomasson, eds., Phenomenology and Philosophy of Mind (New York: Oxford, 2005), pp. 270-94.
    • (2005) Phenomenology and Philosophy of Mind , pp. 270-294
    • Siewert, C.1
  • 8
    • 85038504628 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • What we perceive is determined by⋯what we know how to do
    • "What we perceive is determined by⋯what we know how to do." Noë, Action in Perception, p. 1.
    • Noë, Action in Perception , pp. 1
  • 9
    • 0003802772 scopus 로고
    • New York: Oxford
    • For a comprehensive review of the empirical literature that supports the so-called Perception-Action model of vision, see David Milner and Mel Goodale, The Visual Brain in Action (New York: Oxford, 1995)
    • (1995) The Visual Brain in Action
    • Milner, D.1    Goodale, M.2
  • 11
    • 43249088667 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Review of alva noë, action in perception
    • May
    • Ned Block, "Review of Alva Noë, Action in Perception," this journal, cii, 5 (May 2005): 259-72
    • (2005) This Journal , vol.102 , Issue.5 , pp. 259-272
    • Block, N.1
  • 13
    • 0003700364 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Cambridge: Blackwell
    • Some hand movements can count as perceptual reports. For instance, it was shown that the underlying processes of scaling the distance between thumb and index finger are very different, depending on whether it is for reporting a perceptual judgment or for the visuomotor task of grasping. Jeannerod, The Cognitive Neuroscience of Action (Cambridge: Blackwell, 1997).
    • (1997) The Cognitive Neuroscience of Action
    • Jeannerod1
  • 14
    • 0020595346 scopus 로고
    • Localization without content: A tactile analogue of 'blind sight'
    • For numbsense patients, see Jacques Paillard, Francois Michel, and George Stelmach, "Localization without Content: A Tactile Analogue of 'Blind Sight'," Archives of Neurology, xl, 9 (1983): 548-51
    • (1983) Archives of Neurology , vol.11 , Issue.9 , pp. 548-551
    • Paillard, J.1    Michel, F.2    Stelmach, G.3
  • 15
    • 0028821598 scopus 로고
    • Implicit processing of somaesthetic information: A dissociation between where and how?
    • February
    • as well as Yves Rossetti, Gilles Rode, and Dominique Boisson, "Implicit Processing of Somaesthetic Information: A Dissociation between Where and How?" Neuroreport, vi, 3 (February 1995): 506-10.
    • (1995) Neuroreport , vol.6 , Issue.3 , pp. 506-510
    • Rossetti, Y.1    Rode, G.2    Boisson, D.3
  • 16
    • 63049083395 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A double dissociation between somatosensory processing for perception and action
    • May
    • For a description of KE and JO, see Helen A. Anema et al., "A Double Dissociation between Somatosensory Processing for Perception and Action," Neuropsychologia, xlvii, 6 (May 2009): 1615-20.
    • (2009) Neuropsychologia , vol.47 , Issue.6 , pp. 1615-1620
    • Anema, H.A.1
  • 18
    • 0032545925 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Rubber hands 'feel' touch that eyes see
    • February 19
    • For a description of the standard Rubber Hand Illusion, see Matthew Botvinick and Jonathan Cohen, "Rubber Hands 'Feel' Touch that Eyes See," Nature, cccxci (February 19, 1998): 756.
    • (1998) Nature , vol.391 , pp. 756
    • Botvinick, M.1    Cohen, J.2
  • 19
    • 0034237517 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Aspects of body self-calibration
    • July 1
    • For a description of the standard vibrotactile illusion, see James R. Lackner and Paul A. DiZio, "Aspects of Body Self-Calibration," Trends in Cognitive Sciences, iv, 7 ( July 1, 2000): 279-88.
    • (2000) Trends in Cognitive Sciences , vol.4 , Issue.7 , pp. 279-288
    • Lackner, J.R.1    Dizio, P.A.2
  • 20
    • 33745955825 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Dissociating body representations in healthy individuals: Differential effects of a kinaesthetic illusion on perception and action
    • The immunity of action to the vibrotactile illusion was shown by M. P. Kammers, I. J. van der Ham, and H. C. Dijkerman, "Dissociating Body Representations in Healthy Individuals: Differential Effects of a Kinaesthetic Illusion on Perception and Action," Neuropsychologia, xliv, 12 (2006): 2430-36.
    • (2006) Neuropsychologia , vol.44 , Issue.12 , pp. 2430-2436
    • Kammers, M.P.1    Ham Der Van, I.J.2    Dijkerman, H.C.3
  • 21
    • 57249095887 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The rubber hand illusion in action
    • January
    • The immunity of action to the Rubber Hand Illusion was shown by Kammers, F. de Vignemont, L. Verhagen, and Dijkerman, "The Rubber Hand Illusion in Action," Neuropsychologia, xlvii, 1 ( January 2009): 204-11.
    • (2009) Neuropsychologia , vol.57 , Issue.1 , pp. 204-211
    • Kammers, F.1    De Vignemont, L.V.2    Dijkerman3
  • 22
    • 84920871246 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Vision without representation
    • Gangopadhyay, Michael Madary, and Finn Spicer, eds (New York: Oxford)
    • For this line of argument see, for instance, Noë, "Vision without Representation," in Gangopadhyay, Michael Madary, and Finn Spicer, eds., Perception, Action, and Consciousness: Sensorimotor Dynamics and Two Visual Systems (New York: Oxford, 2010), pp. 245-56.
    • (2010) Perception, Action, and Consciousness: Sensorimotor Dynamics and Two Visual Systems , pp. 245-256
    • Noë1


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.