메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 42, Issue 1, 2010, Pages 82-95

Norms for two types of manipulability (graspability and functional usage), familiarity, and age of acquisition for 320 photographs of objects

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords

ADOLESCENT; ADULT; AGE; ARTICLE; FEMALE; HAND STRENGTH; HUMAN; LEARNING; MALE; RECOGNITION; SEMANTICS;

EID: 77952525705     PISSN: 1554351X     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.3758/BRM.42.1.82     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (44)

References (25)
  • 2
    • 0038473978 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A new set of 299 pictures for psycholinguistic studies: French norms for name agreement, image agreement, conceptual familiarity, visual complexity, image variability, age of acquisition, and naming latencies
    • Bonin, P., Peereman, R., Malardier, N., Méot, A., & Chalard, M. (2003). A new set of 299 pictures for psycholinguistic studies: French norms for name agreement, image agreement, conceptual familiarity, visual complexity, image variability, age of acquisition, and naming latencies. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 158-167.
    • (2003) Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers , vol.35 , pp. 158-167
    • Bonin, P.1    Peereman, R.2    Malardier, N.3    Méot, A.4    Chalard, M.5
  • 3
    • 0037810247 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Category specificity and feature knowledge: Evidence from new sensory-quality categories
    • doi:10.1080/02643290244000310
    • Borgo, F., & Shallice, T. (2003). Category specificity and feature knowledge: Evidence from new sensory-quality categories. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 327-353. doi:10.1080/02643290244000310
    • (2003) Cognitive Neuropsychology , vol.20 , pp. 327-353
    • Borgo, F.1    Shallice, T.2
  • 5
    • 33645766268 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Human parietal cortex in action
    • doi:10.1016/j.conb.2006.03.005
    • Culham, J. C., & Valyear, K. F. (2006). Human parietal cortex in action. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16, 205-212. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2006.03.005
    • (2006) Current Opinion In Neurobiology , vol.16 , pp. 205-212
    • Culham, J.C.1    Valyear, K.F.2
  • 6
    • 40949145707 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Age of acquisition and word frequency effects in picture naming: A dual-task investigation
    • doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.05.005
    • Dent, K., Johnston, R. A., & Humphreys, G. W. (2008). Age of acquisition and word frequency effects in picture naming: A dual-task investigation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 34, 282-301. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.05.005
    • (2008) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition , vol.34 , pp. 282-301
    • Dent, K.1    Johnston, R.A.2    Humphreys, G.W.3
  • 7
    • 0026019228 scopus 로고
    • Can recognition of living things be selectively impaired?
    • doi:10.1016/0028-3932(91)90020-9
    • Farah, M. J., McMullen, P. A., & Meyer, M. M. (1991). Can recognition of living things be selectively impaired? Neuropsychologia, 29, 185-193. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(91)90020-9
    • (1991) Neuropsychologia , vol.29 , pp. 185-193
    • Farah, M.J.1    McMullen, P.A.2    Meyer, M.M.3
  • 8
    • 0000022218 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The living/nonliving dissociation is not an artifact: Giving an a priori implausible hypothesis a strong test
    • doi:10.1080/026432996382097
    • Farah, M. J., Meyer, M. M., & McMullen, P. A. (1996). The living/nonliving dissociation is not an artifact: Giving an a priori implausible hypothesis a strong test. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 137-154. doi:10.1080/026432996382097
    • (1996) Cognitive Neuropsychology , vol.13 , pp. 137-154
    • Farah, M.J.1    Meyer, M.M.2    McMullen, P.A.3
  • 9
    • 11144328862 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Manipulability and living/non-living category effects on object recognition
    • doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2004.08.022
    • Filliter, J. H., McMullen, P. A., & Westwood, D. (2005). Manipulability and living/non-living category effects on object recognition. Brain & Cognition, 57, 61-65. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2004.08.022
    • (2005) Brain & Cognition , vol.57 , pp. 61-65
    • Filliter, J.H.1    McMullen, P.A.2    Westwood, D.3
  • 10
    • 0027467139 scopus 로고
    • A spurious category-specific visual agnosia for living things in normal human and nonhuman primates
    • doi:10.1162/jocn.1993.5.1.118
    • Gaffan, D., & Heywood, C. A. (1993). A spurious category-specific visual agnosia for living things in normal human and nonhuman primates. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 118-128. doi:10.1162/jocn.1993.5.1.118
    • (1993) Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , vol.5 , pp. 118-128
    • Gaffan, D.1    Heywood, C.A.2
  • 11
    • 33646509576 scopus 로고
    • Age-of-acquisition, imagery, concreteness, familiarity, and ambiguity measures for 1,944 words
    • Gilhooly, K. J., & Logie, R. H. (1980). Age-of-acquisition, imagery, concreteness, familiarity, and ambiguity measures for 1,944 words. Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation, 12, 395-427.
    • (1980) Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation , vol.12 , pp. 395-427
    • Gilhooly, K.J.1    Logie, R.H.2
  • 12
    • 0036028002 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a neuroimaging study
    • Grèzes, J., & Decety, J. (2002). Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a neuroimaging study. Neuropsychologia, 40, 212-222.
    • (2002) Neuropsychologia , vol.40 , pp. 212-222
    • Grèzes, J.1    Decety, J.2
  • 13
    • 0041834331 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Objects, affordances ... action!
    • Humphreys, G. W. (2001). Objects, affordances ... action! The Psychologist, 14, 408-412.
    • (2001) The Psychologist , vol.14 , pp. 408-412
    • Humphreys, G.W.1
  • 14
    • 0035152957 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Detection by action: Neuropsychological evidence for action-defined templates in search
    • doi:10.1038/82940
    • Humphreys, G. W., & Riddoch, M. J. (2001). Detection by action: Neuropsychological evidence for action-defined templates in search. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 84-88. doi:10.1038/82940
    • (2001) Nature Neuroscience , vol.4 , pp. 84-88
    • Humphreys, G.W.1    Riddoch, M.J.2
  • 15
    • 0035238889 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Reevaluating age of acquisition effects: Are they simply cumulative frequency effects?
    • doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00117-7
    • Lewis, M. B., Gerhand, S., & Ellis, H. D. (2001). Reevaluating age of acquisition effects: Are they simply cumulative frequency effects? Cognition, 78, 189-205. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00117-7
    • (2001) Cognition , vol.78 , pp. 189-205
    • Lewis, M.B.1    Gerhand, S.2    Ellis, H.D.3
  • 16
    • 0038189574 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The Snodgrass and Vanderwart set revisited: Norms for object manipulability and for pictorial ambiguity of objects, chimeric objects, and nonobjects
    • doi:10.1076/jcen.25.4.521.13873
    • Magnié, M. N., Besson, M., Poncet, M., & Dolisi, C. (2003). The Snodgrass and Vanderwart set revisited: Norms for object manipulability and for pictorial ambiguity of objects, chimeric objects, and nonobjects. Journal of Clinical & Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 521-560. doi:10.1076/jcen.25.4.521.13873
    • (2003) Journal of Clinical & Experimental Neuropsychology , vol.25 , pp. 521-560
    • Magnié, M.N.1    Besson, M.2    Poncet, M.3    Dolisi, C.4
  • 17
    • 0031536184 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Age of acquisition norms for a large set of object names and their relation to adult estimates and other variables
    • doi:10.1080/027249897392017
    • Morrison, C. M., Chappell, T. D., & Ellis, A. W. (1997). Age of acquisition norms for a large set of object names and their relation to adult estimates and other variables. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A, 528-559. doi:10.1080/027249897392017
    • (1997) Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , vol.50 A , pp. 528-559
    • Morrison, C.M.1    Chappell, T.D.2    Ellis, A.W.3
  • 18
    • 33845466648 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Age of acquisition persists as the main factor in picture naming when cumulative word frequency and frequency trajectory are controlled
    • Pérez, M. A. (2007). Age of acquisition persists as the main factor in picture naming when cumulative word frequency and frequency trajectory are controlled. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 32-42.
    • (2007) Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , vol.60 , pp. 32-42
    • Pérez, M.A.1
  • 19
    • 0024378629 scopus 로고
    • Routes to action: Evidence from apraxia
    • doi:10.1080/02643298908253424
    • Riddoch, M. J., Humphreys, G. W., & Price, C. J. (1989). Routes to action: Evidence from apraxia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 6, 437-454. doi:10.1080/02643298908253424
    • (1989) Cognitive Neuropsychology , vol.6 , pp. 437-454
    • Riddoch, M.J.1    Humphreys, G.W.2    Price, C.J.3
  • 20
    • 0018991099 scopus 로고
    • A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity and visual complexity
    • doi:10.1037/0278-7393.6.2.174
    • Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory, 6, 174-215. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.6.2.174
    • (1980) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory , vol.6 , pp. 174-215
    • Snodgrass, J.G.1    Vanderwart, M.2
  • 21
    • 47849092284 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The development of reaching behavior in low-risk preterm infants
    • doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.12.006
    • Toledo, A. M., & Tudella, E. (2008). The development of reaching behavior in low-risk preterm infants. Infant Behavior & Development, 31, 398-407. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.12.006
    • (2008) Infant Behavior & Development , vol.31 , pp. 398-407
    • Toledo, A.M.1    Tudella, E.2
  • 22
    • 0021140542 scopus 로고
    • Category specific semantic impairments
    • Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1984). Category specific semantic impairments. Brain, 107, 829-854.
    • (1984) Brain , vol.107 , pp. 829-854
    • Warrington, E.K.1    Shallice, T.2
  • 23
    • 20444507159 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The development of prospective grasping control between 5 and 7 months: A longitudinal study
    • doi:10.1207/s15327078in0702_2
    • Witherington, D. C. (2005). The development of prospective grasping control between 5 and 7 months: A longitudinal study. Infancy, 7, 143-161. doi:10.1207/s15327078in0702_2
    • (2005) Infancy , vol.7 , pp. 143-161
    • Witherington, D.C.1
  • 24
    • 19344376853 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The role of sensory-motor information in object recognition: Evidence from the category-specific visual agnosia
    • doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2004.10.015
    • Wolk, D. A., Coslett, H. B., & Glosser, G. (2005). The role of sensory-motor information in object recognition: Evidence from the category-specific visual agnosia. Brain & Language, 94, 131-146. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2004.10.015
    • (2005) Brain & Language , vol.94 , pp. 131-146
    • Wolk, D.A.1    Coslett, H.B.2    Glosser, G.3
  • 25
    • 0036261939 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Modeling direct perceptual constraints on action selection: The Naming and Action Model (NAM)
    • doi:10.1080/13506280143000601
    • Yoon, E. Y., Heinke, D., & Humphreys, G. W. (2002). Modeling direct perceptual constraints on action selection: The Naming and Action Model (NAM). Visual Cognition, 9, 615-661. doi:10.1080/13506280143000601
    • (2002) Visual Cognition , vol.9 , pp. 615-661
    • Yoon, E.Y.1    Heinke, D.2    Humphreys, G.W.3


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.