메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 17, Issue 2, 2007, Pages 143-160

Reducing poverty through preschool interventions

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords

CHILD CARE; CURRICULUM; LOWEST INCOME GROUP; POVERTY; REVIEW; SPECIAL EDUCATION; TEACHER; ARTICLE; ECONOMICS; HUMAN; LEGAL ASPECT; PRESCHOOL CHILD; UNITED STATES;

EID: 35449005638     PISSN: 10548289     EISSN: 15501558     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1353/foc.2007.0015     Document Type: Review
Times cited : (79)

References (78)
  • 1
    • 0034346795 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Neural Plasticity and Human Development: The Role of Early Experience in Sculpting Memory Systems
    • Charles A. Nelson, "Neural Plasticity and Human Development: The Role of Early Experience in Sculpting Memory Systems," Developmental Science 3, no. 2 (2000): 115-36;
    • (2000) Developmental Science , vol.3 , Issue.2 , pp. 115-136
    • Nelson, C.A.1
  • 3
    • 0037591895 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Investing in the Future: Reducing Poverty through Human Capital Programs
    • edited by Sheldon H. Danziger and Robert H. Haveman Harvard University Press
    • Lynn Karoly, "Investing in the Future: Reducing Poverty through Human Capital Programs," in Understanding Poverty in America: Progress and Problems, edited by Sheldon H. Danziger and Robert H. Haveman (Harvard University Press, 2002).
    • (2002) Understanding Poverty in America: Progress and Problems
    • Karoly, L.1
  • 9
    • 0042416253 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Christopher Jencks and Meredith Phillips, eds, Brookings
    • Christopher Jencks and Meredith Phillips, eds., The Black-White Test Score Gap (Brookings, 1998);
    • (1998) The Black-White Test Score Gap
  • 10
    • 2442528264 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Understanding the Black-White Test Score Gap in the First Two Years of School
    • Roland Fryer and Steven D. Levitt, "Understanding the Black-White Test Score Gap in the First Two Years of School," Review of Economics and Statistics 86, no. 2 (2004): 447-64;
    • (2004) Review of Economics and Statistics , vol.86 , Issue.2 , pp. 447-464
    • Fryer, R.1    Levitt, S.D.2
  • 13
    • 28044471577 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Assessment Issues in the Testing of Children at School Entry
    • Donald A. Rock and A. Jackson Stenner, "Assessment Issues in the Testing of Children at School Entry," Future of Children 15, no. 1 (2005): 15-34;
    • (2005) Future of Children , vol.15 , Issue.1 , pp. 15-34
    • Rock, D.A.1    Jackson Stenner, A.2
  • 14
    • 28044471801 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The Contribution of Parenting to Ethnic and Racial Gaps in School Readiness
    • J. Brooks-Gunn and L. B. Markman, "The Contribution of Parenting to Ethnic and Racial Gaps in School Readiness," Future of Children 15, no. 1 (2005).
    • (2005) Future of Children , vol.15 , Issue.1
    • Brooks-Gunn, J.1    Markman, L.B.2
  • 15
    • 84858453652 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Neural Plasticity and Human Development (see note 1); National Scientific Council on the Developing Child,
    • Working Paper 3 , February
    • Nelson, "Neural Plasticity and Human Development" (see note 1); National Scientific Council on the Developing Child, "Excessive Stress Disrupts the Architecture of the Developing Brain," Working Paper 3 (2005), www.developingchild.net/pubs/wp/excessive_stress.pdf (February 2007).
    • (2005) Excessive Stress Disrupts the Architecture of the Developing Brain
    • Nelson1
  • 16
    • 0034076609 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Emotion Circuits in the Brain
    • J. LeDoux, "Emotion Circuits in the Brain," Annual Review of Neuroscience 23 (2000): 155-84.
    • (2000) Annual Review of Neuroscience , vol.23 , pp. 155-184
    • LeDoux, J.1
  • 17
    • 4644292897 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Physical Aggression during Early Childhood: Trajectories and Predictors
    • Richard Tremblay and others, "Physical Aggression during Early Childhood: Trajectories and Predictors," Pediatrics 114, no. 1 (2004): e43-50;
    • (2004) Pediatrics , vol.114 , Issue.1
    • Tremblay, R.1    and others2
  • 20
    • 84954125889 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Self-Regulation across Differing Risk and Sociocultural Contexts: Preliminary Findings from the Chicago School Readiness Project
    • paper presented at the, Atlanta, April
    • Cybele C. Raver and others, "Self-Regulation across Differing Risk and Sociocultural Contexts: Preliminary Findings from the Chicago School Readiness Project," paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Atlanta, April 2005.
    • (2005) biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development
    • Raver, C.C.1    and others2
  • 25
    • 36549069081 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Educational Achievement and Black-White Inequality: The Great Unknown
    • For a discussion of measurement issues, see
    • For a discussion of measurement issues, see Jens Ludwig, "Educational Achievement and Black-White Inequality: The Great Unknown," Education Next 3, no. 3 (2003): 79-82.
    • (2003) Education Next , vol.3 , Issue.3 , pp. 79-82
    • Ludwig, J.1
  • 26
    • 84858461070 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • According to U.S. Budget, Fiscal Year 2005, the United States now spends more than $530 billion a year on elementary and secondary schooling for children aged five and older, including $13 billion in extra federal funding through the Title I program for schools serving poor children. In contrast, the federal government spends only about $18 billion on the Head Start program and child care subsidies, most of which go to preschoolers; see testimony of Douglas J. Besharov before the Subcommittee on 21st Century Competitiveness of the Committee on Education and the Workforce, February 27, 2002, www.welfareacademy.org/pubs/testimony-022702.pdf (February 2007).
    • According to U.S. Budget, Fiscal Year 2005, the United States now spends more than $530 billion a year on elementary and secondary schooling for children aged five and older, including $13 billion in extra federal funding through the Title I program for schools serving poor children. In contrast, the federal government spends only about $18 billion on the Head Start program and child care subsidies, most of which go to preschoolers; see testimony of Douglas J. Besharov before the Subcommittee on 21st Century Competitiveness of the Committee on Education and the Workforce, February 27, 2002, www.welfareacademy.org/pubs/testimony-022702.pdf (February 2007).
  • 27
    • 36549013046 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Standard deviation units are a common way of expressing effect sizes. For comparison, the standard deviation for a typical IQ test is 15-16 points, and for the SAT, 100 points.
    • Standard deviation units are a common way of expressing effect sizes. For comparison, the standard deviation for a typical IQ test is 15-16 points, and for the SAT, 100 points.
  • 29
    • 20244372126 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Early Childhood Education: Young Adult Outcomes from the Abecedarian Project
    • Frances A. Campbell and others, "Early Childhood Education: Young Adult Outcomes from the Abecedarian Project," Applied Developmental Science 6, no. 1 (2002): 42-57;
    • (2002) Applied Developmental Science , vol.6 , Issue.1 , pp. 42-57
    • Campbell, F.A.1    and others2
  • 30
    • 33845877469 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Comparative Benefit-Cost Analysis of the Abecedarian Program and Its Policy Implications
    • forthcoming
    • Steven Barnett and Leonard Masse, "Comparative Benefit-Cost Analysis of the Abecedarian Program and Its Policy Implications," Economics of Education Review (2007, forthcoming);
    • (2007) Economics of Education Review
    • Barnett, S.1    Masse, L.2
  • 31
    • 84938050759 scopus 로고
    • Compensatory Education for Disadvantaged Children
    • Craig Ramey and Frances Campbell, "Compensatory Education for Disadvantaged Children," School Review 87, no. 2 (1979): 171-89.
    • (1979) School Review , vol.87 , Issue.2 , pp. 171-189
    • Ramey, C.1    Campbell, F.2
  • 32
    • 0000642099 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The cost estimate is in 2005 dollars. See Janet Currie, Early Childhood Education Programs, Journal of Economic Perspectives 15, no. 2 (2001): 213-38.
    • The cost estimate is in 2005 dollars. See Janet Currie, "Early Childhood Education Programs," Journal of Economic Perspectives 15, no. 2 (2001): 213-38.
  • 33
    • 0021354874 scopus 로고
    • Preventive Education for High-Risk Children: Cognitive Consequences of the Carolina Abecedarian Project
    • Craig T. Ramey and Frances A. Campbell, "Preventive Education for High-Risk Children: Cognitive Consequences of the Carolina Abecedarian Project," American Journal of Mental Deficiency 88, no. 5 (1984): 515-23.
    • (1984) American Journal of Mental Deficiency , vol.88 , Issue.5 , pp. 515-523
    • Ramey, C.T.1    Campbell, F.A.2
  • 35
    • 36549043335 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • In addition, criminal involvement was less common for treatments than controls 14 percent versus 18 percent for misdemeanor convictions, and 8 percent versus 12 percent for felony convictions, although the absolute numbers of those arrested in the two Abecedarian groups were small enough that it is impossible to prove statistically that this particular difference did not result from chance
    • In addition, criminal involvement was less common for treatments than controls (14 percent versus 18 percent for misdemeanor convictions, and 8 percent versus 12 percent for felony convictions), although the absolute numbers of those arrested in the two Abecedarian groups were small enough that it is impossible to prove statistically that this particular difference did not result from chance.
  • 37
    • 36549000135 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Michael Puma and others, Head Start Impact Study: First Year Findings (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families, 2005, Note that the point estimates we report in the text are larger than those in this report. The Head Start report presents the difference between average outcomes for all children assigned to the treatment group and all children assigned to the control group, known in the program evaluation literature as the intent to treat (ITT) effect. But not all children assigned to the experimental group participated in Head Start (the figure is around 84 percent, while some children (18 percent) assigned to the control group enrolled in the program. If we divide the ITT effect by the difference between the treatment and control groups in Head Start participation 66 percent, the implied effect of Head Start participation on participants is around 1.5 times as large as the ITT effects presented in Puma and others' r
    • Michael Puma and others, Head Start Impact Study: First Year Findings (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families, 2005). Note that the point estimates we report in the text are larger than those in this report. The Head Start report presents the difference between average outcomes for all children assigned to the treatment group and all children assigned to the control group, known in the program evaluation literature as the "intent to treat" (ITT) effect. But not all children assigned to the experimental group participated in Head Start (the figure is around 84 percent), while some children (18 percent) assigned to the control group enrolled in the program. If we divide the ITT effect by the difference between the treatment and control groups in Head Start participation (66 percent), the implied effect of Head Start participation on participants is around 1.5 times as large as the ITT effects presented in Puma and others' report.
  • 38
    • 84970443047 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • For a discussion of this methodology, see H. S. Bloom, Accounting for No-Shows in Experimental Evaluation Designs, Evaluation Review 8 (1984): 225-46. If we define the treatment more broadly, as participation in any center-based care, the effects of Head Start participation may be up to 2.5 times as large as the ITT impacts reported by Puma and others, since more than 96 percent of the treatment group receives some sort of center-based care in the experiment but so does around 53-60 percent of the control group (see exhibits 3.2 and 3.3 in Puma and others' report).
    • For a discussion of this methodology, see H. S. Bloom, "Accounting for No-Shows in Experimental Evaluation Designs," Evaluation Review 8 (1984): 225-46. If we define the "treatment" more broadly, as participation in any center-based care, the effects of Head Start participation may be up to 2.5 times as large as the ITT impacts reported by Puma and others, since more than 96 percent of the treatment group receives some sort of center-based care in the experiment but so does around 53-60 percent of the control group (see exhibits 3.2 and 3.3 in Puma and others' report).
  • 39
    • 36549059040 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • For more on our calculations, see Jens Ludwig and Deborah Phillips, The Benefits and Costs of Head Start, Working Paper 12973 Cambridge, Mass, National Bureau of Economic Research, 2007
    • For more on our calculations, see Jens Ludwig and Deborah Phillips, "The Benefits and Costs of Head Start," Working Paper 12973 (Cambridge, Mass.: National Bureau of Economic Research, 2007).
  • 40
    • 36549011154 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The Early Head Start initiative serves children under age three in a mix of home and center-based programs. A rigorous evaluation of the Early Head Start program found some evidence that the program had positive effects on some aspects of children's development and of parenting practices, but in general the effects were smaller than those produced by the Head Start program. See John M. Love and others, Making a Difference in the Lives of Infants and Toddlers and Their Families: The Impacts of Early Head Start U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families, 2002
    • The Early Head Start initiative serves children under age three in a mix of home and center-based programs. A rigorous evaluation of the Early Head Start program found some evidence that the program had positive effects on some aspects of children's development and of parenting practices, but in general the effects were smaller than those produced by the Head Start program. See John M. Love and others, Making a Difference in the Lives of Infants and Toddlers and Their Families: The Impacts of Early Head Start (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families, 2002).
  • 42
    • 0000331222 scopus 로고
    • Does Head Start Make a Difference?
    • Janet Currie and Duncan Thomas, "Does Head Start Make a Difference?" American Economic Review 85, no. 3 (1995): 341-64;
    • (1995) American Economic Review , vol.85 , Issue.3 , pp. 341-364
    • Currie, J.1    Thomas, D.2
  • 43
    • 0040080288 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Longer-Term Effects of Head Start
    • Eliana Garces, Duncan Thomas, and Janet Currie, "Longer-Term Effects of Head Start," American Economic Review 92, no. 4 (2002): 999-1012;
    • (2002) American Economic Review , vol.92 , Issue.4 , pp. 999-1012
    • Garces, E.1    Thomas, D.2    Currie, J.3
  • 44
    • 33947690662 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Does Head Start Improve Children's Life Chances? Evidence from a Regression-Discontinuity Design
    • Jens Ludwig and Douglas L. Miller, "Does Head Start Improve Children's Life Chances? Evidence from a Regression-Discontinuity Design," Quarterly Journal of Economics 122, no. 1 (2007): 159-208.
    • (2007) Quarterly Journal of Economics , vol.122 , Issue.1 , pp. 159-208
    • Ludwig, J.1    Miller, D.L.2
  • 46
    • 24744454636 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Promoting School Readiness in Oklahoma: An Evaluation of Tulsa's Pre-K Program
    • William T. Gormley and Ted Gayer, "Promoting School Readiness in Oklahoma: An Evaluation of Tulsa's Pre-K Program," Journal of Human Resources 40, no. 3 (2005): 533-58;
    • (2005) Journal of Human Resources , vol.40 , Issue.3 , pp. 533-558
    • Gormley, W.T.1    Gayer, T.2
  • 47
    • 28444482174 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The Effects of Universal Pre-K on Cognitive Development
    • William T. Gormley Jr. and others, "The Effects of Universal Pre-K on Cognitive Development," Developmental Psychology 41, no. 6 (2005): 872-84.
    • (2005) Developmental Psychology , vol.41 , Issue.6 , pp. 872-884
    • Gormley Jr., W.T.1    and others2
  • 51
    • 36549086396 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Katie Hamm and Danielle Ewen, Still Going Strong: Head Start Children, Families, Staff, and Programs in 2004, Head Start Series Policy Brief 6 (Center for Law and Policy, November 2005), www.clasp.org/publications/ headstart_brief_6.pdf (February 2007);
    • Katie Hamm and Danielle Ewen, "Still Going Strong: Head Start Children, Families, Staff, and Programs in 2004," Head Start Series Policy Brief 6 (Center for Law and Policy, November 2005), www.clasp.org/publications/ headstart_brief_6.pdf (February 2007);
  • 53
    • 84858468035 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Northwestern University
    • Thomas Cook, Northwestern University, PowerPoint presentation, www.northwestern.edu/ipr/events/briefingdec06-cook/slide16.html.
    • PowerPoint presentation
    • Cook, T.1
  • 54
    • 36549030677 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Specifically, these recent studies all use a regression discontinuity design that compares fall semester tests for kindergarten children who participated in pre-K the previous year and have birthdates close to the previous year's enrollment cutoff with fall tests of children who are currently starting pre-K because their birthdates just barely excluded them from participating the previous year. The key assumption behind these studies is that the selection of children into pre-K does not change dramatically by child age around the birthday enrollment cutoff that is, it changes smoothly with child age, But this need not be the case, because there is a discrete change at the birthday threshold in terms of the choice set that families face in making the decision to select pre-K. Suppose, for instance, that among the children whose birthdays just barely excluded them from enrolling in pre-K during the previous year, those with the most motivated parents were instead sent to pr
    • Specifically, these recent studies all use a regression discontinuity design that compares fall semester tests for kindergarten children who participated in pre-K the previous year and have birthdates close to the previous year's enrollment cutoff with fall tests of children who are currently starting pre-K because their birthdates just barely excluded them from participating the previous year. The key assumption behind these studies is that the selection of children into pre-K does not change dramatically by child age around the birthday enrollment cutoff (that is, it changes "smoothly" with child age). But this need not be the case, because there is a discrete change at the birthday threshold in terms of the choice set that families face in making the decision to select pre-K. Suppose, for instance, that among the children whose birthdays just barely excluded them from enrolling in pre-K during the previous year, those with the most motivated parents were instead sent to private programs that are analogous to the public pre-K program that year and are then enrolled in private kindergarten programs in the fall semester when the pre-K study outcome measures are collected. This type of selection would reduce the share of more motivated parents among the control group in the pre-K studies and lead them to overstate the benefits of pre-K participation.
  • 55
    • 36549081807 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Evaluations do not produce definitive evidence on the importance of the parental outreach component of early childhood education programs. The outreach we propose is more modest than Perry's, which involved home visits, although it does build the connections between classroom teachers and parents
    • Evaluations do not produce definitive evidence on the importance of the parental outreach component of early childhood education programs. The outreach we propose is more modest than Perry's, which involved home visits, although it does build the connections between classroom teachers and parents.
  • 56
    • 36549016018 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A variety of developmental and academic curricula have been developed for preschool programs, but since few have been evaluated rigorously it is hard to compare the relative effectiveness of these programs. The Institute for Educational Sciences is currently sponsoring a number of evaluations, the findings of which should help guide the selection of the national curriculum for our proposed program
    • A variety of developmental and academic curricula have been developed for preschool programs, but since few have been evaluated rigorously it is hard to compare the relative effectiveness of these programs. The Institute for Educational Sciences is currently sponsoring a number of evaluations, the findings of which should help guide the selection of the national curriculum for our proposed program.
  • 57
    • 29144432698 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The National Randomized Field Trial of Success for All: Second-Year Outcomes
    • Geoffrey D. Borman and others, "The National Randomized Field Trial of Success for All: Second-Year Outcomes," American Educational Research Journal 42, no. 4 (2005): 673-96.
    • (2005) American Educational Research Journal , vol.42 , Issue.4 , pp. 673-696
    • Borman, G.D.1    and others2
  • 59
    • 84858468032 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • State and federal governments spend about $9 billion a year on child care subsidies through the Child Care and Development Fund (CCDF, and about 25 percent of these subsidies go to children of ages three and four, about 70 percent of whom attend center-based care; see www.acf.hhs.gov/programs/ccb/ data/index.htm (February 8, 2007, Our take-up rates assume that every child whose family income is below three times the poverty line and who is currently in center-based child care will participate in our program. In this case, CCDBG expenditures would decline by 0.25 x 0.7 x $9 billion, $1.6 billion. Similarly, TANF programs currently spend about $2.5 billion in funding each year for child care subsidies, and under our assumed take-up rates for low-income children a portion of these expenditures would no longer be necessary; see i
    • State and federal governments spend about $9 billion a year on child care subsidies through the Child Care and Development Fund (CCDF), and about 25 percent of these subsidies go to children of ages three and four, about 70 percent of whom attend center-based care; see www.acf.hhs.gov/programs/ccb/ data/index.htm (February 8, 2007). Our take-up rates assume that every child whose family income is below three times the poverty line and who is currently in center-based child care will participate in our program. In this case, CCDBG expenditures would decline by (0.25 x 0.7 x $9 billion) = $1.6 billion. Similarly, TANF programs currently spend about $2.5 billion in funding each year for child care subsidies, and under our assumed take-up rates for low-income children a portion of these expenditures would no longer be necessary; see www.clasp.org/publications/childcareassistance2004.pdf. Current Head Start program expenditures are on the order of $7 billion a year; if our take-up rate assumptions are correct, then more than four-fifths of Head Start participants would switch over to the program we propose, either because their current Head Start providers would take over operation of the program or because these participants would move to another provider, who was providing our program. In either case, spending on Head Start in its current form would decline by around $5.8 billion. Similarly, states are now spending more than $2 billion on pre-K programs, and under our assumed take-up rates about three-quarters of these children would switch over to the new program we propose, saving $1.5 billion in government spending.
  • 60
    • 36549088009 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Specifically, we assume that Head Start's impact on children's cognitive achievement test scores will be on the order of 0.2 of a standard deviation, effects of pre-K programs will be around 0.3 of a standard deviation, and effects of center-based care will be about 0.1 of a standard deviation. The intent to treat effects in the recent national randomized experimental impact of Head Start are on the order of 0.1 to 0.25, where the effects are statistically significant (Puma and others, Head Start Impact Study; see note 24, the implied effect of treatment on the treated will be more on the order of 0.15 to 0.35 standard deviation, and so averaging across relevant outcome domains (including those where standard errors did not allow for detectable impacts) suggests that 0.2 is a reasonable assumption for this program
    • Specifically, we assume that Head Start's impact on children's cognitive achievement test scores will be on the order of 0.2 of a standard deviation, effects of pre-K programs will be around 0.3 of a standard deviation, and effects of center-based care will be about 0.1 of a standard deviation. The "intent to treat" effects in the recent national randomized experimental impact of Head Start are on the order of 0.1 to 0.25, where the effects are statistically significant (Puma and others, Head Start Impact Study; see note 24); the implied effect of treatment on the treated will be more on the order of 0.15 to 0.35 standard deviation, and so averaging across relevant outcome domains (including those where standard errors did not allow for detectable impacts) suggests that 0.2 is a reasonable assumption for this program.
  • 61
    • 84858456155 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Our assumption of 0.3 for state pre-K programs is the average across five states found by Barnett, Lamy, and Jung, The Effects of State Prekindergarten Program on Young Children's School Readiness in Five States (see note 28, for the PPVT vocabulary and Woodcock-Johnson early math tests. Because our program entails the same level of spending per child on early childhood intervention as Perry Preschool about $16,000 in current dollars for two years of half-day high-quality early education, if scale-up of our program does not reduce the intervention's effectiveness by more than half compared with the Perry model demonstration result, then our program's effect would be around 0.4 of a standard deviation. School-based research suggests that the benefits of two years of instruction are roughly twice those of a single year;
    • Our assumption of 0.3 for state pre-K programs is the average across five states found by Barnett, Lamy, and Jung, "The Effects of State Prekindergarten Program on Young Children's School Readiness in Five States" (see note 28), for the PPVT vocabulary and Woodcock-Johnson early math tests. Because our program entails the same level of spending per child on early childhood intervention as Perry Preschool (about $16,000 in current dollars for two years of half-day high-quality early education), if "scale-up" of our program does not reduce the intervention's effectiveness by more than half compared with the Perry model demonstration result, then our program's effect would be around 0.4 of a standard deviation. School-based research suggests that the benefits of two years of instruction are roughly twice those of a single year;
  • 62
    • 31344442775 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • D. Card, The Causal Effect of Education on Earnings, in Handbook of Labor Economics: 3A, edited by. O. Ashenfelter and D. E. Card (New York: Elsevier, 1999, pp. 1801-63. Because our intervention is about equal to twice the early childhood instruction provided by current high-quality, one-year, state pre-K programs, our proposed program could generate impacts of about 0.6 of a standard deviation twice the single-year pre-K impact, We also note that the average impact per participant of our program would be higher if more children than we have assumed stick with Head Start and pre-K. The reason is that both of these programs have larger impacts on children's cognitive skills compared with either regular center-based or other types of child care. If there is an increase in the fraction of children who switch into our program from center-based, parental, or informal care and a decline in the fraction who switch over from Head Start or pre-K, then th
    • D. Card, "The Causal Effect of Education on Earnings," in Handbook of Labor Economics: Volume 3A, edited by. O. Ashenfelter and D. E. Card (New York: Elsevier, 1999), pp. 1801-63. Because our intervention is about equal to twice the early childhood instruction provided by current high-quality, one-year, state pre-K programs, our proposed program could generate impacts of about 0.6 of a standard deviation (twice the single-year pre-K impact). We also note that the average impact per participant of our program would be higher if more children than we have assumed stick with Head Start and pre-K. The reason is that both of these programs have larger impacts on children's cognitive skills compared with either "regular" center-based or other types of child care. If there is an increase in the fraction of children who switch into our program from center-based, parental, or informal care and a decline in the fraction who switch over from Head Start or pre-K, then the average effect of participating in our program rather than the alternative care arrangements they would have experienced will increase as well.
  • 64
    • 33645383535 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The High/Scope Perry Preschool Program: Cost-Benefit Analysis Using Data from the Age 40 Follow-up
    • Clive R. Belfield and others, "The High/Scope Perry Preschool Program: Cost-Benefit Analysis Using Data from the Age 40 Follow-up," Journal of Human Resources 41, no.1 (2006): 162-90.
    • (2006) Journal of Human Resources , vol.41 , Issue.1 , pp. 162-190
    • Belfield, C.R.1    and others2
  • 65
    • 84858477376 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The standard cost estimate for two years of Perry Preschool is around $16,000, less than the $24,000 figure for our proposed program. The difference in costs is due in part to the wraparound child care that we propose. However, as discussed in the text, we think that offsets from reduced spending on other programs will reduce the costs of our proposal by up to one-third, which would make our per-child costs about the same as Perry Preschool.
    • The standard cost estimate for two years of Perry Preschool is around $16,000, less than the $24,000 figure for our proposed program. The difference in costs is due in part to the wraparound child care that we propose. However, as discussed in the text, we think that offsets from reduced spending on other programs will reduce the costs of our proposal by up to one-third, which would make our per-child costs about the same as Perry Preschool.
  • 66
    • 0001555787 scopus 로고
    • The Determinants of Children's Attainments: A Review of Methods and Findings
    • December
    • Robert Haveman and Barbara Wolfe, "The Determinants of Children's Attainments: A Review of Methods and Findings," Journal of Economic Literature 23 (December 1995): 1829-78;
    • (1995) Journal of Economic Literature , vol.23 , pp. 1829-1878
    • Haveman, R.1    Wolfe, B.2
  • 67
    • 0037294036 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Economic Considerations and Class Size
    • Alan B. Krueger, "Economic Considerations and Class Size," Economic Journal 113 (2003): 34-63.
    • (2003) Economic Journal , vol.113 , pp. 34-63
    • Krueger, A.B.1
  • 68
    • 33645763144 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Is Full Better than Half? Examining the Longitudinal Effects of Full-Day Kindergarten Attendance
    • Jill S. Cannon, Allison Jacknowitz, and Gary Painter, "Is Full Better than Half? Examining the Longitudinal Effects of Full-Day Kindergarten Attendance," Journal of Policy Analysis and Management 25, no 2. (2006): 299-321;
    • (2006) Journal of Policy Analysis and Management , vol.25 , Issue.2 , pp. 299-321
    • Cannon, J.S.1    Jacknowitz, A.2    Painter, G.3
  • 69
    • 38249013212 scopus 로고
    • Success Outcomes of Full-Day Kindergarten: More Positive Behavior and Increased Achievement in the Years After
    • John R. Cryan and others, "Success Outcomes of Full-Day Kindergarten: More Positive Behavior and Increased Achievement in the Years After," Early Childhood Research Quarterly 7, no. 2 (1992): 187-203;
    • (1992) Early Childhood Research Quarterly , vol.7 , Issue.2 , pp. 187-203
    • Cryan, J.R.1    and others2
  • 71
    • 0040747342 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • What Do They Do All Day? Comprehensive Evaluation of a Full-School-Day Kindergarten
    • James Elicker and Sangeeta Mathur, "What Do They Do All Day? Comprehensive Evaluation of a Full-School-Day Kindergarten," Early Childhood Research Quarterly 12 (1997): 459-80;
    • (1997) Early Childhood Research Quarterly , vol.12 , pp. 459-480
    • Elicker, J.1    Mathur, S.2
  • 72
    • 0038886083 scopus 로고
    • The Kindergarten Experience
    • Nancy Karweit, "The Kindergarten Experience," Educational Leadership 49 (1992): 82-86.
    • (1992) Educational Leadership , vol.49 , pp. 82-86
    • Karweit, N.1
  • 73
    • 84858474416 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The Science of Early Childhood Development: National Scientific Council on the Developing Child, January
    • "The Science of Early Childhood Development: Closing the Gap between What We Know and What We Do," National Scientific Council on the Developing Child, January 2007, www.developingchild.net/pubs/persp/pdf/ science_of_development.pdf.
    • (2007) Closing the Gap between What We Know and What We Do
  • 74
    • 36549034173 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Child Care Eligibility and Enrollment Estimates for Fiscal Year 2003, Policy Issue Brief (Department of Health and Human Services, Office of the Assistant Secretary for Planning and Evaluation, April 2005), http://aspe.hhs.gov/hsp/05/cc-elig-est03/index.htm (February 2007).
    • "Child Care Eligibility and Enrollment Estimates for Fiscal Year 2003," Policy Issue Brief (Department of Health and Human Services, Office of the Assistant Secretary for Planning and Evaluation, April 2005), http://aspe.hhs.gov/hsp/05/cc-elig-est03/index.htm (February 2007).
  • 75
    • 33748936138 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Culture' and the Intergenerational Transmission of Poverty: The Prevention Paradox
    • Jens Ludwig and Susan E. Mayer, " 'Culture' and the Intergenerational Transmission of Poverty: The Prevention Paradox," Future of Children 16, no. 2 (2006): 175-96..
    • (2006) Future of Children , vol.16 , Issue.2 , pp. 175-196
    • Ludwig, J.1    Mayer, S.E.2
  • 76
  • 77
    • 11944258221 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Does Money Really Matter? Estimating Impacts of Family Income on Children's Achievement with Data from Random-Assignment Experiments
    • paper presented at the, University of Chicago
    • Pamela Morris, Greg Duncan, and Christopher Rodrigues, "Does Money Really Matter? Estimating Impacts of Family Income on Children's Achievement with Data from Random-Assignment Experiments," paper presented at the Chicago Workshop on Black-White Inequality, University of Chicago, 2006.
    • (2006) Chicago Workshop on Black-White Inequality
    • Morris, P.1    Duncan, G.2    Rodrigues, C.3


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.