메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 8, Issue , 2001, Pages 337-357

Subordination and discourse segmentation revisited, or: Why matrix clauses may be more dependent than complements

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords


EID: 85190684566     PISSN: 13876724     EISSN: None     Source Type: Book Series    
DOI: 10.1075/hcp.8.18ver     Document Type: Chapter
Times cited : (29)

References (55)
  • 1
    • 85190647349 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Under certain analyses, the second segment of (1) might be said to properly contain an infinite clause (the complement of want), but I will not consider that in this paper, though I believe that the present approach can ultimately be helpful in clarifying that as well
    • Under certain analyses, the second segment of (1) might be said to properly contain an infinite clause (the complement of want), but I will not consider that issue in this paper, though I believe that the present approach can ultimately be helpful in clarifying that as well.
  • 2
    • 85190708335 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Verhagen (1995) for some suggestions
    • See Verhagen (1995) for some suggestions.
  • 3
    • 85190659012 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • This is not meant to imply that the three arguments are necessarily to be taken as equal. Recall that the here is just segmentation, not the assignment of (hierarchical) structure. Thus the grammatical structure of
    • This is not meant to imply that the three arguments are necessarily to be taken as equal. Recall that the issue here is just segmentation, not the assignment of (hierarchical) structure. Thus the grammatical structure of
  • 4
    • 85190669476 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • could very well be taken as an indication that the last two arguments are to be taken as constituting a set to be added to the single argument in sentence
    • could very well be taken as an indication that the last two arguments are to be taken as constituting a set to be added to the single argument in sentence
  • 5
    • 85190690766 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • cf. below). The point here is simply that the question of segmentation precedes the assignment of structure
    • (cf. below). The point here is simply that the question of segmentation precedes the assignment of structure.
  • 6
    • 85190688358 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Again: only segmentation is the here, not the assignment of structural relationships; cf. note 2
    • Again: only segmentation is the issue here, not the assignment of structural relationships; cf. note 2.
  • 7
    • 85190667990 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A clear exposition of the view of linguistic communication as influencing another person's cognition by displaying the intention to do so can be found in Keller 1995, 153ff., 1998, 136ff
    • A clear exposition of the view of linguistic communication as influencing another person's cognition by displaying the intention to do so can be found in Keller (1995, p. 153ff., 1998, p. 136ff.).
  • 8
    • 85190659075 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • It is crucially related to Grice's (1957) notion 'meaningNN' and also occurs, in slightly variable forms, in several other approaches to pragmatics
    • NN' and also occurs, in slightly variable forms, in several other approaches to pragmatics.
  • 9
    • 85190687939 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • This distinction is related, but not identical, to distinctions between different domains of use, for example the distinction between epistemic and content domains as proposed in Sweetser 1990
    • This distinction is related, but not identical, to distinctions between different domains of use, for example the distinction between epistemic and content domains as proposed in Sweetser (1990)
  • 10
    • 85190651797 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • or that between pragmatic and semantic sources of coherence as proposed in Sanders 1992
    • or that between pragmatic and semantic sources of coherence as proposed in Sanders (1992).
  • 11
    • 85190679639 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Cf. Foolen (1996) and Verhagen (1996b, 274/5) for some discussion
    • Cf. Foolen (1996) and Verhagen (1996b, p. 274/5) for some discussion.
  • 12
    • 85190698393 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • This difference consists in the distinction between what I called “speaker-hearer-subjectivity” and “character-subjectivity” cf. below; also J. Sanders 1994
    • This difference consists in the distinction between what I called “speaker-hearer-subjectivity” and “character-subjectivity” (cf. below; also J. Sanders 1994).
  • 13
    • 85190698907 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • certain areas, such as that of language change, this difference is very important cf. Verhagen 2000, for an example
    • In certain areas, such as that of language change, this difference is very important (cf. Verhagen 2000, for an example).
  • 14
    • 85190651123 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • this representation the coordinating conjunction is taken to be an element in the coordination dimension, but this is not crucial. moment I have principled considerations to offer on this point, but I find this representation useful for expository purposes
    • In this representation the coordinating conjunction is taken to be an element in the coordination dimension, but this is not crucial. At the moment I have no principled considerations to offer on this point, but I find this representation useful for expository purposes.
  • 15
    • 85190712703 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • a different terminology, a similar insight has been formulated for that-clauses, the most prototypical subclass of complements, by Wierzbicka (1988: 132-140): “… reference to knowledge is present in all sentences with THAT” (137; Wierzbicka in turn cites a few other linguists who have proposed partly similar analyses, notably Bolinger
    • In a different terminology, a similar insight has been formulated for that-clauses, the most prototypical subclass of complements, by Wierzbicka (1988: 132-140): “… reference to knowledge is present in all sentences with THAT” (p.137; Wierzbicka in turn cites a few other linguists who have proposed partly similar analyses, notably Bolinger).
  • 16
    • 85190707634 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • I believe that the mental space cum construction approach provides a generalization over these and other types of complements (linking the 'space building' feature to the construction and leaving other aspects of the semantics to the lexical specifications of the verbs and complementizers involved), as well as one that allows for integration into a more general theory of perspectivization
    • I believe that the mental space cum construction approach provides a generalization over these and other types of complements (linking the 'space building' feature to the construction and leaving other aspects of the semantics to the lexical specifications of the verbs and complementizers involved), as well as one that allows for integration into a more general theory of perspectivization.
  • 17
    • 85190669241 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The direction of the generalization I would like to propose is that causation is also attribution of the situation denoted by the complement clause to something else, but then to an objective factor (i.e. the cause) rather than to a subjective one
    • The direction of the generalization I would like to propose is that causation is also attribution of the situation denoted by the complement clause to something else, but then to an objective factor (i.e. the cause) rather than to a subjective one.
  • 18
    • 85190652167 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • that perspective, the complementation construction would constitute an example of a particular kind of constructional polysemy
    • In that perspective, the complementation construction would constitute an example of a particular kind of constructional polysemy.
  • 19
    • 85190713533 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Verhagen (1996b) for some discussion of this idea, and Foolen (1996) for some criticism
    • See Verhagen (1996b) for some discussion of this idea, and Foolen (1996) for some criticism.
  • 20
    • 85190646491 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Dutch, the matrix clauses do not have to contain the pro-form it, for neither category of predicate nominal. Thus Dutch does not only have matrix clauses of the type Een probleem is dat… “A problem is that…
    • In Dutch, the matrix clauses do not have to contain the pro-form it, for neither category of predicate nominal. Thus Dutch does not only have matrix clauses of the type Een probleem is dat… (“A problem is that…”)
  • 21
    • 85190648223 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • but also Duidelijk is dat… lit. “Clear is that…
    • but also Duidelijk is dat… (lit. “Clear is that…”).
  • 22
    • 85190674795 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The parallel between these two types is one reason why in some grammatical traditions, the initial noun phrase in a clause of the type Een/het probleem is dat.. is analyzed as a preposed predicate nominal rather than a subject
    • The parallel between these two types is one reason why in some grammatical traditions, the initial noun phrase in a clause of the type Een/het probleem is dat.. is analyzed as a preposed predicate nominal rather than a subject.
  • 23
    • 85190675474 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • As with complement clauses, the matrix of a subject clause may also have a causal relationship with the subordinate clause (Cf. The result/reason is…). As mentioned in note 9, I think a further generalization is possible, so that we actually have constructional polysemy here, but I will not pursue that in this paper
    • As with complement clauses, the matrix of a subject clause may also have a causal relationship with the subordinate clause (Cf. The result/reason is…). As mentioned in note 9, I think a further generalization is possible, so that we actually have constructional polysemy here, but I will not pursue that issue in this paper.
  • 24
    • 85190654897 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • There should also be independent grammar-internal arguments for positing a construction such as (11) as part of the grammar of a language. I think such arguments can indeed be provided, at least for Dutch (cf. Verhagen 1996b and Foolen 1996, for somewhat different views
    • There should also be independent grammar-internal arguments for positing a construction such as (11) as part of the grammar of a language. I think such arguments can indeed be provided, at least for Dutch (cf. Verhagen 1996b and Foolen 1996, for somewhat different views).
  • 25
    • 85190682792 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Furthermore, this analysis has consequences for the grammatical characterization of subordination as such. Again, these issues are only indirectly related to the matter of discourse segmentation, so I will not go into them here
    • Furthermore, this analysis has consequences for the grammatical characterization of subordination as such. Again, these issues are only indirectly related to the matter of discourse segmentation, so I will not go into them here.
  • 26
    • 85190660698 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The Eindhoven Corpus, in the version the Free University in Amsterdam; it is described in Uit den Boogaart (1975) and Renkema (1981
    • The Eindhoven Corpus, in the version available from the Free University in Amsterdam; it is described in Uit den Boogaart (1975) and Renkema (1981).
  • 31
    • 85187927745 scopus 로고
    • Mental Spaces
    • Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. First edition, 1985, Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press
    • Fauconnier, G. (1994). Mental Spaces. Aspects of Meaning Construction in Natural Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [First edition, 1985, Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.]
    • (1994) Aspects of Meaning Construction in Natural Language
    • Fauconnier, G.1
  • 32
    • 85064509574 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Tekstsegmentatie, onderschikking en subjectiviteit. Commentaar op Arie Verhagen
    • Foolen, A. (1996). Tekstsegmentatie, onderschikking en subjectiviteit. Commentaar op Arie Verhagen. Gramma/TTT, 5, 269-272.
    • (1996) Gramma/TTT , vol.5 , pp. 269-272
    • Foolen, A.1
  • 35
    • 0039481188 scopus 로고
    • Tübingen/Basel: Francke Verlag
    • Keller, R. (1995). Zeichentheorie.Tübingen/Basel: Francke Verlag.
    • (1995) Zeichentheorie
    • Keller, R.1
  • 38
    • 84961480199 scopus 로고
    • Rhetorical Structure Theory: Toward a functional theory of text organization
    • Mann, W. C., & Thompson, S. A. (1988). Rhetorical Structure Theory: Toward a functional theory of text organization.Text, 8, 243-281.
    • (1988) Text , vol.8 , pp. 243-281
    • Mann, W.C.1    Thompson, S.A.2
  • 39
    • 0003288854 scopus 로고
    • The structure of discourse and 'subordination
    • J. Haiman & S. A. Thompson Eds, Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 275-329
    • Matthiessen, C., & Thompson, S. A. (1988). The structure of discourse and 'subordination'. In J. Haiman & S. A. Thompson (Eds.), Clause Combining in Grammar and Discourse (pp. 275-329). Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 275-329.
    • (1988) Clause Combining in Grammar and Discourse , pp. 275-329
    • Matthiessen, C.1    Thompson, S.A.2
  • 41
    • 85190685851 scopus 로고
    • Groningen: Martinus Nijhoff
    • Pander Maat, H. (1994). Tekstanalyse. Groningen: Martinus Nijhoff.
    • (1994) Tekstanalyse
    • Pander Maat, H.1
  • 45
    • 0010793161 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • PISA - A procedure for analyzing the structure of explanatory texts
    • Sanders, T. J. M., & van Wijk, C. (1996). PISA - A procedure for analyzing the structure of explanatory texts. Text, 16, 91-132.
    • (1996) Text , vol.16 , pp. 91-132
    • Sanders, T.J.M.1    van Wijk, C.2
  • 48
    • 29244463728 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Conceptual dependency and the clausal structure of discourse
    • Jean-Pierre Koenig Ed, Stanford: CSLI Publications
    • Schilperoord, J., & Verhagen, A. (1998). Conceptual dependency and the clausal structure of discourse. In Jean-Pierre Koenig (Ed.), Discourse and Cognition. Bridging the Gap (pp. 141-163). Stanford: CSLI Publications.
    • (1998) Discourse and Cognition. Bridging the Gap , pp. 141-163
    • Schilperoord, J.1    Verhagen, A.2
  • 51
    • 29044444894 scopus 로고
    • Subjectification, syntax, and communication
    • D. Stein & S. Wright Eds, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
    • Verhagen, A. (1995). Subjectification, syntax, and communication. In D. Stein & S. Wright (Eds.), Subjectivity and subjectivisation: linguistic perspectives (pp. 103-128). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    • (1995) Subjectivity and subjectivisation: linguistic perspectives , pp. 103-128
    • Verhagen, A.1
  • 53
    • 25444516345 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Tekstsegmentatie, onderschikking en subjectiviteit
    • 273-275
    • Verhagen, A. (1996b). Tekstsegmentatie, onderschikking en subjectiviteit. Gramma/TTT, 5, 249-268, 273-275.
    • (1996) Gramma/TTT , vol.5 , pp. 249-268
    • Verhagen, A.1
  • 54
    • 61149181978 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The girl that promised to become something”: An exploration into diachronic subjectification in Dutch
    • T. F. Shannon & J. Snapper Eds, Lanham, MD: University Press of America
    • Verhagen, A. (2000). 'The girl that promised to become something”: An exploration into diachronic subjectification in Dutch. In T. F. Shannon & J.P. Snapper (Eds.), The Berkeley Conference on dutch Linguistics 1997: the Dutch Language at the Millennium (pp. 197-208). Lanham, MD: University Press of America.
    • (2000) The Berkeley Conference on dutch Linguistics 1997: the Dutch Language at the Millennium , pp. 197-208
    • Verhagen, A.1


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.