-
1
-
-
85190388939
-
-
The en- has an orthographic variant e- which, in standard Dutch, does not differ in pronunciation
-
The -en- has an orthographic variant -e- which, in standard Dutch, does not differ in pronunciation.
-
-
-
-
2
-
-
85190365565
-
-
We did not test any semantic rules because celex does not provide the required semantic information
-
We did not test any semantic rules because celex does not provide the required semantic information.
-
-
-
-
3
-
-
85190394720
-
-
For a list of all applicable rules Appendix D
-
For a list of all applicable rules see Appendix D.
-
-
-
-
4
-
-
85190364270
-
-
All results of TiMBL (version 3.0) in this paper are obtained by using the standard IB1 algorithm, the overlap similarity metric with information gain weighting, and one nearest neighbor for extrapolation. In our simulation studies, this set of parameters has been proven to lead to the best results. For AM we excluded ‘=’ as a variable, set the option ‘given’ to ‘exclude’, the option ‘probability’ to unity, and used the option ‘squared’ without specifying any frequency range
-
All results of TiMBL (version 3.0) in this paper are obtained by using the standard IB1 algorithm, the overlap similarity metric with information gain weighting, and one nearest neighbor for extrapolation. In our simulation studies, this set of parameters has been proven to lead to the best results. For AM we excluded ‘=’ as a variable, set the option ‘given’ to ‘exclude’, the option ‘probability’ to unity, and used the option ‘squared’ without specifying any frequency range.
-
-
-
-
5
-
-
85190375519
-
-
Vance (1980) reports similar findings in his study of Lyman’s law which predicts the occurrence of rendaku in Japanese compounds. He concludes that rendaku is psychologically real only for a rather small minority of speakers
-
Vance (1980) reports similar findings in his study of Lyman’s law which predicts the occurrence of rendaku in Japanese compounds. He concludes that rendaku is psychologically real only for a rather small minority of speakers.
-
-
-
-
6
-
-
85190363822
-
-
Using different parameter settings does not enhance performance. Training on constituent, suffix, and rime while using the IGTREE algorithm leads to 30.1% correctly predicted compounds. With TRIBL we reach 37.0%. If we enhance the number of nearest neighbors for extrapolation to three, both IG and TRIBL reach a prediction accuracy of 30.1%
-
Using different parameter settings does not enhance performance. Training on constituent, suffix, and rime while using the IGTREE algorithm leads to 30.1% correctly predicted compounds. With TRIBL we reach 37.0%. If we enhance the number of nearest neighbors for extrapolation to three, both IG and TRIBL reach a prediction accuracy of 30.1%.
-
-
-
-
7
-
-
84936526602
-
Producing morphologically complex words
-
Anshen, Frank, & Mark Aronoff (1988). Producing morphologically complex words. Linguistics, 26, 641–655.
-
(1988)
Linguistics
, vol.26
, pp. 641-655
-
-
Anshen, F.1
Aronoff, M.2
-
9
-
-
0003443729
-
-
University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA: Linguistic Data Consortium
-
Baayen, R. Harald, Richard Piepenbrock, & Léon Gulikers (1995). The CELEX lexical database (CD-ROM). University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA: Linguistic Data Consortium.
-
(1995)
The CELEX lexical database (CD-ROM)
-
-
Baayen, R.H.1
Piepenbrock, R.2
Gulikers, L.3
-
10
-
-
0034154998
-
The balance of storage and computation in morphological processing: The role of word formation type, affixal homonymy, and productivity
-
Bertram, Raymond, Robert Schreuder, & R. Harald Baayen (2000). The balance of storage and computation in morphological processing: The role of word formation type, affixal homonymy, and productivity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Memory, Learning, and Cognition, 26, 419–511.
-
(2000)
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Memory, Learning, and Cognition
, vol.26
, pp. 419-511
-
-
Bertram, R.1
Schreuder, R.2
Harald Baayen, R.3
-
11
-
-
0033394150
-
Lexical entries and rules of language: a multi-disciplinary study of German inflection
-
Clahsen, Harald (1999). Lexical entries and rules of language: a multi-disciplinary study of German inflection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 991–1060.
-
(1999)
Behavioral and Brain Sciences
, vol.22
, pp. 991-1060
-
-
Clahsen, H.1
-
12
-
-
0009787473
-
-
Technical Report ILK 00-01, Computational Linguistics, Tilburg University
-
Daelemans, Walter, Jakub Zavrel, Ko van der Sloot, & Antal van den Bosch (2000). TiMBL: Tilburg memory based learner reference guide. Version 3.0. Technical Report ILK 00-01, Computational Linguistics, Tilburg University.
-
(2000)
TiMBL: Tilburg memory based learner reference guide. Version 3.0
-
-
Daelemans, W.1
Zavrel, J.2
van der Sloot, K.3
van den Bosch, A.4
-
13
-
-
0033821079
-
The morphological family size effect and morphology
-
De Jong, Nivja H., Robert Schreuder, & R. Harald Baayen (2000). The morphological family size effect and morphology. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15, 329–365.
-
(2000)
Language and Cognitive Processes
, vol.15
, pp. 329-365
-
-
De Jong, N.H.1
Schreuder, R.2
Harald Baayen, R.3
-
15
-
-
0009764719
-
-
Groningen: Martinus Nijhoff
-
Haeseryn, Walter, K. Romijn, Guido Geerts, Jaap de Rooij, & Maarten C. van den Toorn (1997). Algemene Nederlandse Spraakkunst. Groningen: Martinus Nijhoff.
-
(1997)
Algemene Nederlandse Spraakkunst
-
-
Haeseryn, W.1
Romijn, K.2
Geerts, G.3
de Rooij, J.4
van den Toorn, M.C.5
-
16
-
-
0009766463
-
Analogy in morphology: modeling the choice of linking morphemes in Dutch
-
Krott, Andrea, R. Harald Baayen, & Robert Schreuder (2001). Analogy in morphology: modeling the choice of linking morphemes in Dutch. Linguistics, 39.
-
(2001)
Linguistics
, vol.39
-
-
Krott, A.1
Harald Baayen, R.2
Schreuder, R.3
-
18
-
-
84927455354
-
De voorspelbaarheid van tussenklanken in nominale samenstellingen
-
Mattens, W. H. M. (1984). De voorspelbaarheid van tussenklanken in nominale samenstellingen. De nieuwe taalgids, 7, 333–343.
-
(1984)
De nieuwe taalgids
, vol.7
, pp. 333-343
-
-
Mattens, W.H.M.1
-
19
-
-
0025743892
-
Rules of language
-
Pinker, Steven (1991). Rules of language. Science, 153, 530–535.
-
(1991)
Science
, vol.153
, pp. 530-535
-
-
Pinker, S.1
-
20
-
-
0030927448
-
Words and rules in the human brain
-
Pinker, Steven (1997). Words and rules in the human brain. Nature, 387, 547–548.
-
(1997)
Nature
, vol.387
, pp. 547-548
-
-
Pinker, S.1
-
21
-
-
0033212074
-
A connectionist model of English past tense and plural morphology
-
Plunkett, Kim, & Patrick Juola (1999). A connectionist model of English past tense and plural morphology. Cognitive Science, 23, 463–490.
-
(1999)
Cognitive Science
, vol.23
, pp. 463-490
-
-
Plunkett, K.1
Juola, P.2
-
23
-
-
0031116032
-
Morphological priming, fragment completion, and connectionist networks
-
Rueckl, Jay G, Michelle Mikolinski, Michal Raveh, Caroline S. Miner, & Frans Mars (1997). Morphological priming, fragment completion, and connectionist networks. Journal of Memory and Language, 36, 382–405.
-
(1997)
Journal of Memory and Language
, vol.36
, pp. 382-405
-
-
Rueckl, J.G.1
Mikolinski, M.2
Raveh, M.3
Miner, C.S.4
Mars, F.5
-
25
-
-
85066736642
-
Sublexical structures in visual word recognition: Access units or orthographic redundancy
-
M. Coltheart Ed, Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
-
Seidenberg, Mark (1987). Sublexical structures in visual word recognition: Access units or orthographic redundancy. In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and Performance XII (pp. 245–263). Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
-
(1987)
Attention and Performance XII
, pp. 245-263
-
-
Seidenberg, M.1
-
26
-
-
0001320879
-
Evaluating behavioral and neuroimaging data on past tense processing
-
Seidenberg, Mark S., & James H. Hoeffner (1998). Evaluating behavioral and neuroimaging data on past tense processing. Language, 74, 104–122.
-
(1998)
Language
, vol.74
, pp. 104-122
-
-
Seidenberg, M.S.1
Hoeffner, J.H.2
-
29
-
-
84925971915
-
De tussenklank in samenstellingen waarvan het eerste lid een afleiding is
-
van der Toorn, Maarten C. (1981a). De tussenklank in samenstellingen waarvan het eerste lid een afleiding is. De nieuwe taalgids, 74, 197–205.
-
(1981)
De nieuwe taalgids
, vol.74
, pp. 197-205
-
-
van der Toorn, M.C.1
-
30
-
-
84925971915
-
De tussenklank in samenstellingen waarvan het eerste lid systematisch uitheems is
-
van der Toorn, Maarten C. (1981b). De tussenklank in samenstellingen waarvan het eerste lid systematisch uitheems is. De nieuwe taalgids, 74, 547–552.
-
(1981)
De nieuwe taalgids
, vol.74
, pp. 547-552
-
-
van der Toorn, M.C.1
-
31
-
-
84925978941
-
Tendenzen bij de beregeling van de verbindingsklank in nominale samenstellingen I
-
van der Toorn, Maarten C. (1982a). Tendenzen bij de beregeling van de verbindingsklank in nominale samenstellingen I. De nieuwe taalgids, 75, 24–33.
-
(1982)
De nieuwe taalgids
, vol.75
, pp. 24-33
-
-
van der Toorn, M.C.1
-
32
-
-
84925978946
-
Tendenzen bij de beregeling van de verbindingsklank in nominale samenstellingen II
-
van der Toorn, Maarten C. (1982b). Tendenzen bij de beregeling van de verbindingsklank in nominale samenstellingen II. De nieuwe taalgids, 75, 153–160.
-
(1982)
De nieuwe taalgids
, vol.75
, pp. 153-160
-
-
van der Toorn, M.C.1
-
33
-
-
84925926097
-
The psychological status of a constraint on Japanese consonant alternations
-
Vance, Timothy J. (1980). The psychological status of a constraint on Japanese consonant alternations. Linguistics, 18, 145–167.
-
(1980)
Linguistics
, vol.18
, pp. 145-167
-
-
Vance, T.J.1
|