메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 2, Issue 2, 2007, Pages 271-284

Lingua: The language-independent neighbourhood generator of the university of alberta

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords


EID: 84989402485     PISSN: 18711340     EISSN: 18711375     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1075/ml.2.2.09wes     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (15)

References (30)
  • 1
    • 0001442210 scopus 로고
    • Frequency and neighborhood effects on lexical access: Lexical similarity ororthographic redundancy?
    • Andrews, S. (1992). Frequency and neighborhood effects on lexical access: Lexical similarity ororthographic redundancy? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and-Cognition, 18, 234-254.
    • (1992) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, And-Cognition , vol.18 , pp. 234-254
    • Andrews, S.1
  • 2
    • 0000200881 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The effect of orthographic similarity on lexical retrieval: Resolvingneigh-borhood conflicts
    • Andrews, S. (1997). The effect of orthographic similarity on lexical retrieval: Resolvingneigh-borhood conflicts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 439-461.
    • (1997) Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , vol.4 , pp. 439-461
    • Andrews, S.1
  • 3
    • 0021437034 scopus 로고
    • Are lexical decisions a good measure of lexical access?The role of word frequency in the neglected decision stage
    • Balota, D. A., & Chumbley, J. I. (1984). Are lexical decisions a good measure of lexical access?The role of word frequency in the neglected decision stage. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 10, 340-357.
    • (1984) Journal of Experimental Psychology , vol.10 , pp. 340-357
    • Balota, D.A.1    Chumbley, J.I.2
  • 4
    • 84977940268 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • BootCaT: Bootstrapping corpora and terms from the web
    • Baroni, M., & Bernardini, S. (2004). BootCaT: Bootstrapping corpora and terms from the web. Proceedings of LREC 2004.
    • (2004) Proceedings of LREC , pp. 2004
    • Baroni, M.1    Bernardini, S.2
  • 5
    • 0009806991 scopus 로고
    • The recognition of tachistoscopically presented five-letter words as afunction of digram frequency
    • Biederman, G. B. (1966). The recognition of tachistoscopically presented five-letter words as afunction of digram frequency. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 208-209.
    • (1966) Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior , vol.5 , pp. 208-209
    • Biederman, G.B.1
  • 6
    • 33749263849 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Tuning of thehuman left fusiform gyrus to sublexical orthographic structure
    • Binder, J., Medler, D., Westbury, C., Liebenthal, E., & Buchanan, L. (2006). Tuning of thehuman left fusiform gyrus to sublexical orthographic structure. Neuroimage, 33(2), 739-748
    • (2006) Neuroimage , vol.33 , Issue.2 , pp. 739-748
    • Binder, J.1    Medler, D.2    Westbury, C.3    Liebenthal, E.4    Buchanan, L.5
  • 9
    • 0001163077 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Perception of Wordlikeness: Effects of Segment-Probability and Length on the Processing of Nonwords
    • Frisch, S. A, Large, N. R., & Pisoni, D. B. (2000) Perception of Wordlikeness: Effects of Segment-Probability and Length on the Processing of Nonwords, Journal of Memory and Language, 42(4), 481-496.
    • (2000) Journal of Memory and Language , vol.42 , Issue.4 , pp. 481-496
    • Frisch, S.A.1    Large, N.R.2    Pisoni, D.B.3
  • 11
    • 0021436743 scopus 로고
    • Resolving 20 years of inconsistent interactions between lexicalfa-miliarity and orthography, concreteness, and polysemy
    • Gernsbacher, M. A. (1984). Resolving 20 years of inconsistent interactions between lexicalfa-miliarity and orthography, concreteness, and polysemy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113(2), 256-281.
    • (1984) Journal of Experimental Psychology: General , vol.113 , Issue.2 , pp. 256-281
    • Gernsbacher, M.A.1
  • 13
    • 33749033072 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • NUANCE 3.0: Using Genetic Programming to-Model Variable Relationships
    • Hollis, G., Westbury, C., & Peterson, J. (2006). NUANCE 3.0: Using Genetic Programming to-Model Variable Relationships. Behavior Research Methods, 38(2), 218-228.
    • (2006) Behavior Research Methods , vol.38 , Issue.2 , pp. 218-228
    • Hollis, G.1    Westbury, C.2    Peterson, J.3
  • 14
    • 0031144865 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Consistency effects in high-frequency words
    • Jared, D. (1997). Consistency effects in high-frequency words. Journal of Memory and Language, 36, 505-529.
    • (1997) Journal of Memory and Language , vol.36 , pp. 505-529
    • Jared, D.1
  • 17
    • 0017672356 scopus 로고
    • The role of familiar units in the perception of word-sand nonwords
    • McClelland, J. L., & Johnson, J. C. (1977). The role of familiar units in the perception of word-sand nonwords. Perception & Psychophysics, 22, 249-261.
    • (1977) Perception & Psychophysics , vol.22 , pp. 249-261
    • Mc Clelland, J.L.1    Johnson, J.C.2
  • 19
    • 0009762523 scopus 로고
    • The role of bigram frequency in perception of words and nonwords
    • Rice, G. A., & Robinson, D. O. (1975). The role of bigram frequency in perception of words and nonwords. Memory & Cognition, 3, 513-518.
    • (1975) Memory & Cognition , vol.3 , pp. 513-518
    • Rice, G.A.1    Robinson, D.O.2
  • 22
    • 0016041177 scopus 로고
    • Processes of recognizing tachistoscopically presentedwords
    • Rumelhart, D. E., & Siple, P. (1974). Processes of recognizing tachistoscopically presentedwords. Psychological Review, 81, 99-118.
    • (1974) Psychological Review , vol.81 , pp. 99-118
    • Rumelhart, D.E.1    Siple, P.2
  • 24
    • 84940644968 scopus 로고
    • A mathematical theory of communication
    • Shannon, C. E. (1948). A mathematical theory of communication. The Bell System Technical Journal, 27, 379-423.
    • (1948) The Bell System Technical Journal , vol.27 , pp. 379-423
    • Shannon, C.E.1
  • 25
    • 33749033340 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Word frequency effects in high dimensional co-occurrenc-emodels: A new approach
    • Shaoul, C., & Westbury, C. (2006). Word frequency effects in high dimensional co-occurrenc-emodels: A new approach. Behavior Research Methods, 38(2), 190-195.
    • (2006) Behavior Research Methods , vol.38 , Issue.2 , pp. 190-195
    • Shaoul, C.1    Westbury, C.2
  • 26
    • 0025145292 scopus 로고
    • Not all nonwords are alike: Implications forread-ing development and theory
    • Treiman R., Goswami U., & Bruck M. (1990). Not all nonwords are alike: Implications forread-ing development and theory. Memory & Cognition. 18(6), 559-567.
    • (1990) Memory & Cognition , vol.18 , Issue.6 , pp. 559-567
    • Treiman, R.1    Goswami, U.2    Bruck, M.3
  • 28
    • 0036315234 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The probability of the least likely non length-controlled bigram affects lexical decision RTs
    • Westbury, C., & Buchanan, L. (2002). The probability of the least likely non length-controlled bigram affects lexical decision RTs. Brain and Language, 81(1/2/3), 66-78.
    • (2002) Brain and Language , vol.81 , Issue.3 , pp. 66-78
    • Westbury, C.1    Buchanan, L.2


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.