메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 9, Issue MAY, 2015, Pages

Attention to sound improves auditory reliability in audio-tactile spatial optimal integration

Author keywords

Attention; Auditory; Bayes theorem; Multisensory integration; Sensory cue

Indexed keywords

ACCURACY; ADULT; ARTICLE; ASSOCIATION; ATTENTION; AUDIO TACTILE SIGNAL; AUDIO TACTILE SPATIAL TASK; AUDITORY STIMULATION; HEARING; HUMAN; HUMAN EXPERIMENT; MAXIMUM LIKELIHOOD METHOD; NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL TEST; SENSORIMOTOR INTEGRATION; SPATIOTEMPORAL ANALYSIS; TACTILE STIMULATION;

EID: 84930205566     PISSN: 16625145     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.3389/fnint.2015.00034     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (28)

References (25)
  • 1
    • 1242288384 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The ventriloquist effect results from near-optimal bimodal integration
    • Alais, D., and Burr, D. (2004). The ventriloquist effect results from near-optimal bimodal integration. Curr. Biol. 14, 257-262. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.01.029
    • (2004) Curr. Biol , vol.14 , pp. 257-262
    • Alais, D.1    Burr, D.2
  • 2
    • 18844457069 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Audiovisual integration of speech falters under high attention demands
    • Alsius, A., Navarra, J., Campbell, R., and Soto-Faraco, S. (2005). Audiovisual integration of speech falters under high attention demands. Curr. Biol. 15, 839-843. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.03.046
    • (2005) Curr. Biol , vol.15 , pp. 839-843
    • Alsius, A.1    Navarra, J.2    Campbell, R.3    Soto-Faraco, S.4
  • 3
    • 0019573904 scopus 로고
    • Cross-modal bias and perceptual fusion with auditory-visual spatial discordance
    • Bertelson, P., and Radeau, M. (1981). Cross-modal bias and perceptual fusion with auditory-visual spatial discordance.Percept. Psychophys. 29, 578-584. doi: 10.3758/BF03207374
    • (1981) Percept. Psychophys , vol.29 , pp. 578-584
    • Bertelson, P.1    Radeau, M.2
  • 4
    • 0034134173 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The ventriloquist effect does not depend on the direction of deliberate visual attention
    • Bertelson, P., Vroomen, J., de Gelder, B., and Driver, J. (2000). The ventriloquist effect does not depend on the direction of deliberate visual attention. Percept. Psychophys. 62, 321-332. doi: 10.3758/BF03205552
    • (2000) Percept. Psychophys , vol.62 , pp. 321-332
    • Bertelson, P.1    Vroomen, J.2    de Gelder, B.3    Driver, J.4
  • 5
    • 33644771081 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Time perception: Space-time in the brain
    • Burr, D., and Morrone, C. (2006). Time perception: space-time in the brain. Curr. Biol. 16, R171-R173. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.02.038
    • (2006) Curr. Biol , vol.16 , pp. R171-R173
    • Burr, D.1    Morrone, C.2
  • 6
    • 84868087239 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Multisensory representation of frequency across audition and touch: High density electrical mapping reveals early sensory-perceptual coupling
    • Butler, J. S., Foxe, J. J., Fiebelkorn, I. C., Mercier, M. R., and Molholm, S. (2012). Multisensory representation of frequency across audition and touch: high density electrical mapping reveals early sensory-perceptual coupling. J. Neurosci. 32, 15338-15344. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1796-12.2012
    • (2012) J. Neurosci , vol.32 , pp. 15338-15344
    • Butler, J.S.1    Foxe, J.J.2    Fiebelkorn, I.C.3    Mercier, M.R.4    Molholm, S.5
  • 8
    • 0029935458 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Enhancement of selective listening by illusory mislocation of speech sounds due to lip-reading
    • Driver, J. (1996). Enhancement of selective listening by illusory mislocation of speech sounds due to lip-reading. Nature 381, 66-68. doi: 10.1038/381066a0
    • (1996) Nature , vol.381 , pp. 66-68
    • Driver, J.1
  • 10
    • 0037165165 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion
    • Ernst, M. O., and Banks, M. S. (2002). Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature415, 429-433. doi: 10.1038/415429a
    • (2002) Nature , vol.415 , pp. 429-433
    • Ernst, M.O.1    Banks, M.S.2
  • 11
    • 77956748592 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Computational models of sensorimotor integration
    • eds P. G. Morasso and V. Sanguineti (Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers)
    • Ghahramani, Z., Wolpert, D. M., and Jordan, M. I. (1997). "Computational models of sensorimotor integration," in Self-Organization, Computational Maps and Motor Control, eds P. G. Morasso and V. Sanguineti (Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers), 117-147.
    • (1997) Self-Organization, Computational Maps and Motor Control , pp. 117-147
    • Ghahramani, Z.1    Wolpert, D.M.2    Jordan, M.I.3
  • 12
    • 84867053122 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Development of visuo-auditory integration in space and time
    • Gori, M., Sandini, G., and Burr, D. (2012). Development of visuo-auditory integration in space and time. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6:77. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00077
    • (2012) Front. Integr. Neurosci , vol.6 , pp. 77
    • Gori, M.1    Sandini, G.2    Burr, D.3
  • 13
    • 38849099827 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Visual-haptic cue weighting is independent of modality-specific attention
    • Helbig, H. B., and Ernst, M. O. (2008). Visual-haptic cue weighting is independent of modality-specific attention. J. Vis. 8, 21-16. doi: 10.1167/8.1.21
    • (2008) J. Vis , vol.8 , pp. 21-16
    • Helbig, H.B.1    Ernst, M.O.2
  • 14
    • 33644750219 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Spatially localized distortions of event time
    • Johnston, A., Arnold, D. H., and Nishida, S. (2006). Spatially localized distortions of event time. Curr. Biol. 16, 472-479. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.01.032
    • (2006) Curr. Biol , vol.16 , pp. 472-479
    • Johnston, A.1    Arnold, D.H.2    Nishida, S.3
  • 15
    • 80855126448 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Ideal-observer models of cue integration
    • eds J. Trommershauser, K. Körding, and M. S. Landy (New York, NY: Oxford University Press)
    • Landy, M. S., Banks, M. S., and Knill, D. C. (2011). "Ideal-observer models of cue integration," in Book of Sensory Cue Integration, eds J. Trommershauser, K. Körding, and M. S. Landy (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 5-30.
    • (2011) Book of Sensory Cue Integration , pp. 5-30
    • Landy, M.S.1    Banks, M.S.2    Knill, D.C.3
  • 16
    • 69049106429 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Optimal integration of auditory and vibrotactile information for judgments of temporal order
    • Ley, I., Haggard, P., and Yarrow, K. (2009). Optimal integration of auditory and vibrotactile information for judgments of temporal order. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 35, 1005-1019. doi: 10.1037/a0015021
    • (2009) J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform , vol.35 , pp. 1005-1019
    • Ley, I.1    Haggard, P.2    Yarrow, K.3
  • 17
    • 55549110768 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The effect of attention on the illusory capture of motion in bimodal stimuli
    • Oruc, I., Sinnett, S., Bischof, W. F., Soto-Faraco, S., Lock, K., and Kingstone, A. (2008). The effect of attention on the illusory capture of motion in bimodal stimuli. Brain Res. 1242, 200-208. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.04.014
    • (2008) Brain Res , vol.1242 , pp. 200-208
    • Oruc, I.1    Sinnett, S.2    Bischof, W.F.3    Soto-Faraco, S.4    Lock, K.5    Kingstone, A.6
  • 18
    • 0036597926 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The ventriloquist in motion: Illusory capture of dynamic information across sensory modalities
    • Soto-Faraco, S., Lyons, J., Gazzaniga, M., Spence, C., and Kingstone, A. (2002). The ventriloquist in motion: illusory capture of dynamic information across sensory modalities. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 14, 139-146. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00068-X
    • (2002) Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res , vol.14 , pp. 139-146
    • Soto-Faraco, S.1    Lyons, J.2    Gazzaniga, M.3    Spence, C.4    Kingstone, A.5
  • 19
    • 0023885023 scopus 로고
    • Increased attention enhances both behavioral and neuronal performance
    • Spitzer, H., Desimone, R., and Moran, J. (1988). Increased attention enhances both behavioral and neuronal performance.Science 240, 338-340. doi: 10.1126/science.3353728
    • (1988) Science , vol.240 , pp. 338-340
    • Spitzer, H.1    Desimone, R.2    Moran, J.3
  • 20
    • 0035345876 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The ventriloquist effect does not depend on the direction of automatic visual attention
    • Vroomen, J., Bertelson, P., and de Gelder, B. (2001). The ventriloquist effect does not depend on the direction of automatic visual attention. Percept. Psychophys. 63, 651-659. doi: 10.3758/BF03194427
    • (2001) Percept. Psychophys , vol.63 , pp. 651-659
    • Vroomen, J.1    Bertelson, P.2    de Gelder, B.3
  • 21
    • 0019832386 scopus 로고
    • The role of visual-auditory "compellingness" in the ventriloquism effect: Implications for transitivity among the spatial senses
    • Warren, D. H., Welch, R. B., and McCarthy, T. J. (1981). The role of visual-auditory "compellingness" in the ventriloquism effect: implications for transitivity among the spatial senses. Percept. Psychophys. 30, 557-564. doi: 10.3758/BF03202010
    • (1981) Percept. Psychophys , vol.30 , pp. 557-564
    • Warren, D.H.1    Welch, R.B.2    McCarthy, T.J.3
  • 22
    • 0020713980 scopus 로고
    • QUEST: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method
    • Watson, A. B., and Pelli, D. G. (1983). QUEST: a Bayesian adaptive psychometric method. Percept. Psychophys. 33, 113-120. doi: 10.3758/BF03202828
    • (1983) Percept. Psychophys , vol.33 , pp. 113-120
    • Watson, A.B.1    Pelli, D.G.2
  • 23
    • 64049108071 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Temporal frequency channels are linked across audition and touch
    • Yau, J. M., Olenczak, J. B., Dammann, J. F., and Bensmaia, S. J. (2009). Temporal frequency channels are linked across audition and touch. Curr. Biol. 19, 561-566. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.02.013
    • (2009) Curr. Biol , vol.19 , pp. 561-566
    • Yau, J.M.1    Olenczak, J.B.2    Dammann, J.F.3    Bensmaia, S.J.4
  • 24
    • 84255180431 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Separate mechanisms for audio-tactile pitch and loudness interactions
    • Yau, J. M., Weber, A. I., and Bensmaia, S. J. (2010). Separate mechanisms for audio-tactile pitch and loudness interactions.Front. Psychol. 1:160. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00160
    • (2010) Front. Psychol , vol.1 , pp. 160
    • Yau, J.M.1    Weber, A.I.2    Bensmaia, S.J.3
  • 25
    • 0032487866 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Attention improves or impairs visual performance by enhancing spatial resolution
    • Yeshurun, Y., and Carrasco, M. (1998). Attention improves or impairs visual performance by enhancing spatial resolution.Nature 396, 72-75. doi: 10.1038/23936
    • (1998) Nature , vol.396 , pp. 72-75
    • Yeshurun, Y.1    Carrasco, M.2


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.