메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 48, Issue 1, 2012, Pages 185-191

Sometimes losing your self in space: Children's and adults' spontaneous use of multiple spatial reference frames

Author keywords

Cognitive development; Perspective taking; Spatial cognition; Theory of mind

Indexed keywords

ADOLESCENT; ADULT; AGE; AGING; ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE; ARTICLE; CHILD; CHILD DEVELOPMENT; COGNITION; DECISION MAKING; DEPTH PERCEPTION; FEMALE; HUMAN; MALE; NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL TEST; ORIENTATION; PHOTOSTIMULATION; PHYSIOLOGY; REFERENCE VALUE; SOCIAL BEHAVIOR;

EID: 84860824012     PISSN: 00121649     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1037/a0025863     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (25)

References (18)
  • 1
    • 84874233098 scopus 로고
    • Children's interpretations of ambiguous spatial descriptions
    • doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.1987.tb01055.x
    • Bialystok, E., & Codd, J. (1987). Children's interpretations of ambiguous spatial descriptions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 205-211. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.1987.tb01055.x
    • (1987) British Journal of Developmental Psychology , vol.5 , pp. 205-211
    • Bialystok, E.1    Codd, J.2
  • 2
    • 30044447268 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Object-centred spatial reference in 4-month-old infants
    • doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2005.06.003
    • Bremner, A. J., Bryant, P. E., & Mareschal, D. (2006). Object-centred spatial reference in 4-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 29, 1-10. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2005.06.003
    • (2006) Infant Behavior and Development , vol.29 , pp. 1-10
    • Bremner, A.J.1    Bryant, P.E.2    Mareschal, D.3
  • 3
    • 35548999562 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Recognition of complex object-centred spatial configurations in early infancy
    • doi:10.1080/13506280601029739
    • Bremner, A. J., Bryant, P., Mareschal, D., & Volein, A. (2007). Recognition of complex object-centred spatial configurations in early infancy. Visual Cognition, 15, 896-926. doi:10.1080/13506280601029739
    • (2007) Visual Cognition , vol.15 , pp. 896-926
    • Bremner, A.J.1    Bryant, P.2    Mareschal, D.3    Volein, A.4
  • 4
    • 0027569935 scopus 로고
    • Frames of reference in vision and language: Where is above?
    • doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(93)90011-J
    • Carlson-Radvansky, L. A., & Irwin, D. E. (1993). Frames of reference in vision and language: Where is above? Cognition, 46, 223-244. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(93)90011-J
    • (1993) Cognition , vol.46 , pp. 223-244
    • Carlson-Radvansky, L.A.1    Irwin, D.E.2
  • 5
    • 0346155222 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Inhibition accompanies reference-frame selection
    • doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00072
    • Carlson-Radvansky, L. A., & Jiang, Y. H. (1998). Inhibition accompanies reference-frame selection. Psychological Science, 9, 386-391. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00072
    • (1998) Psychological Science , vol.9 , pp. 386-391
    • Carlson-Radvansky, L.A.1    Jiang, Y.H.2
  • 6
    • 84970399864 scopus 로고
    • Interpretation of the spatial prepositions in front of and behind
    • Cox, M. V. (1981). Interpretation of the spatial prepositions in front of and behind. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 4, 359-368.
    • (1981) International Journal of Behavioral Development , vol.4 , pp. 359-368
    • Cox, M.V.1
  • 7
    • 33745609502 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching
    • doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia. 2006.02.006
    • Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L. C., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2037-2078. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia. 2006.02.006
    • (2006) Neuropsychologia , vol.44 , pp. 2037-2078
    • Davidson, M.C.1    Amso, D.2    Anderson, L.C.3    Diamond, A.4
  • 8
    • 55449105553 scopus 로고
    • Role of front-back features in children's front, back, and beside placements of objects
    • Harris, L. J., & Strommen, E. A. (1972). Role of front-back features in children's front, back, and beside placements of objects. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 18, 259-271.
    • (1972) Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development , vol.18 , pp. 259-271
    • Harris, L.1    Strommen, E.A.2
  • 9
    • 71549126835 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The embodied nature of spatial perspective taking: Embodied transformation versus sensorimotor interference
    • doi:10.1016/j.cognition .2009.08.015
    • Kessler, K., & Thomson, L. A. (2010). The embodied nature of spatial perspective taking: Embodied transformation versus sensorimotor interference. Cognition, 114, 72-88. doi:10.1016/j.cognition .2009.08.015
    • (2010) Cognition , vol.114 , pp. 72-88
    • Kessler, K.1    Thomson, L.A.2
  • 10
    • 0000862205 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Language and space
    • doi:10.1146/annurev.anthro.25.1.353
    • Levinson, S. C. (1996). Language and space. Annual Review of Anthropology, 25, 353-382. doi:10.1146/annurev.anthro.25.1.353
    • (1996) Annual Review of Anthropology , vol.25 , pp. 353-382
    • Levinson, S.C.1
  • 11
    • 0020744521 scopus 로고
    • Own view versus good view in a perspectivetaking task
    • doi:10.2307/1129709
    • Light, P., & Nix, C. (1983). Own view versus good view in a perspectivetaking task. Child Development, 54, 480-483. doi:10.2307/1129709
    • (1983) Child Development , vol.54 , pp. 480-483
    • Light, P.1    Nix, C.2
  • 12
    • 33745770479 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Two kinds of visual perspective taking
    • doi:10.3758/ BF03193680
    • Michelon, P., & Zacks, J. M. (2006). Two kinds of visual perspective taking. Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 327-337. doi:10.3758/ BF03193680
    • (2006) Perception & Psychophysics , vol.68 , pp. 327-337
    • Michelon, P.1    Zacks, J.M.2
  • 13
    • 0033477656 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • How we know-and sometimes misjudge-what others know: Imputing one's own knowledge to others
    • doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.6.737
    • Nickerson, R. S. (1999). How we know-and sometimes misjudge-what others know: Imputing one's own knowledge to others. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 737-759. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.6.737
    • (1999) Psychological Bulletin , vol.125 , pp. 737-759
    • Nickerson, R.S.1
  • 15
    • 77957655494 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Executive function is necessary for perspective selection, not Level-1 visual perspective calculation: Evidence from a dual-task study of adults
    • doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.08.003
    • Qureshi, A. W., Apperly, I. A., & Samson, D. (2010). Executive function is necessary for perspective selection, not Level-1 visual perspective calculation: Evidence from a dual-task study of adults. Cognition, 117, 230-236. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.08.003
    • (2010) Cognition , vol.117 , pp. 230-236
    • Qureshi, A.W.1    Apperly, I.A.2    Samson, D.3
  • 17
    • 84874261469 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Egocentrism and automatic perspective-taking in children and adults
    • Surtees, A. D. R., & Apperly, I. A. (in press). Egocentrism and automatic perspective-taking in children and adults. Child Development.
    • Child Development
    • Surtees, A.D.R.1    Apperly, I.A.2
  • 18
    • 58049198293 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Embodied and disembodied cognition: Spatial perspective-taking
    • doi:10.1016/ j.cognition.2008.10.008
    • Tversky, B., & Hard, B. M. (2009). Embodied and disembodied cognition: Spatial perspective-taking. Cognition, 110, 124-129. doi:10.1016/ j.cognition.2008.10.008
    • (2009) Cognition , vol.110 , pp. 124-129
    • Tversky, B.1    Hard, B.M.2


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.