메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 57, Issue 2, 1999, Pages 145-160

Horror and humor

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords


EID: 61249378611     PISSN: 00218529     EISSN: 15406245     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.2307/432309     Document Type: Review
Times cited : (69)

References (43)
  • 2
    • 77952125394 scopus 로고
    • Preface to the Second Edition
    • Walpole himself described the work as a mixture of "buffoonery and solemnity" in the London: Collier-Macmillan
    • Walpole himself described the work as a mixture of "buffoonery and solemnity" in the "Preface to the Second Edition" of The Castle of Otranto (London: Collier-Macmillan, 1963), p. 21
    • (1963) The Castle of Otranto , pp. 21
  • 4
    • 0041023365 scopus 로고
    • Metuchen, NJ: Scarecrow Press
    • Donald F. Glut, The Frankenstein Legend (Metuchen, NJ: Scarecrow Press, 1973), p. 33
    • (1973) The Frankenstein Legend , pp. 33
    • Glut, D.F.1
  • 5
    • 77952173037 scopus 로고
    • as interviewed ed. Stanley Wlator New York: Avon
    • Stuart Gordon, as interviewed in Dark Visions, ed. Stanley Wlator (New York: Avon, 1992), p. 84
    • (1992) Dark Visions , pp. 84
    • Gordon, S.1
  • 6
    • 77952155262 scopus 로고
    • as interviewed ed. Douglas Winter New York: Berkeley Books
    • Robert Bloch, as interviewed in Faces of Fear, ed. Douglas Winter (New York: Berkeley Books, 1985), p. 22
    • (1985) Faces of Fear , pp. 22
    • Bloch, R.1
  • 7
    • 77952123533 scopus 로고
    • American Prose Writers, No. 2: N. P. Willis
    • January 18
    • Edgar Allan Poe, "American Prose Writers, No. 2: N. P. Willis," Broadway Journal no. 3, January 18, 1845
    • (1845) Broadway Journal , Issue.3
    • Poe, E.A.1
  • 8
    • 0010859839 scopus 로고
    • The 'Uncanny,'
    • New York: Collier
    • Sigmund Freud, "The 'Uncanny,'" in Studies in Parapsychology (New York: Collier, 1963), pp. 19-62
    • (1963) Studies in Parapsychology , pp. 19-62
    • Freud, S.1
  • 11
    • 0003674479 scopus 로고
    • The notion that problems of perceptual indiscernibility are the hallmark of philosophical inquiry is advanced by in his Harvard University Press
    • The notion that problems of perceptual indiscernibility are the hallmark of philosophical inquiry is advanced by Arthur Danto in his Transfiguration of the Commonplace (Harvard University Press, 1981)
    • (1981) Transfiguration of the Commonplace
    • Danto, A.1
  • 12
    • 53949118107 scopus 로고
    • I am not fully convinced that we should construct a theory of horror that includes these psychotics. Thus, what follows above is a conditional extension of the theory that I presented in The Philosophy of Horror under the presumption that the theory should be expanded to accommodate certain psychotics. So, if one wishes to count The Silence of the Lambs as a horror fiction, the above account suggests how that might be done in a way that is maximally consistent with my Philosophy of Horror. A similar approach can be found in Peter Penzoldt, The Supernatural in Fiction (London: Peter Neville, 1952), p. 12
    • (1952) The Supernatural in Fiction , pp. 12
    • Penzoldt, P.1
  • 13
    • 0039150724 scopus 로고
    • University of Texas Press
    • and S. T. Joshi, The Weird Tale (University of Texas Press, 1990), p. 80
    • (1990) The Weird Tale , pp. 80
    • Joshi, S.T.1
  • 15
    • 77952117022 scopus 로고
    • Enjoying Horror Fictions: A Reply to Gaut
    • This paragraph repeats an argument that I made in
    • This paragraph repeats an argument that I made in Noel Carroll, "Enjoying Horror Fictions: A Reply to Gaut," The British Journal of Aesthetics 35 (1995): 67-72
    • (1995) The British Journal of Aesthetics , vol.35 , pp. 67-72
    • Carroll, N.1
  • 16
    • 61149288447 scopus 로고
    • The Paradox of Horror
    • I have characterized the relation of fear and disgust above as a complex compound because horror does not merely involve the simple addition of these two components. Horror is not simply the result of adding danger to impurity. For when elements that are independently harmful and impure are yoked together by horrific iconography, the impurity component undergoes a change. It becomes fearsome in its own right. That is, the impurity element comes to be fearsome in itself. It is as if the impurity comes to be, so to speak, toxic. The fearsomeness component in horrific imagery works like a chemical agent in activating or releasing a dormant property of the impurity. It catalyzes the impurity component. The impurity of the monster becomes, in addition to being merely disgusting, one of the fearsome properties of the monster. In Alien, when the creature bursts out of the egg, it is fearsome because of its evident power and speed. But the fearsomeness of the creature in light of its power and speed also encourages us to regard its squishy carapace as dangerous in its own terms. You wouldn't want to touch it for fear that it might contaminate you. Horror, then, is not simply a function of fear in response to the overt lethal capacity of the monster to maim plus disgust in response to the monster's impurity. For when fear and disgust are mixed in horror-provoking imagery, what is disgusting becomes additionally fearsome in its own way. Call this process toxification. This process of toxification, moreover, is important theoretically. For one of the things that happens, as we will see in the next section of this essay, is that when fear is subtracted from potentially horrific imagery-as happens in much comedy-the imagery becomes detoxified. This is why what I call category jamming is not a sufficient condition for a horrific response. Impure, incongruous entities can be presented detoxified, so to say, as is the case in much humor. Lastly, the phenomenon of toxification is important because it suggests a way in which I might be able to answer a recent criticism of my Philosophy of Horror. In the process of answering what I call the paradox of horror, I maintained that being horrified is unpleasant and that the pleasure we derive from horror fictions comes from elsewhere (notably from our fascination with the design of the monstrosity along with certain recurrent forms of plotting). Berys Gaut, in contrast, argues that the pleasure derived from horror fictions comes from being horrified. One of the ways that Gaut defends this view is by pointing out that even if being horrified is necessarily typically unpleasant, this is consistent with some people sometimes taking pleasure from being horrified. These will be atypical people in atypical situations. And horror audiences, by hypothesis, are of this sort. Responding to this proposal, I argue that it is strange to regard either the responses of horror audiences or the situation of being art-horrified by horror fictions to be atypical. Indeed, I contend that the situation of being art-horrified by horror fictions is the norm, since we are rarely, if ever, horrified, in the sense of art-horror, anywhere else but in response to horror fictions. In ordinary experience there are no monsters. So we have little recourse in real life to be horrified in the sense that I use that term. But Gaut questions my claim that we rarely, if ever, experience the relevant sort of horror in real life. He maintains that we often experience fear and disgust separately. So, if horror is the result of merely conjoining fear and disgust, then there is no reason to suppose that they might not be experienced together with respect to some object in real life. However, in response to Gaut, I would like to argue that what I call horror involves the toxification process discussed in the first paragraph of this note. Thus, art-horror involves fear (divorced from impurity), disgust, and, as a consequence of the mixture of these two elements, a third element, viz., fear-of-toxification. This emotion, particularly with regard to the impression of toxification, is not typical in ordinary life. It is primarily an artifact of the horror genre. So, it does not seem right to characterize the horror audience as atypical with respect to art-horror. Rather, they are definitive of it. Therefore, Gaut cannot exploit the typicality operator, in the way that he suggests, in order to dissolve the paradox of horror. See: Berys Gaut, "The Paradox of Horror," The British Journal of Aesthetics 33 (1993): 333-345
    • (1993) The British Journal of Aesthetics , vol.33 , pp. 333-345
    • Gaut, B.1
  • 17
    • 77952195190 scopus 로고
    • The Enjoyment Theory of Horror: A Response to Carroll
    • Berys Gaut, "The Enjoyment Theory of Horror: A Response to Carroll," The British Journal of Aesthetics 36 (1995): 284-289
    • (1995) The British Journal of Aesthetics , vol.36 , pp. 284-289
    • Gaut, B.1
  • 19
    • 0004086998 scopus 로고
    • London: Routledge and Kegan Paul
    • Mary Douglas, Purity and Danger (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1966)
    • (1966) Purity and Danger
    • Douglas, M.1
  • 20
    • 0002809050 scopus 로고
    • Anthropological Aspects of Language: Animal Categories and Verbal Abuse
    • ed. Eric H. Lenneberg MIT Press
    • and Edmund Leach, "Anthropological Aspects of Language: Animal Categories and Verbal Abuse," in New Directions in the Study of Language, ed. Eric H. Lenneberg (MIT Press, 1964), pp. 23-63
    • (1964) New Directions in the Study of Language , pp. 23-63
    • Leach, E.1
  • 23
    • 79957291126 scopus 로고
    • reprinted in John Morreall, ed., SUNY Press
    • reprinted in John Morreall, ed., The Philosophy of Laughter and Humor (SUNY Press, 1987), p. 32
    • (1987) The Philosophy of Laughter and Humor , pp. 32
  • 24
    • 70349508163 scopus 로고
    • On Laughter and Ludicrous Composition
    • his Edinburgh: Creech
    • James Beattie, "On Laughter and Ludicrous Composition," in his Essays (Edinburgh: Creech, 1776)
    • (1776) Essays
    • Beattie, J.1
  • 27
    • 0039806967 scopus 로고
    • trans. R. B. Haidane and John Kemp, 6th ed. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, book I, section 13
    • Arthur Schopenhauer, The World as Will and Idea, trans. R. B. Haidane and John Kemp, 6th ed. (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1907), book I, section 13
    • (1907) The World As Will and Idea
    • Schopenhauer, A.1
  • 31
    • 84971946969 scopus 로고
    • Humor and Incongruity
    • Michael Clark, øHumor and Incongruity,ø Philosophy 45 (1970): 20-32
    • (1970) Philosophy , vol.45 , pp. 20-32
    • Clark, M.1
  • 32
    • 0345876699 scopus 로고
    • Adapted from Mel Brooks. See Harmondsworth: Penguin
    • Adapted from Mel Brooks. See Joshua Halberstam, Everyday Ethics (Harmondsworth: Penguin, 1993), p. 83
    • (1993) Everyday Ethics , pp. 83
    • Halberstam, J.1
  • 34
    • 79957360036 scopus 로고
    • The Clown as the Lord of Disorder
    • ed. M. C. Hyers New York: Seabury
    • Wolfgang Zucker, "The Clown as the Lord of Disorder," in Holy Laughter, ed. M. C. Hyers (New York: Seabury, 1960)
    • (1960) Holy Laughter
    • Zucker, W.1
  • 37
    • 84928451492 scopus 로고
    • The Virgin and the Clown
    • Pnina Werbner, "The Virgin and the Clown," Man, n.s. (1986): 21
    • (1986) Man , pp. 21
    • Werbner, P.1
  • 39
    • 79957006233 scopus 로고
    • This routine can be seen on which is distributed by Goodtimes Home Video Corp., 401 5th Avenue, New York, New York Goodtimes Home Video Corp., Movietime Inc. Archives, 1986
    • This routine can be seen on Great Comedians: TV-The Early Years, which is distributed by Goodtimes Home Video Corp., 401 5th Avenue, New York, New York (Goodtimes Home Video Corp., 1987; Movietime Inc. Archives, 1986)
    • (1987) Great Comedians: TV-The Early Years
  • 40
    • 0037825345 scopus 로고
    • Incongruity, Problem-Solving and Laughter
    • eds. Antony J. Chapman and Hugh C. Foot London: John Wiley and Sons
    • Mary K. Rothbart, "Incongruity, Problem-Solving and Laughter," in Humour and Laughter: Theory, Research and Applications, eds. Antony J. Chapman and Hugh C. Foot (London: John Wiley and Sons, 1976), pp. 37-54
    • (1976) Humour and Laughter: Theory, Research and Applications , pp. 37-54
    • Rothbart, M.K.1
  • 41
    • 0039070463 scopus 로고
    • "Elephants and Marshmallows: A Theoretical Synthesis of Incongruity-Resolution and Arousal Theories of Humour," and "Psychological Approaches to the Study of Humour,"
    • See also Mary K. Rothbart and Diana Pien, "Elephants and Marshmallows: A Theoretical Synthesis of Incongruity-Resolution and Arousal Theories of Humour," and "Psychological Approaches to the Study of Humour," in It's A Funny Thing, Humour, eds. Antony J. Chapman and Hugh C. Foot (New York: Pergamon Press, 1977), pp. 37-40 and 87-94 respectively
    • (1977) It's a Funny Thing, Humour , pp. 37-40
    • Rothbart, M.K.1    Pien, D.2
  • 43
    • 0015729294 scopus 로고
    • Laughter in Young Children
    • Mary Rothbart, "Laughter in Young Children," Psychological Bulletin 80 (1973): 247-256
    • (1973) Psychological Bulletin , vol.80 , pp. 247-256
    • Rothbart, M.1


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.