-
2
-
-
0004145176
-
-
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
-
For this historical background, see Kurt Danziger, Constructing the Subject: Historical Origins of Psychological Research (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1990) and Saul Rosenzweig, "Norms and the Individual in the Psychologist's Perspective," in Feelings and Emotions: The Mooseheart Symposium, M. L. Reymert, ed. (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1950): 327-335.
-
(1990)
Constructing the Subject: Historical Origins of Psychological Research
-
-
Danziger, K.1
-
3
-
-
0242640094
-
Norms and the Individual in the Psychologist's Perspective
-
M. L. Reymert, ed. New York: McGraw-Hill
-
For this historical background, see Kurt Danziger, Constructing the Subject: Historical Origins of Psychological Research (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1990) and Saul Rosenzweig, "Norms and the Individual in the Psychologist's Perspective," in Feelings and Emotions: The Mooseheart Symposium, M. L. Reymert, ed. (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1950): 327-335.
-
(1950)
Feelings and Emotions: The Mooseheart Symposium
, pp. 327-335
-
-
Rosenzweig, S.1
-
5
-
-
0004303824
-
-
Leipzig: Engelmann
-
This statement regarding Wundt's orientation refers primarily to his earlier contributions and to the influence he exerted in America through the rather selective emphasis of Edward B. Titchener who received his Ph.D. under Wundt at Leipzig in 1892. It was through Titchener that most American psychologists of that generation knew about Wundt and believed him to be a "structuralist" like Titchener who limited scientific psychology to facts acquired by observers trained in introspection. In fact, Wundt in the totality of his career had a broad purview which included what is today called social psychology. To that scientific area he contributed his Völkerpsychologie: Eine Untersuchung der Entwicklungsgesetze von Sprache, Mythus, und Sitte (Leipzig: Engelmann) which appeared in ten volumes over a period of twenty years (1900-1920). The year 1920 was the year of his death. An abridged English translation of the first five volumes was published under the title Elements of Folk Psychology: Outlines of a Psychological History of the Development of Mankind (New York: Macmillan, 1916). The biased and partial nature of Wundt's reputation has recently been strenuously corrected by psychologists like Arthur L. Blumenthal. For example, see Arthur Blumenthal, "A Reappraisal of Wilhelm Wundt," American Psychologist 30 (1975): 1081-1088. See also the collective volume Wilhelm Wundt and the Making of a Scientific Psychology R. W. Rieber, Ed. (New York: Plenum Press, 1980); and Wundt Studies: A Centennial Collection Wolfgang G. Bringmann and Ryan D. Tweney, Eds. (Toronto: C. J. Hogrefe, 1980). In this context it has not been sufficiently stressed that the extra-physiological (experimental) work of Wundt was done in the last fourth of his life, long after his more influential American students, like G. S. Hall and Titchener had known him in Leipzig. Oddly enough, and unlike Wundt's American students, his contributions to folk psychology were recognized and cited by the founder of psychoanalysis, Sigmund Freud, who in 1911-1913 discussed Wundt's extensive treatment of totemism, taboos and exogamy in his own book Totem and Taboo in the Standard Edition of Sigmund Freud's Complete Psychological Works, volume 13, (London, Hogarth Press, 1955): 1-161. Despite the recent repudiation of Titchener's alleged one-sided interpretation of Wundt's contribution to psychology. Titchener himself in a generous tribute to his early mentor, published as an obituary that included the Völkerpsychologie, came to the prophetic conclusion: ". . . If a man is to gain his niche in history he must have the total vision, the generative idea. . . . In this sense I am prepared to say that Wundt is the founder, not of experimental psychology alone, but of psychology. . . . But the dominant idea of Wundt's life, the idea upon which his reputation is most solidly based, the idea that persisted with him to the very end of his university activity, is the idea of an experimental psychology." See American Journal of Psychology 32 (1921): 161-178.
-
(1900)
Völkerpsychologie: Eine Untersuchung der Entwicklungsgesetze Von Sprache, Mythus, und Sitte
-
-
-
6
-
-
0003857153
-
-
New York: Macmillan
-
This statement regarding Wundt's orientation refers primarily to his earlier contributions and to the influence he exerted in America through the rather selective emphasis of Edward B. Titchener who received his Ph.D. under Wundt at Leipzig in 1892. It was through Titchener that most American psychologists of that generation knew about Wundt and believed him to be a "structuralist" like Titchener who limited scientific psychology to facts acquired by observers trained in introspection. In fact, Wundt in the totality of his career had a broad purview which included what is today called social psychology. To that scientific area he contributed his Völkerpsychologie: Eine Untersuchung der Entwicklungsgesetze von Sprache, Mythus, und Sitte (Leipzig: Engelmann) which appeared in ten volumes over a period of twenty years (1900-1920). The year 1920 was the year of his death. An abridged English translation of the first five volumes was published under the title Elements of Folk Psychology: Outlines of a Psychological History of the Development of Mankind (New York: Macmillan, 1916). The biased and partial nature of Wundt's reputation has recently been strenuously corrected by psychologists like Arthur L. Blumenthal. For example, see Arthur Blumenthal, "A Reappraisal of Wilhelm Wundt," American Psychologist 30 (1975): 1081-1088. See also the collective volume Wilhelm Wundt and the Making of a Scientific Psychology R. W. Rieber, Ed. (New York: Plenum Press, 1980); and Wundt Studies: A Centennial Collection Wolfgang G. Bringmann and Ryan D. Tweney, Eds. (Toronto: C. J. Hogrefe, 1980). In this context it has not been sufficiently stressed that the extra-physiological (experimental) work of Wundt was done in the last fourth of his life, long after his more influential American students, like G. S. Hall and Titchener had known him in Leipzig. Oddly enough, and unlike Wundt's American students, his contributions to folk psychology were recognized and cited by the founder of psychoanalysis, Sigmund Freud, who in 1911-1913 discussed Wundt's extensive treatment of totemism, taboos and exogamy in his own book Totem and Taboo in the Standard Edition of Sigmund Freud's Complete Psychological Works, volume 13, (London, Hogarth Press, 1955): 1-161. Despite the recent repudiation of Titchener's alleged one-sided interpretation of Wundt's contribution to psychology. Titchener himself in a generous tribute to his early mentor, published as an obituary that included the Völkerpsychologie, came to the prophetic conclusion: ". . . If a man is to gain his niche in history he must have the total vision, the generative idea. . . . In this sense I am prepared to say that Wundt is the founder, not of experimental psychology alone, but of psychology. . . . But the dominant idea of Wundt's life, the idea upon which his reputation is most solidly based, the idea that persisted with him to the very end of his university activity, is the idea of an experimental psychology." See American Journal of Psychology 32 (1921): 161-178.
-
(1916)
Elements of Folk Psychology: Outlines of a Psychological History of the Development of Mankind
-
-
-
7
-
-
0000056646
-
A Reappraisal of Wilhelm Wundt
-
This statement regarding Wundt's orientation refers primarily to his earlier contributions and to the influence he exerted in America through the rather selective emphasis of Edward B. Titchener who received his Ph.D. under Wundt at Leipzig in 1892. It was through Titchener that most American psychologists of that generation knew about Wundt and believed him to be a "structuralist" like Titchener who limited scientific psychology to facts acquired by observers trained in introspection. In fact, Wundt in the totality of his career had a broad purview which included what is today called social psychology. To that scientific area he contributed his Völkerpsychologie: Eine Untersuchung der Entwicklungsgesetze von Sprache, Mythus, und Sitte (Leipzig: Engelmann) which appeared in ten volumes over a period of twenty years (1900-1920). The year 1920 was the year of his death. An abridged English translation of the first five volumes was published under the title Elements of Folk Psychology: Outlines of a Psychological History of the Development of Mankind (New York: Macmillan, 1916). The biased and partial nature of Wundt's reputation has recently been strenuously corrected by psychologists like Arthur L. Blumenthal. For example, see Arthur Blumenthal, "A Reappraisal of Wilhelm Wundt," American Psychologist 30 (1975): 1081-1088. See also the collective volume Wilhelm Wundt and the Making of a Scientific Psychology R. W. Rieber, Ed. (New York: Plenum Press, 1980); and Wundt Studies: A Centennial Collection Wolfgang G. Bringmann and Ryan D. Tweney, Eds. (Toronto: C. J. Hogrefe, 1980). In this context it has not been sufficiently stressed that the extra-physiological (experimental) work of Wundt was done in the last fourth of his life, long after his more influential American students, like G. S. Hall and Titchener had known him in Leipzig. Oddly enough, and unlike Wundt's American students, his contributions to folk psychology were recognized and cited by the founder of psychoanalysis, Sigmund Freud, who in 1911-1913 discussed Wundt's extensive treatment of totemism, taboos and exogamy in his own book Totem and Taboo in the Standard Edition of Sigmund Freud's Complete Psychological Works, volume 13, (London, Hogarth Press, 1955): 1-161. Despite the recent repudiation of Titchener's alleged one-sided interpretation of Wundt's contribution to psychology. Titchener himself in a generous tribute to his early mentor, published as an obituary that included the Völkerpsychologie, came to the prophetic conclusion: ". . . If a man is to gain his niche in history he must have the total vision, the generative idea. . . . In this sense I am prepared to say that Wundt is the founder, not of experimental psychology alone, but of psychology. . . . But the dominant idea of Wundt's life, the idea upon which his reputation is most solidly based, the idea that persisted with him to the very end of his university activity, is the idea of an experimental psychology." See American Journal of Psychology 32 (1921): 161-178.
-
(1975)
American Psychologist
, vol.30
, pp. 1081-1088
-
-
Blumenthal, A.1
-
8
-
-
33845573011
-
-
New York: Plenum Press
-
This statement regarding Wundt's orientation refers primarily to his earlier contributions and to the influence he exerted in America through the rather selective emphasis of Edward B. Titchener who received his Ph.D. under Wundt at Leipzig in 1892. It was through Titchener that most American psychologists of that generation knew about Wundt and believed him to be a "structuralist" like Titchener who limited scientific psychology to facts acquired by observers trained in introspection. In fact, Wundt in the totality of his career had a broad purview which included what is today called social psychology. To that scientific area he contributed his Völkerpsychologie: Eine Untersuchung der Entwicklungsgesetze von Sprache, Mythus, und Sitte (Leipzig: Engelmann) which appeared in ten volumes over a period of twenty years (1900-1920). The year 1920 was the year of his death. An abridged English translation of the first five volumes was published under the title Elements of Folk Psychology: Outlines of a Psychological History of the Development of Mankind (New York: Macmillan, 1916). The biased and partial nature of Wundt's reputation has recently been strenuously corrected by psychologists like Arthur L. Blumenthal. For example, see Arthur Blumenthal, "A Reappraisal of Wilhelm Wundt," American Psychologist 30 (1975): 1081-1088. See also the collective volume Wilhelm Wundt and the Making of a Scientific Psychology R. W. Rieber, Ed. (New York: Plenum Press, 1980); and Wundt Studies: A Centennial Collection Wolfgang G. Bringmann and Ryan D. Tweney, Eds. (Toronto: C. J. Hogrefe, 1980). In this context it has not been sufficiently stressed that the extra-physiological (experimental) work of Wundt was done in the last fourth of his life, long after his more influential American students, like G. S. Hall and Titchener had known him in Leipzig. Oddly enough, and unlike Wundt's American students, his contributions to folk psychology were recognized and cited by the founder of psychoanalysis, Sigmund Freud, who in 1911-1913 discussed Wundt's extensive treatment of totemism, taboos and exogamy in his own book Totem and Taboo in the Standard Edition of Sigmund Freud's Complete Psychological Works, volume 13, (London, Hogarth Press, 1955): 1-161. Despite the recent repudiation of Titchener's alleged one-sided interpretation of Wundt's contribution to psychology. Titchener himself in a generous tribute to his early mentor, published as an obituary that included the Völkerpsychologie, came to the prophetic conclusion: ". . . If a man is to gain his niche in history he must have the total vision, the generative idea. . . . In this sense I am prepared to say that Wundt is the founder, not of experimental psychology alone, but of psychology. . . . But the dominant idea of Wundt's life, the idea upon which his reputation is most solidly based, the idea that persisted with him to the very end of his university activity, is the idea of an experimental psychology." See American Journal of Psychology 32 (1921): 161-178.
-
(1980)
Wilhelm Wundt and the Making of a Scientific Psychology
-
-
Rieber, R.W.1
-
9
-
-
0010147827
-
-
Toronto: C. J. Hogrefe
-
This statement regarding Wundt's orientation refers primarily to his earlier contributions and to the influence he exerted in America through the rather selective emphasis of Edward B. Titchener who received his Ph.D. under Wundt at Leipzig in 1892. It was through Titchener that most American psychologists of that generation knew about Wundt and believed him to be a "structuralist" like Titchener who limited scientific psychology to facts acquired by observers trained in introspection. In fact, Wundt in the totality of his career had a broad purview which included what is today called social psychology. To that scientific area he contributed his Völkerpsychologie: Eine Untersuchung der Entwicklungsgesetze von Sprache, Mythus, und Sitte (Leipzig: Engelmann) which appeared in ten volumes over a period of twenty years (1900-1920). The year 1920 was the year of his death. An abridged English translation of the first five volumes was published under the title Elements of Folk Psychology: Outlines of a Psychological History of the Development of Mankind (New York: Macmillan, 1916). The biased and partial nature of Wundt's reputation has recently been strenuously corrected by psychologists like Arthur L. Blumenthal. For example, see Arthur Blumenthal, "A Reappraisal of Wilhelm Wundt," American Psychologist 30 (1975): 1081-1088. See also the collective volume Wilhelm Wundt and the Making of a Scientific Psychology R. W. Rieber, Ed. (New York: Plenum Press, 1980); and Wundt Studies: A Centennial Collection Wolfgang G. Bringmann and Ryan D. Tweney, Eds. (Toronto: C. J. Hogrefe, 1980). In this context it has not been sufficiently stressed that the extra-physiological (experimental) work of Wundt was done in the last fourth of his life, long after his more influential American students, like G. S. Hall and Titchener had known him in Leipzig. Oddly enough, and unlike Wundt's American students, his contributions to folk psychology were recognized and cited by the founder of psychoanalysis, Sigmund Freud, who in 1911-1913 discussed Wundt's extensive treatment of totemism, taboos and exogamy in his own book Totem and Taboo in the Standard Edition of Sigmund Freud's Complete Psychological Works, volume 13, (London, Hogarth Press, 1955): 1-161. Despite the recent repudiation of Titchener's alleged one-sided interpretation of Wundt's contribution to psychology. Titchener himself in a generous tribute to his early mentor, published as an obituary that included the Völkerpsychologie, came to the prophetic conclusion: ". . . If a man is to gain his niche in history he must have the total vision, the generative idea. . . . In this sense I am prepared to say that Wundt is the founder, not of experimental psychology alone, but of psychology. . . . But the dominant idea of Wundt's life, the idea upon which his reputation is most solidly based, the idea that persisted with him to the very end of his university activity, is the idea of an experimental psychology." See American Journal of Psychology 32 (1921): 161-178.
-
(1980)
Wundt Studies: A Centennial Collection
-
-
Bringmann, W.G.1
Tweney, R.D.2
-
10
-
-
0348068998
-
-
London, Hogarth Press
-
This statement regarding Wundt's orientation refers primarily to his earlier contributions and to the influence he exerted in America through the rather selective emphasis of Edward B. Titchener who received his Ph.D. under Wundt at Leipzig in 1892. It was through Titchener that most American psychologists of that generation knew about Wundt and believed him to be a "structuralist" like Titchener who limited scientific psychology to facts acquired by observers trained in introspection. In fact, Wundt in the totality of his career had a broad purview which included what is today called social psychology. To that scientific area he contributed his Völkerpsychologie: Eine Untersuchung der Entwicklungsgesetze von Sprache, Mythus, und Sitte (Leipzig: Engelmann) which appeared in ten volumes over a period of twenty years (1900-1920). The year 1920 was the year of his death. An abridged English translation of the first five volumes was published under the title Elements of Folk Psychology: Outlines of a Psychological History of the Development of Mankind (New York: Macmillan, 1916). The biased and partial nature of Wundt's reputation has recently been strenuously corrected by psychologists like Arthur L. Blumenthal. For example, see Arthur Blumenthal, "A Reappraisal of Wilhelm Wundt," American Psychologist 30 (1975): 1081-1088. See also the collective volume Wilhelm Wundt and the Making of a Scientific Psychology R. W. Rieber, Ed. (New York: Plenum Press, 1980); and Wundt Studies: A Centennial Collection Wolfgang G. Bringmann and Ryan D. Tweney, Eds. (Toronto: C. J. Hogrefe, 1980). In this context it has not been sufficiently stressed that the extra-physiological (experimental) work of Wundt was done in the last fourth of his life, long after his more influential American students, like G. S. Hall and Titchener had known him in Leipzig. Oddly enough, and unlike Wundt's American students, his contributions to folk psychology were recognized and cited by the founder of psychoanalysis, Sigmund Freud, who in 1911-1913 discussed Wundt's extensive treatment of totemism, taboos and exogamy in his own book Totem and Taboo in the Standard Edition of Sigmund Freud's Complete Psychological Works, volume 13, (London, Hogarth Press, 1955): 1-161. Despite the recent repudiation of Titchener's alleged one-sided interpretation of Wundt's contribution to psychology. Titchener himself in a generous tribute to his early mentor, published as an obituary that included the Völkerpsychologie, came to the prophetic conclusion: ". . . If a man is to gain his niche in history he must have the total vision, the generative idea. . . . In this sense I am prepared to say that Wundt is the founder, not of experimental psychology alone, but of psychology. . . . But the dominant idea of Wundt's life, the idea upon which his reputation is most solidly based, the idea that persisted with him to the very end of his university activity, is the idea of an experimental psychology." See American Journal of Psychology 32 (1921): 161-178.
-
(1955)
Totem and Taboo in the Standard Edition of Sigmund Freud's Complete Psychological Works
, vol.13
, pp. 1-161
-
-
-
11
-
-
0001110546
-
-
This statement regarding Wundt's orientation refers primarily to his earlier contributions and to the influence he exerted in America through the rather selective emphasis of Edward B. Titchener who received his Ph.D. under Wundt at Leipzig in 1892. It was through Titchener that most American psychologists of that generation knew about Wundt and believed him to be a "structuralist" like Titchener who limited scientific psychology to facts acquired by observers trained in introspection. In fact, Wundt in the totality of his career had a broad purview which included what is today called social psychology. To that scientific area he contributed his Völkerpsychologie: Eine Untersuchung der Entwicklungsgesetze von Sprache, Mythus, und Sitte (Leipzig: Engelmann) which appeared in ten volumes over a period of twenty years (1900-1920). The year 1920 was the year of his death. An abridged English translation of the first five volumes was published under the title Elements of Folk Psychology: Outlines of a Psychological History of the Development of Mankind (New York: Macmillan, 1916). The biased and partial nature of Wundt's reputation has recently been strenuously corrected by psychologists like Arthur L. Blumenthal. For example, see Arthur Blumenthal, "A Reappraisal of Wilhelm Wundt," American Psychologist 30 (1975): 1081-1088. See also the collective volume Wilhelm Wundt and the Making of a Scientific Psychology R. W. Rieber, Ed. (New York: Plenum Press, 1980); and Wundt Studies: A Centennial Collection Wolfgang G. Bringmann and Ryan D. Tweney, Eds. (Toronto: C. J. Hogrefe, 1980). In this context it has not been sufficiently stressed that the extra-physiological (experimental) work of Wundt was done in the last fourth of his life, long after his more influential American students, like G. S. Hall and Titchener had known him in Leipzig. Oddly enough, and unlike Wundt's American students, his contributions to folk psychology were recognized and cited by the founder of psychoanalysis, Sigmund Freud, who in 1911-1913 discussed Wundt's extensive treatment of totemism, taboos and exogamy in his own book Totem and Taboo in the Standard Edition of Sigmund Freud's Complete Psychological Works, volume 13, (London, Hogarth Press, 1955): 1-161. Despite the recent repudiation of Titchener's alleged one-sided interpretation of Wundt's contribution to psychology. Titchener himself in a generous tribute to his early mentor, published as an obituary that included the Völkerpsychologie, came to the prophetic conclusion: ". . . If a man is to gain his niche in history he must have the total vision, the generative idea. . . . In this sense I am prepared to say that Wundt is the founder, not of experimental psychology alone, but of psychology. . . . But the dominant idea of Wundt's life, the idea upon which his reputation is most solidly based, the idea that persisted with him to the very end of his university activity, is the idea of an experimental psychology." See American Journal of Psychology 32 (1921): 161-178.
-
(1921)
American Journal of Psychology
, vol.32
, pp. 161-178
-
-
-
15
-
-
0038693184
-
-
Halle: Verlag von Max Niemeyer
-
Eduard Spranger, Lebensforemen: Geisteswissenschaftliche Psychologie und Ethik der Persönlichkeit, 1st edition (Halle: Verlag von Max Niemeyer, 1921). Eduard Spranger, Types of Men: The Psychology and Ethics of Personality, 5th ed., P. J. W. Pigors, Trans. (Halle: Max Niemeyer Verlag, 1928).
-
(1921)
Lebensforemen: Geisteswissenschaftliche Psychologie und Ethik der Persönlichkeit, 1st Edition
-
-
Spranger, E.1
-
16
-
-
0002346928
-
-
P. J. W. Pigors, Trans. Halle: Max Niemeyer Verlag
-
Eduard Spranger, Lebensforemen: Geisteswissenschaftliche Psychologie und Ethik der Persönlichkeit, 1st edition (Halle: Verlag von Max Niemeyer, 1921). Eduard Spranger, Types of Men: The Psychology and Ethics of Personality, 5th ed., P. J. W. Pigors, Trans. (Halle: Max Niemeyer Verlag, 1928).
-
(1928)
Types of Men: the Psychology and Ethics of Personality, 5th Ed.
-
-
Spranger, E.1
-
18
-
-
0348068964
-
-
note
-
It is not strictly true that the classification of the Geisteswissenschaften necessarily implies an idiographic orientation. The point is more correctly conceptualized in the formulation of three types of norms called for in the idiodynamic purview, in which demographic norms, along with the nomothetic and idiodynamic, allow for what is collectively embraced under Geisteswissenschaften.
-
-
-
-
19
-
-
33846284962
-
-
Leipzig: Barth, 3 volumes
-
See William Stern, Person und Sache: System des Kritischen Personalismus (Leipzig: Barth, 1923) 3 volumes; and William Stern, General Psychology from the Personalistic Standpoint, H. D. Spoerl, Trans. (New York: Macmillan, 1938).
-
(1923)
Person und Sache: System des Kritischen Personalismus
-
-
Stern, W.1
-
20
-
-
0003699189
-
-
H. D. Spoerl, Trans. New York: Macmillan
-
See William Stern, Person und Sache: System des Kritischen Personalismus (Leipzig: Barth, 1923) 3 volumes; and William Stern, General Psychology from the Personalistic Standpoint, H. D. Spoerl, Trans. (New York: Macmillan, 1938).
-
(1938)
General Psychology from the Personalistic Standpoint
-
-
Stern, W.1
-
23
-
-
0242640097
-
The Place of the Individual and of Idiodynamics in Psychology: A Dialogue
-
Saul Rosenzweig, "The Place of the Individual and of Idiodynamics in Psychology: A Dialogue," Journal of Individual Psychology 14 (1958): 3-20.
-
(1958)
Journal of Individual Psychology
, vol.14
, pp. 3-20
-
-
Rosenzweig, S.1
-
24
-
-
0002013184
-
Idiodynamics in Personality Theory with Special Reference to Projective Methods
-
Cf. Saul Rosenzweig, "Idiodynamics in Personality Theory with Special Reference to Projective Methods," Psychological Review 58 (1951): 213-223; Rosenzweig, "Freud and Experimental Psychology: The Emergence of Idiodynamics," in A Century of Psychology as Science, Sigmund Koch and David E. Leary, eds. (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1985); 135-207; Rosenzweig, Freud, Jung, and Hall the King-Maker: The Historic Expedition to America (1909) (Seattle: Hogrefe & Huber and St. Louis: Rana House, 1992); Rosenzweig, The Historic Expedition to America (1909): Freud, Jung, and Hall the King-Maker, 2nd edition (St. Louis: Rana House, 1994); Rosenzweig, "Idiodynamics," in Encyclopedia of Psychology, Vol.2, 2nd ed. (New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1994): 205-208.
-
(1951)
Psychological Review
, vol.58
, pp. 213-223
-
-
Rosenzweig, S.1
-
25
-
-
0002013184
-
Freud and Experimental Psychology: The Emergence of Idiodynamics
-
Sigmund Koch and David E. Leary, eds. New York: McGraw-Hill
-
Cf. Saul Rosenzweig, "Idiodynamics in Personality Theory with Special Reference to Projective Methods," Psychological Review 58 (1951): 213-223; Rosenzweig, "Freud and Experimental Psychology: The Emergence of Idiodynamics," in A Century of Psychology as Science, Sigmund Koch and David E. Leary, eds. (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1985); 135-207; Rosenzweig, Freud, Jung, and Hall the King-Maker: The Historic Expedition to America (1909) (Seattle: Hogrefe & Huber and St. Louis: Rana House, 1992); Rosenzweig, The Historic Expedition to America (1909): Freud, Jung, and Hall the King-Maker, 2nd edition (St. Louis: Rana House, 1994); Rosenzweig, "Idiodynamics," in Encyclopedia of Psychology, Vol.2, 2nd ed. (New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1994): 205-208.
-
(1985)
A Century of Psychology As Science
, pp. 135-207
-
-
Rosenzweig1
-
26
-
-
0002013184
-
-
Seattle: Hogrefe & Huber and St. Louis: Rana House
-
Cf. Saul Rosenzweig, "Idiodynamics in Personality Theory with Special Reference to Projective Methods," Psychological Review 58 (1951): 213-223; Rosenzweig, "Freud and Experimental Psychology: The Emergence of Idiodynamics," in A Century of Psychology as Science, Sigmund Koch and David E. Leary, eds. (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1985); 135-207; Rosenzweig, Freud, Jung, and Hall the King-Maker: The Historic Expedition to America (1909) (Seattle: Hogrefe & Huber and St. Louis: Rana House, 1992); Rosenzweig, The Historic Expedition to America (1909): Freud, Jung, and Hall the King-Maker, 2nd edition (St. Louis: Rana House, 1994); Rosenzweig, "Idiodynamics," in Encyclopedia of Psychology, Vol.2, 2nd ed. (New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1994): 205-208.
-
(1992)
Freud, Jung, and Hall the King-Maker: The Historic Expedition to America (1909)
-
-
Rosenzweig1
-
27
-
-
0002013184
-
-
St. Louis: Rana House
-
Cf. Saul Rosenzweig, "Idiodynamics in Personality Theory with Special Reference to Projective Methods," Psychological Review 58 (1951): 213-223; Rosenzweig, "Freud and Experimental Psychology: The Emergence of Idiodynamics," in A Century of Psychology as Science, Sigmund Koch and David E. Leary, eds. (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1985); 135-207; Rosenzweig, Freud, Jung, and Hall the King-Maker: The Historic Expedition to America (1909) (Seattle: Hogrefe & Huber and St. Louis: Rana House, 1992); Rosenzweig, The Historic Expedition to America (1909): Freud, Jung, and Hall the King-Maker, 2nd edition (St. Louis: Rana House, 1994); Rosenzweig, "Idiodynamics," in Encyclopedia of Psychology, Vol.2, 2nd ed. (New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1994): 205-208.
-
(1994)
The Historic Expedition to America (1909): Freud, Jung, and Hall the King-Maker, 2nd Edition
-
-
Rosenzweig1
-
28
-
-
0002013184
-
Idiodynamics
-
New York: John Wiley & Sons
-
Cf. Saul Rosenzweig, "Idiodynamics in Personality Theory with Special Reference to Projective Methods," Psychological Review 58 (1951): 213-223; Rosenzweig, "Freud and Experimental Psychology: The Emergence of Idiodynamics," in A Century of Psychology as Science, Sigmund Koch and David E. Leary, eds. (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1985); 135-207; Rosenzweig, Freud, Jung, and Hall the King-Maker: The Historic Expedition to America (1909) (Seattle: Hogrefe & Huber and St. Louis: Rana House, 1992); Rosenzweig, The Historic Expedition to America (1909): Freud, Jung, and Hall the King-Maker, 2nd edition (St. Louis: Rana House, 1994); Rosenzweig, "Idiodynamics," in Encyclopedia of Psychology, Vol.2, 2nd ed. (New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1994): 205-208.
-
(1994)
Encyclopedia of Psychology, Vol.2, 2nd Ed.
, vol.2
, pp. 205-208
-
-
Rosenzweig1
-
29
-
-
0004017833
-
-
See Francis Galton, Inquiries into Human Faculty, 182-203. For a comprehensive review of the literature on word association and the related "complexes," see Samuel C. Kohs, "The Association Method in its Relation to the Complex and Complex Indicators," American Journal of Psychology 25 (1914): 544-594. This survey begins with Martin Trautscholdt (1883) and continues through Emil Kraepelin (1892), Gustav Aschaffenburg (1896), Max Wertheimer and Julius Klein (1904), and C. G. Jung (1904-1910).
-
Inquiries into Human Faculty
, pp. 182-203
-
-
Galton, F.1
-
30
-
-
0346177520
-
The Association Method in its Relation to the Complex and Complex Indicators
-
See Francis Galton, Inquiries into Human Faculty, 182-203. For a comprehensive review of the literature on word association and the related "complexes," see Samuel C. Kohs, "The Association Method in its Relation to the Complex and Complex Indicators," American Journal of Psychology 25 (1914): 544-594. This survey begins with Martin Trautscholdt (1883) and continues through Emil Kraepelin (1892), Gustav Aschaffenburg (1896), Max Wertheimer and Julius Klein (1904), and C. G. Jung (1904-1910).
-
(1914)
American Journal of Psychology
, vol.25
, pp. 544-594
-
-
Kohs, S.C.1
-
35
-
-
0346808058
-
-
Saul Rosenzweig to Gordon W. Allport 7 October 1958
-
Saul Rosenzweig to Gordon W. Allport 7 October 1958.
-
-
-
-
36
-
-
0348068996
-
-
Washington: The Catholic University of America Press
-
R. M. Martin, Events, Reference, and Logical Form (Washington: The Catholic University of America Press, 1978). A generation earlier, there were the pioneering efforts of the theoretical biologist, J. H. Woodger, Biology and Language: An Introduction to the Methodology of the Biological Sciences Including Medicine (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1952), and J. H. Woodger, Physics, Psychology and Medicine: A Methodological Essay (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1956). In neurology, the methods and discoveries of Wilder Penfield pointed to some of the organic correlates of the registration, retention, and apperceptive recall of individual experiences; see Wilder Penfield, The Excitable Cortex in Conscious Man (Springfield, Illinois: Thomas, 1958), and Wilder Penfield, The Mystery of Mind (Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1975). Related developments in cognitive psychology are exemplified by the innovative investigations in H. F. Crovitz, Galton's Walk: Methods for the Analysis of Thinking, Intelligence, and Creativity (New York: Harper & Rowe, 1970), H. F. Crovitz and H. Schiffman, "Frequency of Episodic Memories as a Function of Their Age," Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 4 (1974): 517-518, and D. C. Rubin, "On the Retention Function for Autobiographical Memory," Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 21 (1982): 21-38, on memory for autobiographical incidents.
-
(1978)
Events, Reference, and Logical Form
-
-
Martin, R.M.1
-
37
-
-
0004247760
-
-
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
-
R. M. Martin, Events, Reference, and Logical Form (Washington: The Catholic University of America Press, 1978). A generation earlier, there were the pioneering efforts of the theoretical biologist, J. H. Woodger, Biology and Language: An Introduction to the Methodology of the Biological Sciences Including Medicine (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1952), and J. H. Woodger, Physics, Psychology and Medicine: A Methodological Essay (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1956). In neurology, the methods and discoveries of Wilder Penfield pointed to some of the organic correlates of the registration, retention, and apperceptive recall of individual experiences; see Wilder Penfield, The Excitable Cortex in Conscious Man (Springfield, Illinois: Thomas, 1958), and Wilder Penfield, The Mystery of Mind (Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1975). Related developments in cognitive psychology are exemplified by the innovative investigations in H. F. Crovitz, Galton's Walk: Methods for the Analysis of Thinking, Intelligence, and Creativity (New York: Harper & Rowe, 1970), H. F. Crovitz and H. Schiffman, "Frequency of Episodic Memories as a Function of Their Age," Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 4 (1974): 517-518, and D. C. Rubin, "On the Retention Function for Autobiographical Memory," Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 21 (1982): 21-38, on memory for autobiographical incidents.
-
(1952)
Biology and Language: An Introduction to the Methodology of the Biological Sciences Including Medicine
-
-
Woodger, J.H.1
-
38
-
-
0039116124
-
-
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
-
R. M. Martin, Events, Reference, and Logical Form (Washington: The Catholic University of America Press, 1978). A generation earlier, there were the pioneering efforts of the theoretical biologist, J. H. Woodger, Biology and Language: An Introduction to the Methodology of the Biological Sciences Including Medicine (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1952), and J. H. Woodger, Physics, Psychology and Medicine: A Methodological Essay (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1956). In neurology, the methods and discoveries of Wilder Penfield pointed to some of the organic correlates of the registration, retention, and apperceptive recall of individual experiences; see Wilder Penfield, The Excitable Cortex in Conscious Man (Springfield, Illinois: Thomas, 1958), and Wilder Penfield, The Mystery of Mind (Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1975). Related developments in cognitive psychology are exemplified by the innovative investigations in H. F. Crovitz, Galton's Walk: Methods for the Analysis of Thinking, Intelligence, and Creativity (New York: Harper & Rowe, 1970), H. F. Crovitz and H. Schiffman, "Frequency of Episodic Memories as a Function of Their Age," Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 4 (1974): 517-518, and D. C. Rubin, "On the Retention Function for Autobiographical Memory," Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 21 (1982): 21-38, on memory for autobiographical incidents.
-
(1956)
Physics, Psychology and Medicine: A Methodological Essay
-
-
Woodger, J.H.1
-
39
-
-
0003594860
-
-
Springfield, Illinois: Thomas
-
R. M. Martin, Events, Reference, and Logical Form (Washington: The Catholic University of America Press, 1978). A generation earlier, there were the pioneering efforts of the theoretical biologist, J. H. Woodger, Biology and Language: An Introduction to the Methodology of the Biological Sciences Including Medicine (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1952), and J. H. Woodger, Physics, Psychology and Medicine: A Methodological Essay (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1956). In neurology, the methods and discoveries of Wilder Penfield pointed to some of the organic correlates of the registration, retention, and apperceptive recall of individual experiences; see Wilder Penfield, The Excitable Cortex in Conscious Man (Springfield, Illinois: Thomas, 1958), and Wilder Penfield, The Mystery of Mind (Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1975). Related developments in cognitive psychology are exemplified by the innovative investigations in H. F. Crovitz, Galton's Walk: Methods for the Analysis of Thinking, Intelligence, and Creativity (New York: Harper & Rowe, 1970), H. F. Crovitz and H. Schiffman, "Frequency of Episodic Memories as a Function of Their Age," Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 4 (1974): 517-518, and D. C. Rubin, "On the Retention Function for Autobiographical Memory," Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 21 (1982): 21-38, on memory for autobiographical incidents.
-
(1958)
The Excitable Cortex in Conscious Man
-
-
Penfield, W.1
-
40
-
-
0003997572
-
-
Princeton: Princeton University Press
-
R. M. Martin, Events, Reference, and Logical Form (Washington: The Catholic University of America Press, 1978). A generation earlier, there were the pioneering efforts of the theoretical biologist, J. H. Woodger, Biology and Language: An Introduction to the Methodology of the Biological Sciences Including Medicine (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1952), and J. H. Woodger, Physics, Psychology and Medicine: A Methodological Essay (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1956). In neurology, the methods and discoveries of Wilder Penfield pointed to some of the organic correlates of the registration, retention, and apperceptive recall of individual experiences; see Wilder Penfield, The Excitable Cortex in Conscious Man (Springfield, Illinois: Thomas, 1958), and Wilder Penfield, The Mystery of Mind (Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1975). Related developments in cognitive psychology are exemplified by the innovative investigations in H. F. Crovitz, Galton's Walk: Methods for the Analysis of Thinking, Intelligence, and Creativity (New York: Harper & Rowe, 1970), H. F. Crovitz and H. Schiffman, "Frequency of Episodic Memories as a Function of Their Age," Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 4 (1974): 517-518, and D. C. Rubin, "On the Retention Function for Autobiographical Memory," Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 21 (1982): 21-38, on memory for autobiographical incidents.
-
(1975)
The Mystery of Mind
-
-
Penfield, W.1
-
41
-
-
0342494072
-
-
New York: Harper & Rowe
-
R. M. Martin, Events, Reference, and Logical Form (Washington: The Catholic University of America Press, 1978). A generation earlier, there were the pioneering efforts of the theoretical biologist, J. H. Woodger, Biology and Language: An Introduction to the Methodology of the Biological Sciences Including Medicine (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1952), and J. H. Woodger, Physics, Psychology and Medicine: A Methodological Essay (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1956). In neurology, the methods and discoveries of Wilder Penfield pointed to some of the organic correlates of the registration, retention, and apperceptive recall of individual experiences; see Wilder Penfield, The Excitable Cortex in Conscious Man (Springfield, Illinois: Thomas, 1958), and Wilder Penfield, The Mystery of Mind (Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1975). Related developments in cognitive psychology are exemplified by the innovative investigations in H. F. Crovitz, Galton's Walk: Methods for the Analysis of Thinking, Intelligence, and Creativity (New York: Harper & Rowe, 1970), H. F. Crovitz and H. Schiffman, "Frequency of Episodic Memories as a Function of Their Age," Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 4 (1974): 517-518, and D. C. Rubin, "On the Retention Function for Autobiographical Memory," Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 21 (1982): 21-38, on memory for autobiographical incidents.
-
(1970)
Galton's Walk: Methods for the Analysis of Thinking, Intelligence, and Creativity
-
-
Crovitz, H.F.1
-
42
-
-
0016335787
-
Frequency of Episodic Memories as a Function of Their Age
-
R. M. Martin, Events, Reference, and Logical Form (Washington: The Catholic University of America Press, 1978). A generation earlier, there were the pioneering efforts of the theoretical biologist, J. H. Woodger, Biology and Language: An Introduction to the Methodology of the Biological Sciences Including Medicine (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1952), and J. H. Woodger, Physics, Psychology and Medicine: A Methodological Essay (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1956). In neurology, the methods and discoveries of Wilder Penfield pointed to some of the organic correlates of the registration, retention, and apperceptive recall of individual experiences; see Wilder Penfield, The Excitable Cortex in Conscious Man (Springfield, Illinois: Thomas, 1958), and Wilder Penfield, The Mystery of Mind (Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1975). Related developments in cognitive psychology are exemplified by the innovative investigations in H. F. Crovitz, Galton's Walk: Methods for the Analysis of Thinking, Intelligence, and Creativity (New York: Harper & Rowe, 1970), H. F. Crovitz and H. Schiffman, "Frequency of Episodic Memories as a Function of Their Age," Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 4 (1974): 517-518, and D. C. Rubin, "On the Retention Function for Autobiographical Memory," Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 21 (1982): 21-38, on memory for autobiographical incidents.
-
(1974)
Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society
, vol.4
, pp. 517-518
-
-
Crovitz, H.F.1
Schiffman, H.2
-
43
-
-
0001836506
-
On the Retention Function for Autobiographical Memory
-
R. M. Martin, Events, Reference, and Logical Form (Washington: The Catholic University of America Press, 1978). A generation earlier, there were the pioneering efforts of the theoretical biologist, J. H. Woodger, Biology and Language: An Introduction to the Methodology of the Biological Sciences Including Medicine (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1952), and J. H. Woodger, Physics, Psychology and Medicine: A Methodological Essay (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1956). In neurology, the methods and discoveries of Wilder Penfield pointed to some of the organic correlates of the registration, retention, and apperceptive recall of individual experiences; see Wilder Penfield, The Excitable Cortex in Conscious Man (Springfield, Illinois: Thomas, 1958), and Wilder Penfield, The Mystery of Mind (Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1975). Related developments in cognitive psychology are exemplified by the innovative investigations in H. F. Crovitz, Galton's Walk: Methods for the Analysis of Thinking, Intelligence, and Creativity (New York: Harper & Rowe, 1970), H. F. Crovitz and H. Schiffman, "Frequency of Episodic Memories as a Function of Their Age," Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 4 (1974): 517-518, and D. C. Rubin, "On the Retention Function for Autobiographical Memory," Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 21 (1982): 21-38, on memory for autobiographical incidents.
-
(1982)
Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior
, vol.21
, pp. 21-38
-
-
Rubin, D.C.1
-
45
-
-
0003572529
-
-
New York: Oxford University Press
-
Thus in Endel Tulving's book, Elements of Episodic Memory (New York: Oxford University Press, 1983): 142-149, the author considers the definition of "Original events" and their basic role in memory as a process.
-
(1983)
Elements of Episodic Memory
, pp. 142-149
-
-
Tulving, E.1
-
46
-
-
0011147750
-
-
Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, chap. 2
-
See Varieties of Memory and Consciousness: Essays Presented in Honor of Endel Tulving, Henry L. Roediger, III and Fergus I. Craik, Eds., (Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1989), chap. 2. The event continues to be of focal interest in Memory Systems 1994, Daniel R. Schacter and Endel Tulving, Eds. (Cambridge: MIT Press, 1994). See especially the section, "Defining Memory Systems," 13f.
-
(1989)
Varieties of Memory and Consciousness: Essays Presented in Honor of Endel Tulving
-
-
Roediger H.L. III1
Craik, F.I.2
-
47
-
-
0004199997
-
-
Cambridge: MIT Press
-
See Varieties of Memory and Consciousness: Essays Presented in Honor of Endel Tulving, Henry L. Roediger, III and Fergus I. Craik, Eds., (Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1989), chap. 2. The event continues to be of focal interest in Memory Systems 1994, Daniel R. Schacter and Endel Tulving, Eds. (Cambridge: MIT Press, 1994). See especially the section, "Defining Memory Systems," 13f.
-
(1994)
Memory Systems 1994
-
-
Schacter, D.R.1
Tulving, E.2
-
48
-
-
25944439747
-
-
See Varieties of Memory and Consciousness: Essays Presented in Honor of Endel Tulving, Henry L. Roediger, III and Fergus I. Craik, Eds., (Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1989), chap. 2. The event continues to be of focal interest in Memory Systems 1994, Daniel R. Schacter and Endel Tulving, Eds. (Cambridge: MIT Press, 1994). See especially the section, "Defining Memory Systems," 13f.
-
Defining Memory Systems
-
-
-
49
-
-
0242640094
-
Norms
-
Reymert, Ed. New York: McGraw-Hill
-
See the following publications by S. Rosenzweig: "Norms" in Feelings and Emotions, Reymert, Ed. (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1950) 327-335; Rosenzweig, "Idiodynamics in Personality Theory." 213-223; "Background to Idiodynamics," The Clinical Psychologist 39 (1986): 83-89; Freud and Experimental Psychology: The Emergence of Idiodynamics, 2nd rev. ed. (Rana House: St. Louis and New York: McGraw-Hill, 1986); Freud, Jung and Hall; and "Idiodynamics," in Encyclopedia of Psychology: 205-208.
-
(1950)
Feelings and Emotions
, pp. 327-335
-
-
Rosenzweig, S.1
-
50
-
-
0347438665
-
-
See the following publications by S. Rosenzweig: "Norms" in Feelings and Emotions, Reymert, Ed. (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1950) 327-335; Rosenzweig, "Idiodynamics in Personality Theory." 213-223; "Background to Idiodynamics," The Clinical Psychologist 39 (1986): 83-89; Freud and Experimental Psychology: The Emergence of Idiodynamics, 2nd rev. ed. (Rana House: St. Louis and New York: McGraw-Hill, 1986); Freud, Jung and Hall; and "Idiodynamics," in Encyclopedia of Psychology: 205-208.
-
Idiodynamics in Personality Theory
, pp. 213-223
-
-
Rosenzweig1
-
51
-
-
0242640098
-
Background to Idiodynamics
-
See the following publications by S. Rosenzweig: "Norms" in Feelings and Emotions, Reymert, Ed. (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1950) 327-335; Rosenzweig, "Idiodynamics in Personality Theory." 213-223; "Background to Idiodynamics," The Clinical Psychologist 39 (1986): 83-89; Freud and Experimental Psychology: The Emergence of Idiodynamics, 2nd rev. ed. (Rana House: St. Louis and New York: McGraw-Hill, 1986); Freud, Jung and Hall; and "Idiodynamics," in Encyclopedia of Psychology: 205-208.
-
(1986)
The Clinical Psychologist
, vol.39
, pp. 83-89
-
-
-
52
-
-
0348068993
-
-
Rana House: St. Louis and New York: McGraw-Hill
-
See the following publications by S. Rosenzweig: "Norms" in Feelings and Emotions, Reymert, Ed. (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1950) 327-335; Rosenzweig, "Idiodynamics in Personality Theory." 213-223; "Background to Idiodynamics," The Clinical Psychologist 39 (1986): 83-89; Freud and Experimental Psychology: The Emergence of Idiodynamics, 2nd rev. ed. (Rana House: St. Louis and New York: McGraw-Hill, 1986); Freud, Jung and Hall; and "Idiodynamics," in Encyclopedia of Psychology: 205-208.
-
(1986)
Freud and Experimental Psychology: The Emergence of Idiodynamics, 2nd Rev. Ed.
-
-
-
53
-
-
0346808066
-
-
See the following publications by S. Rosenzweig: "Norms" in Feelings and Emotions, Reymert, Ed. (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1950) 327-335; Rosenzweig, "Idiodynamics in Personality Theory." 213-223; "Background to Idiodynamics," The Clinical Psychologist 39 (1986): 83-89; Freud and Experimental Psychology: The Emergence of Idiodynamics, 2nd rev. ed. (Rana House: St. Louis and New York: McGraw-Hill, 1986); Freud, Jung and Hall; and "Idiodynamics," in Encyclopedia of Psychology: 205-208.
-
Freud, Jung and Hall
-
-
-
54
-
-
0347438670
-
Idiodynamics
-
See the following publications by S. Rosenzweig: "Norms" in Feelings and Emotions, Reymert, Ed. (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1950) 327-335; Rosenzweig, "Idiodynamics in Personality Theory." 213-223; "Background to Idiodynamics," The Clinical Psychologist 39 (1986): 83-89; Freud and Experimental Psychology: The Emergence of Idiodynamics, 2nd rev. ed. (Rana House: St. Louis and New York: McGraw-Hill, 1986); Freud, Jung and Hall; and "Idiodynamics," in Encyclopedia of Psychology: 205-208.
-
Encyclopedia of Psychology
, pp. 205-208
-
-
-
56
-
-
0346808029
-
-
Allport, Pattern and Growth, 13. Allport's original source (without the parenthetical statements) is Personality in Nature, Society and Culture, Clyde Kluckhohn, Henry A. Murray, and David Scheider, Eds. (New York: Alfred Knopf, 1953): 53.
-
Pattern and Growth
, pp. 13
-
-
Allport1
-
57
-
-
0004110745
-
-
New York: Alfred Knopf
-
Allport, Pattern and Growth, 13. Allport's original source (without the parenthetical statements) is Personality in Nature, Society and Culture, Clyde Kluckhohn, Henry A. Murray, and David Scheider, Eds. (New York: Alfred Knopf, 1953): 53.
-
(1953)
Personality in Nature, Society and Culture
, pp. 53
-
-
Kluckhohn, C.1
Murray, H.A.2
Scheider, D.3
-
60
-
-
0001234253
-
The General and the Unique in Psychological Science
-
G. W. Allport, "The General and the Unique in Psychological Science," Journal of Personality 30 (1962): 405-422.
-
(1962)
Journal of Personality
, vol.30
, pp. 405-422
-
-
Allport, G.W.1
-
62
-
-
0001831108
-
Gordon W. Allport
-
New York: Appleton-Century Crofts
-
"Gordon W. Allport," in A History of Psychology in Autobiography, Edwin G. Boring and Gardner Lindzey, eds. (New York: Appleton-Century Crofts, 1967): vol. 5, 3-25. One could argue that a reference in Gordon W. Allport, The Person in Psychology: Selected Essays (Boston: Beacon Press, 1968), 395, to an "on-moving structure of personality" still smacked of the idiodynamic.
-
(1967)
A History of Psychology in Autobiography
, vol.5
, pp. 3-25
-
-
Boring, E.G.1
Lindzey, G.2
-
63
-
-
0040071031
-
-
Boston: Beacon Press
-
"Gordon W. Allport," in A History of Psychology in Autobiography, Edwin G. Boring and Gardner Lindzey, eds. (New York: Appleton-Century Crofts, 1967): vol. 5, 3-25. One could argue that a reference in Gordon W. Allport, The Person in Psychology: Selected Essays (Boston: Beacon Press, 1968), 395, to an "on-moving structure of personality" still smacked of the idiodynamic.
-
(1968)
The Person in Psychology: Selected Essays
, pp. 395
-
-
Allport, G.W.1
-
64
-
-
0346704970
-
Gordon Allport and 'Letters from Jenny,'
-
Kenneth H. Craik, Robert Hogan, and Raymond N. Wolfe, eds. New York: Plenum Press
-
See, for example, David G. Winter, "Gordon Allport and 'Letters from Jenny,'" in Fifty Years of Personality Psychology, Kenneth H. Craik, Robert Hogan, and Raymond N. Wolfe, eds. (New York: Plenum Press, 1993): 153.
-
(1993)
Fifty Years of Personality Psychology
, pp. 153
-
-
Winter, D.G.1
-
65
-
-
0001898986
-
Traits Revisited
-
G. W. Allport, "Traits Revisited," American Psychologist 21 (1966): 1-10. Reprinted in G. W. Allport, The Person in Psychology: Selected Essays (Boston: Beacon Press, 1968): 43-66.
-
(1966)
American Psychologist
, vol.21
, pp. 1-10
-
-
Allport, G.W.1
-
66
-
-
0040071031
-
-
Boston: Beacon Press
-
G. W. Allport, "Traits Revisited," American Psychologist 21 (1966): 1-10. Reprinted in G. W. Allport, The Person in Psychology: Selected Essays (Boston: Beacon Press, 1968): 43-66.
-
(1968)
The Person in Psychology: Selected Essays
, pp. 43-66
-
-
Allport, G.W.1
-
75
-
-
0346808062
-
-
unpublished manuscript, W. G. T. Douglas, Recorded by and Ed. Boston University School of Theology
-
Allport, "My Encounters with Personality Theory," unpublished manuscript, W. G. T. Douglas, Recorded by and Ed. Boston University School of Theology, 1962. Cited in Alan C. Elms "Allport's Personality and Allport's Personality," in Fifty Years, Craik, Hogan, and Wolfe, eds. 39-55.
-
(1962)
My Encounters with Personality Theory
-
-
Allport1
-
76
-
-
0346074182
-
Allport's Personality and Allport's Personality
-
Craik, Hogan, and Wolfe, eds.
-
Allport, "My Encounters with Personality Theory," unpublished manuscript, W. G. T. Douglas, Recorded by and Ed. Boston University School of Theology, 1962. Cited in Alan C. Elms "Allport's Personality and Allport's Personality," in Fifty Years, Craik, Hogan, and Wolfe, eds. 39-55.
-
Fifty Years
, pp. 39-55
-
-
Elms, A.C.1
-
77
-
-
0346808059
-
-
London: George Allen & Unwin Ltd.
-
Peter A. Bertocci, The Person God Is (London: George Allen & Unwin Ltd., 1970): 67.
-
(1970)
The Person God Is
, pp. 67
-
-
Bertocci, P.A.1
-
80
-
-
0347438666
-
Gordon Willard Allport: 1897-1967
-
Jerome Bruner, "Gordon Willard Allport: 1897-1967," American Journal of Psychology 81 (1968): 279-284.
-
(1968)
American Journal of Psychology
, vol.81
, pp. 279-284
-
-
Bruner, J.1
-
82
-
-
0346704974
-
-
Boston: Published by author, Allport papers, Harvard University Archives
-
G. W. Allport, The Quest of Nellie Wise Allport (Boston: Published by author, 1944). Allport papers, Harvard University Archives.
-
(1944)
The Quest of Nellie Wise Allport
-
-
Allport, G.W.1
-
83
-
-
84956848884
-
Letters from Jenny
-
"Letters from Jenny," (Anon). Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 41 (1946): 315-350; 449-480. A later more complete edition appeared as a separate monograph with G. W. Allport as the acknowledged editor.
-
(1946)
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology
, vol.41
, pp. 315-350
-
-
-
86
-
-
0346177519
-
-
Ibid., 15-16.
-
The Quest
, pp. 15-16
-
-
-
88
-
-
0346074165
-
-
Ph.D. diss., York University, North York, Ontario, Canada
-
Cf. Ian A. M. Nicholson, "Moral Projects and Disciplinary Practices: Gordon Allport and the Development of American Personality Psychology." Ph.D. diss., York University, North York, Ontario, Canada, 1996, 146.
-
(1996)
Moral Projects and Disciplinary Practices: Gordon Allport and the Development of American Personality Psychology
, pp. 146
-
-
Nicholson, I.A.M.1
-
90
-
-
0343814343
-
-
New York: Harper & Row
-
See Richard Cabot, Social Work: Essays on the Meeting-ground of Doctor and Social Worker (Boston: Houghton-Mifflin, 1919); and Richard Cabot, Adventures on the Borderlands of Ethics (New York: Harper & Row, 1926).
-
(1926)
Adventures on the Borderlands of Ethics
-
-
Cabot, R.1
-
91
-
-
0002373350
-
A Psychological Study of Eminent Psychologists and Anthropologists, and a Comparison with Biological and Physical Scientists
-
Anne Roe, "A Psychological Study of Eminent Psychologists and Anthropologists, and a Comparison with Biological and Physical Scientists," Psychological Monographs: General and Applied 67 (1953): 1-55.
-
(1953)
Psychological Monographs: General and Applied
, vol.67
, pp. 1-55
-
-
Roe, A.1
-
92
-
-
0346177487
-
Gordon Allport and His Religion
-
June
-
Ian A. M. Nicholson, "Gordon Allport and His Religion," paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Cheiron Society, June 1994, p. 1. (unpublished, quoted by permission). The quoted letter from Allport to Anne Roe is dated March 29, 1951.
-
(1994)
Annual Meeting of the Cheiron Society
, pp. 1
-
-
Nicholson, I.A.M.1
-
94
-
-
0347438663
-
"the Problem, the Mystery: Some Reflections on Theological Education," (Matriculation Dinner Address)
-
G. W. Allport, "The Problem, the Mystery: Some Reflections on Theological Education," (Matriculation Dinner Address) Bulletin of the Episcopal Theological School 59 (1967): 15-18. Allport's brother, Floyd Allport, did not share Gordon's viewpoint; in his autobiography, "Floyd H. Allport," in A History of Psychology, vol. 6, Lindzey, Ed., 3, Floyd noted that "I must have reacted differently from Gordon to what I felt to be the rather heavy religious influence in our early life. . . . Later when I went to college, we would have long and friendly arguments concerning science and religion." Another comparison (and rivalry) that is not pursued in the present article concerns Gordon Allport's relationship with Henry Murray.
-
(1967)
Bulletin of the Episcopal Theological School
, vol.59
, pp. 15-18
-
-
Allport, G.W.1
-
95
-
-
0347438667
-
Floyd H. Allport
-
G. W. Allport, "The Problem, the Mystery: Some Reflections on Theological Education," (Matriculation Dinner Address) Bulletin of the Episcopal Theological School 59 (1967): 15-18. Allport's brother, Floyd Allport, did not share Gordon's viewpoint; in his autobiography, "Floyd H. Allport," in A History of Psychology, vol. 6, Lindzey, Ed., 3, Floyd noted that "I must have reacted differently from Gordon to what I felt to be the rather heavy religious influence in our early life. . . . Later when I went to college, we would have long and friendly arguments concerning science and religion." Another comparison (and rivalry) that is not pursued in the present article concerns Gordon Allport's relationship with Henry Murray.
-
A History of Psychology
, vol.6
, pp. 3
-
-
Lindzey1
|