메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 83, Issue 10, 1999, Pages 748-753

Whole language and the great plummet of 1987-92: An urban legend from California

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords


EID: 0039700333     PISSN: 00317217     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1177/003172170208301008     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (9)

References (59)
  • 1
    • 0040516757 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Urban Legends Research Centre
    • Urban Legends Research Centre, www.ulrc.com.au.
  • 2
    • 0041110799 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Other urban legends include: Humphrey Bogart was the original Gerber baby in the baby-food ads; the FBI monitors public libraries and notes who is reading "subversive" books; and, my own favorite, if the entire population of China jumped up at the same time, the U.S. would be swamped by a tidal wave. None of these are true.
  • 3
    • 0039331450 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Portsmouth, N.H.: Heinemann, in press
    • The more recent skill-building approaches to literacy have been labeled "balanced" approaches, balancing reading for meaning and skills. However, Gerald Coles, in Reading Unmentionables: Damaging Reading Instruction While Seeming to Fix It (Portsmouth, N.H.: Heinemann, in press), points out that "a close look reveals that the comprehension end of the seesaw remains close to the ground for a long time." The approach is essentially skill-building, with some real reading used as a means of practicing skills. For evidence, see Every Child a Reader (Sacramento: California Department of Education, 1995), the state of California's report of its Reading Task Force, which contains very little mention of real reading but gives birth-to-grave (actually K-8) time lines for phonics instruction and the teaching of other skills.
    • Reading Unmentionables: Damaging Reading Instruction While Seeming to Fix It
    • Coles, G.1
  • 4
    • 0007323762 scopus 로고
    • Sacramento: California Department of Education
    • The more recent skill-building approaches to literacy have been labeled "balanced" approaches, balancing reading for meaning and skills. However, Gerald Coles, in Reading Unmentionables: Damaging Reading Instruction While Seeming to Fix It (Portsmouth, N.H.: Heinemann, in press), points out that "a close look reveals that the comprehension end of the seesaw remains close to the ground for a long time." The approach is essentially skill-building, with some real reading used as a means of practicing skills. For evidence, see Every Child a Reader (Sacramento: California Department of Education, 1995), the state of California's report of its Reading Task Force, which contains very little mention of real reading but gives birth-to-grave (actually K-8) time lines for phonics instruction and the teaching of other skills.
    • (1995) Every Child a Reader
  • 7
    • 0039923838 scopus 로고
    • School library collections and services: Ranking the states
    • Howard D. White, "School Library Collections and Services: Ranking the States," School Library Media Quarterly, vol. 19, 1990, pp. 13-26.
    • (1990) School Library Media Quarterly , vol.19 , pp. 13-26
    • White, H.D.1
  • 8
    • 0039331452 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • For verification of this claim, readers are invited to visit
    • For verification of this claim, readers are invited to visit www.cde. ca.gov/library/libstats.html.
  • 10
    • 0041110798 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • www.cde.ca.gov/library/libstats.html
  • 11
    • 0003652326 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Washington, D.C.: National Center for Education Statistics, Table 422
    • Digest of Educational Statistics (Washington, D.C.: National Center for Education Statistics, 2001 ), Table 422.
    • (2001) Digest of Educational Statistics
  • 14
    • 0039330535 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Access to print in low-income and middle-income communities
    • Poverty has profound effects on literacy development. This is consistent with the claim, discussed below, that access leads to more reading and that more reading leads to better literacy development; children of poverty have far less access to books than more privileged children do (see Susan B. Neuman and Donna Celano, "Access to Print in Low-Income and Middle-Income Communities," Reading Research Quarterly, vol. 36, 2001, pp. 8-26). McQuillan's finding that literacy development is related to access even when proverty is controlled shows that access itself is an important factor in lack of literacy development.
    • (2001) Reading Research Quarterly , vol.36 , pp. 8-26
    • Neuman, S.B.1    Celano, D.2
  • 15
    • 0345896200 scopus 로고
    • Home and school correlates of early interest in literature
    • Leslie Morrow, "Home and School Correlates of Early Interest in Literature," Journal of Educational Research, vol. 76, 1983. pp. 221-30; Susan B. Neuman, "The Home Environment and Fifth-Grade Students' Leisure Reading," Elementary School Journal, vol. 86, 1986, pp. 335-43; and Vincent Creaney and Mary Hagerty, "Correlations of Leisure Time Reading," Journal of Research in Reading, vol. 10, 1987, pp. 3-20.
    • (1983) Journal of Educational Research , vol.76 , pp. 221-230
    • Morrow, L.1
  • 16
    • 0345896200 scopus 로고
    • The home environment and fifth-grade students' leisure reading
    • Leslie Morrow, "Home and School Correlates of Early Interest in Literature," Journal of Educational Research, vol. 76, 1983. pp. 221-30; Susan B. Neuman, "The Home Environment and Fifth-Grade Students' Leisure Reading," Elementary School Journal, vol. 86, 1986, pp. 335-43; and Vincent Creaney and Mary Hagerty, "Correlations of Leisure Time Reading," Journal of Research in Reading, vol. 10, 1987, pp. 3-20.
    • (1986) Elementary School Journal , vol.86 , pp. 335-343
    • Neuman, S.B.1
  • 17
    • 84986771075 scopus 로고
    • Correlations of leisure time reading
    • Leslie Morrow, "Home and School Correlates of Early Interest in Literature," Journal of Educational Research, vol. 76, 1983. pp. 221-30; Susan B. Neuman, "The Home Environment and Fifth-Grade Students' Leisure Reading," Elementary School Journal, vol. 86, 1986, pp. 335-43; and Vincent Creaney and Mary Hagerty, "Correlations of Leisure Time Reading," Journal of Research in Reading, vol. 10, 1987, pp. 3-20.
    • (1987) Journal of Research in Reading , vol.10 , pp. 3-20
    • Creaney, V.1    Hagerty, M.2
  • 19
    • 0039331449 scopus 로고
    • Increasing children's use of literature through program and physical changes
    • Leslie Morrow and Carol Weinstein, "Increasing Children's Use of Literature Through Program and Physical Changes," Elementary School Journal, vol. 83, 1982, pp. 131-37.
    • (1982) Elementary School Journal , vol.83 , pp. 131-137
    • Morrow, L.1    Weinstein, C.2
  • 21
    • 85011227503 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The effect of print access on reading frequency
    • Jeff McQuillan and Julie Au, "The Effect of Print Access on Reading Frequency," Reading Psychology, vol. 22, 2001, pp. 225-48. Of course, simply providing access is not always enough. Sam Pack, in a study of children's after-school activities, identified a group of children he labeled "library latch-key kids," children whose parents used the public library from one to six hours a day as a "free source of after-school care." Pack reported that the children did "little more than 'hang out' at the library." They did not read but passed the entire time socializing with other children and play ing on the computer (Sam Pack, "Public Library Use, School Performance, and the Parental X-Factor: A Bio-Documentary Approach to Children's Snapshots," Reading Improvement, vol. 37, 2000, p. 166). Modest interventions on the part of teachers and librarians, however, can remedy this indifference. Many readers report that one positive experience with reading - a "home run" book experience - was enough to make them dedicated pleasure readers. See Jim Trelease, The Read-Aloud Handbook, 5th ed. (New York: Penguin, 2001). For empirical evidence, see Debra Von Sprecken, Jiyoung Kim, and Stephen Krashen, "The Home Run Book: Can One Positive Reading Experience Create a Reader?," California School Library Journal, vol. 23, no. 2, 2000, pp. 8-9; and Jiyoung Kim and Stephen Krashen, "Another Home Run," California English, vol. 6, no. 2, 2000, p. 25. There are many ways to help ensure that home run experiences will happen, among them, conducting read-alouds (Trelease, op. cit.); modeling reading (see, for example, Kevin Wheldall and Judy Entwhistle, "Back in the USSR: The Effect of Teacher Modeling of Silent Reading on Pupils' Reading Behavior in the Primary School Classroom," Educational Psychology, vol. 8, 1988, pp. 51-56); holding interesting book discussions; and just providing time to read. There is consistent evidence showing that, when students are provided time to read, they will take advantage of it. When observations of sustained silent reading classes are made in the middle of the school year and when students have adequate access to interesting reading material, the vast majority of students are involved in reading during the designated time (see Debra Von Sprecken and Stephen Krashen, "Do Students Read During Sustained Silent Reading?," California Reader, vol. 32, no. 1, 1998, pp. 11-13; Kera Cohen, "Reluctant Eighth-Grade Readers Enjoy Sustained Silent Reading," California Reader, vol. 33, no. 1, 1999, pp. 22-25; and Rene Herda and Francisco Ramos, "How Consistently Do Students Read During Sustained Silent Reading?," California School Library Journal, vol. 24, no. 2, 2001, pp. 29-31).
    • (2001) Reading Psychology , vol.22 , pp. 225-248
    • McQuillan, J.1    Julie, A.2
  • 22
    • 0039331443 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Public library use, school performance, and the parental X-factor: A bio-documentary approach to children's snapshots
    • Jeff McQuillan and Julie Au, "The Effect of Print Access on Reading Frequency," Reading Psychology, vol. 22, 2001, pp. 225-48. Of course, simply providing access is not always enough. Sam Pack, in a study of children's after-school activities, identified a group of children he labeled "library latch-key kids," children whose parents used the public library from one to six hours a day as a "free source of after-school care." Pack reported that the children did "little more than 'hang out' at the library." They did not read but passed the entire time socializing with other children and play ing on the computer (Sam Pack, "Public Library Use, School Performance, and the Parental X-Factor: A Bio-Documentary Approach to Children's Snapshots," Reading Improvement, vol. 37, 2000, p. 166). Modest interventions on the part of teachers and librarians, however, can remedy this indifference. Many readers report that one positive experience with reading - a "home run" book experience - was enough to make them dedicated pleasure readers. See Jim Trelease, The Read-Aloud Handbook, 5th ed. (New York: Penguin, 2001). For empirical evidence, see Debra Von Sprecken, Jiyoung Kim, and Stephen Krashen, "The Home Run Book: Can One Positive Reading Experience Create a Reader?," California School Library Journal, vol. 23, no. 2, 2000, pp. 8-9; and Jiyoung Kim and Stephen Krashen, "Another Home Run," California English, vol. 6, no. 2, 2000, p. 25. There are many ways to help ensure that home run experiences will happen, among them, conducting read-alouds (Trelease, op. cit.); modeling reading (see, for example, Kevin Wheldall and Judy Entwhistle, "Back in the USSR: The Effect of Teacher Modeling of Silent Reading on Pupils' Reading Behavior in the Primary School Classroom," Educational Psychology, vol. 8, 1988, pp. 51-56); holding interesting book discussions; and just providing time to read. There is consistent evidence showing that, when students are provided time to read, they will take advantage of it. When observations of sustained silent reading classes are made in the middle of the school year and when students have adequate access to interesting reading material, the vast majority of students are involved in reading during the designated time (see Debra Von Sprecken and Stephen Krashen, "Do Students Read During Sustained Silent Reading?," California Reader, vol. 32, no. 1, 1998, pp. 11-13; Kera Cohen, "Reluctant Eighth-Grade Readers Enjoy Sustained Silent Reading," California Reader, vol. 33, no. 1, 1999, pp. 22-25; and Rene Herda and Francisco Ramos, "How Consistently Do Students Read During Sustained Silent Reading?," California School Library Journal, vol. 24, no. 2, 2001, pp. 29-31).
    • (2000) Reading Improvement , vol.37 , pp. 166
    • Pack, S.1
  • 23
    • 0010118525 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • New York: Penguin
    • Jeff McQuillan and Julie Au, "The Effect of Print Access on Reading Frequency," Reading Psychology, vol. 22, 2001, pp. 225-48. Of course, simply providing access is not always enough. Sam Pack, in a study of children's after-school activities, identified a group of children he labeled "library latch-key kids," children whose parents used the public library from one to six hours a day as a "free source of after-school care." Pack reported that the children did "little more than 'hang out' at the library." They did not read but passed the entire time socializing with other children and play ing on the computer (Sam Pack, "Public Library Use, School Performance, and the Parental X-Factor: A Bio-Documentary Approach to Children's Snapshots," Reading Improvement, vol. 37, 2000, p. 166). Modest interventions on the part of teachers and librarians, however, can remedy this indifference. Many readers report that one positive experience with reading - a "home run" book experience - was enough to make them dedicated pleasure readers. See Jim Trelease, The Read-Aloud Handbook, 5th ed. (New York: Penguin, 2001). For empirical evidence, see Debra Von Sprecken, Jiyoung Kim, and Stephen Krashen, "The Home Run Book: Can One Positive Reading Experience Create a Reader?," California School Library Journal, vol. 23, no. 2, 2000, pp. 8-9; and Jiyoung Kim and Stephen Krashen, "Another Home Run," California English, vol. 6, no. 2, 2000, p. 25. There are many ways to help ensure that home run experiences will happen, among them, conducting read-alouds (Trelease, op. cit.); modeling reading (see, for example, Kevin Wheldall and Judy Entwhistle, "Back in the USSR: The Effect of Teacher Modeling of Silent Reading on Pupils' Reading Behavior in the Primary School Classroom," Educational Psychology, vol. 8, 1988, pp. 51-56); holding interesting book discussions; and just providing time to read. There is consistent evidence showing that, when students are provided time to read, they will take advantage of it. When observations of sustained silent reading classes are made in the middle of the school year and when students have adequate access to interesting reading material, the vast majority of students are involved in reading during the designated time (see Debra Von Sprecken and Stephen Krashen, "Do Students Read During Sustained Silent Reading?," California Reader, vol. 32, no. 1, 1998, pp. 11-13; Kera Cohen, "Reluctant Eighth-Grade Readers Enjoy Sustained Silent Reading," California Reader, vol. 33, no. 1, 1999, pp. 22-25; and Rene Herda and Francisco Ramos, "How Consistently Do Students Read During Sustained Silent Reading?," California School Library Journal, vol. 24, no. 2, 2001, pp. 29-31).
    • (2001) The Read-Aloud Handbook, 5th Ed.
    • Trelease, J.1
  • 24
    • 0041110793 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The home run book: Can one positive reading experience create a reader?
    • Jeff McQuillan and Julie Au, "The Effect of Print Access on Reading Frequency," Reading Psychology, vol. 22, 2001, pp. 225-48. Of course, simply providing access is not always enough. Sam Pack, in a study of children's after-school activities, identified a group of children he labeled "library latch-key kids," children whose parents used the public library from one to six hours a day as a "free source of after-school care." Pack reported that the children did "little more than 'hang out' at the library." They did not read but passed the entire time socializing with other children and play ing on the computer (Sam Pack, "Public Library Use, School Performance, and the Parental X-Factor: A Bio-Documentary Approach to Children's Snapshots," Reading Improvement, vol. 37, 2000, p. 166). Modest interventions on the part of teachers and librarians, however, can remedy this indifference. Many readers report that one positive experience with reading - a "home run" book experience - was enough to make them dedicated pleasure readers. See Jim Trelease, The Read-Aloud Handbook, 5th ed. (New York: Penguin, 2001). For empirical evidence, see Debra Von Sprecken, Jiyoung Kim, and Stephen Krashen, "The Home Run Book: Can One Positive Reading Experience Create a Reader?," California School Library Journal, vol. 23, no. 2, 2000, pp. 8-9; and Jiyoung Kim and Stephen Krashen, "Another Home Run," California English, vol. 6, no. 2, 2000, p. 25. There are many ways to help ensure that home run experiences will happen, among them, conducting read-alouds (Trelease, op. cit.); modeling reading (see, for example, Kevin Wheldall and Judy Entwhistle, "Back in the USSR: The Effect of Teacher Modeling of Silent Reading on Pupils' Reading Behavior in the Primary School Classroom," Educational Psychology, vol. 8, 1988, pp. 51-56); holding interesting book discussions; and just providing time to read. There is consistent evidence showing that, when students are provided time to read, they will take advantage of it. When observations of sustained silent reading classes are made in the middle of the school year and when students have adequate access to interesting reading material, the vast majority of students are involved in reading during the designated time (see Debra Von Sprecken and Stephen Krashen, "Do Students Read During Sustained Silent Reading?," California Reader, vol. 32, no. 1, 1998, pp. 11-13; Kera Cohen, "Reluctant Eighth-Grade Readers Enjoy Sustained Silent Reading," California Reader, vol. 33, no. 1, 1999, pp. 22-25; and Rene Herda and Francisco Ramos, "How Consistently Do Students Read During Sustained Silent Reading?," California School Library Journal, vol. 24, no. 2, 2001, pp. 29-31).
    • (2000) California School Library Journal , vol.23 , Issue.2 , pp. 8-9
    • Von Sprecken, D.1    Kim, J.2    Krashen, S.3
  • 25
    • 0039331445 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Another home run
    • Jeff McQuillan and Julie Au, "The Effect of Print Access on Reading Frequency," Reading Psychology, vol. 22, 2001, pp. 225-48. Of course, simply providing access is not always enough. Sam Pack, in a study of children's after-school activities, identified a group of children he labeled "library latch-key kids," children whose parents used the public library from one to six hours a day as a "free source of after-school care." Pack reported that the children did "little more than 'hang out' at the library." They did not read but passed the entire time socializing with other children and play ing on the computer (Sam Pack, "Public Library Use, School Performance, and the Parental X-Factor: A Bio-Documentary Approach to Children's Snapshots," Reading Improvement, vol. 37, 2000, p. 166). Modest interventions on the part of teachers and librarians, however, can remedy this indifference. Many readers report that one positive experience with reading - a "home run" book experience - was enough to make them dedicated pleasure readers. See Jim Trelease, The Read-Aloud Handbook, 5th ed. (New York: Penguin, 2001). For empirical evidence, see Debra Von Sprecken, Jiyoung Kim, and Stephen Krashen, "The Home Run Book: Can One Positive Reading Experience Create a Reader?," California School Library Journal, vol. 23, no. 2, 2000, pp. 8-9; and Jiyoung Kim and Stephen Krashen, "Another Home Run," California English, vol. 6, no. 2, 2000, p. 25. There are many ways to help ensure that home run experiences will happen, among them, conducting read-alouds (Trelease, op. cit.); modeling reading (see, for example, Kevin Wheldall and Judy Entwhistle, "Back in the USSR: The Effect of Teacher Modeling of Silent Reading on Pupils' Reading Behavior in the Primary School Classroom," Educational Psychology, vol. 8, 1988, pp. 51-56); holding interesting book discussions; and just providing time to read. There is consistent evidence showing that, when students are provided time to read, they will take advantage of it. When observations of sustained silent reading classes are made in the middle of the school year and when students have adequate access to interesting reading material, the vast majority of students are involved in reading during the designated time (see Debra Von Sprecken and Stephen Krashen, "Do Students Read During Sustained Silent Reading?," California Reader, vol. 32, no. 1, 1998, pp. 11-13; Kera Cohen, "Reluctant Eighth-Grade Readers Enjoy Sustained Silent Reading," California Reader, vol. 33, no. 1, 1999, pp. 22-25; and Rene Herda and Francisco Ramos, "How Consistently Do Students Read During Sustained Silent Reading?," California School Library Journal, vol. 24, no. 2, 2001, pp. 29-31).
    • (2000) California English , vol.6 , Issue.2 , pp. 25
    • Kim, J.1    Krashen, S.2
  • 26
    • 0039331445 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Another home run
    • Jeff McQuillan and Julie Au, "The Effect of Print Access on Reading Frequency," Reading Psychology, vol. 22, 2001, pp. 225-48. Of course, simply providing access is not always enough. Sam Pack, in a study of children's after-school activities, identified a group of children he labeled "library latch-key kids," children whose parents used the public library from one to six hours a day as a "free source of after-school care." Pack reported that the children did "little more than 'hang out' at the library." They did not read but passed the entire time socializing with other children and play ing on the computer (Sam Pack, "Public Library Use, School Performance, and the Parental X-Factor: A Bio-Documentary Approach to Children's Snapshots," Reading Improvement, vol. 37, 2000, p. 166). Modest interventions on the part of teachers and librarians, however, can remedy this indifference. Many readers report that one positive experience with reading - a "home run" book experience - was enough to make them dedicated pleasure readers. See Jim Trelease, The Read-Aloud Handbook, 5th ed. (New York: Penguin, 2001). For empirical evidence, see Debra Von Sprecken, Jiyoung Kim, and Stephen Krashen, "The Home Run Book: Can One Positive Reading Experience Create a Reader?," California School Library Journal, vol. 23, no. 2, 2000, pp. 8-9; and Jiyoung Kim and Stephen Krashen, "Another Home Run," California English, vol. 6, no. 2, 2000, p. 25. There are many ways to help ensure that home run experiences will happen, among them, conducting read-alouds (Trelease, op. cit.); modeling reading (see, for example, Kevin Wheldall and Judy Entwhistle, "Back in the USSR: The Effect of Teacher Modeling of Silent Reading on Pupils' Reading Behavior in the Primary School Classroom," Educational Psychology, vol. 8, 1988, pp. 51-56); holding interesting book discussions; and just providing time to read. There is consistent evidence showing that, when students are provided time to read, they will take advantage of it. When observations of sustained silent reading classes are made in the middle of the school year and when students have adequate access to interesting reading material, the vast majority of students are involved in reading during the designated time (see Debra Von Sprecken and Stephen Krashen, "Do Students Read During Sustained Silent Reading?," California Reader, vol. 32, no. 1, 1998, pp. 11-13; Kera Cohen, "Reluctant Eighth-Grade Readers Enjoy Sustained Silent Reading," California Reader, vol. 33, no. 1, 1999, pp. 22-25; and Rene Herda and Francisco Ramos, "How Consistently Do Students Read During Sustained Silent Reading?," California School Library Journal, vol. 24, no. 2, 2001, pp. 29-31).
    • (2000) California English , vol.6 , Issue.2 , pp. 25
    • Trelease1
  • 27
    • 0040516750 scopus 로고
    • Back in the USSR: The effect of teacher modeling of silent reading on pupils' reading behavior in the primary school classroom
    • Jeff McQuillan and Julie Au, "The Effect of Print Access on Reading Frequency," Reading Psychology, vol. 22, 2001, pp. 225-48. Of course, simply providing access is not always enough. Sam Pack, in a study of children's after-school activities, identified a group of children he labeled "library latch-key kids," children whose parents used the public library from one to six hours a day as a "free source of after-school care." Pack reported that the children did "little more than 'hang out' at the library." They did not read but passed the entire time socializing with other children and play ing on the computer (Sam Pack, "Public Library Use, School Performance, and the Parental X-Factor: A Bio-Documentary Approach to Children's Snapshots," Reading Improvement, vol. 37, 2000, p. 166). Modest interventions on the part of teachers and librarians, however, can remedy this indifference. Many readers report that one positive experience with reading - a "home run" book experience - was enough to make them dedicated pleasure readers. See Jim Trelease, The Read-Aloud Handbook, 5th ed. (New York: Penguin, 2001). For empirical evidence, see Debra Von Sprecken, Jiyoung Kim, and Stephen Krashen, "The Home Run Book: Can One Positive Reading Experience Create a Reader?," California School Library Journal, vol. 23, no. 2, 2000, pp. 8-9; and Jiyoung Kim and Stephen Krashen, "Another Home Run," California English, vol. 6, no. 2, 2000, p. 25. There are many ways to help ensure that home run experiences will happen, among them, conducting read-alouds (Trelease, op. cit.); modeling reading (see, for example, Kevin Wheldall and Judy Entwhistle, "Back in the USSR: The Effect of Teacher Modeling of Silent Reading on Pupils' Reading Behavior in the Primary School Classroom," Educational Psychology, vol. 8, 1988, pp. 51-56); holding interesting book discussions; and just providing time to read. There is consistent evidence showing that, when students are provided time to read, they will take advantage of it. When observations of sustained silent reading classes are made in the middle of the school year and when students have adequate access to interesting reading material, the vast majority of students are involved in reading during the designated time (see Debra Von Sprecken and Stephen Krashen, "Do Students Read During Sustained Silent Reading?," California Reader, vol. 32, no. 1, 1998, pp. 11-13; Kera Cohen, "Reluctant Eighth-Grade Readers Enjoy Sustained Silent Reading," California Reader, vol. 33, no. 1, 1999, pp. 22-25; and Rene Herda and Francisco Ramos, "How Consistently Do Students Read During Sustained Silent Reading?," California School Library Journal, vol. 24, no. 2, 2001, pp. 29-31).
    • (1988) Educational Psychology , vol.8 , pp. 51-56
    • Wheldall, K.1    Entwhistle, J.2
  • 28
    • 0041110784 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Do students read during sustained silent reading?
    • Jeff McQuillan and Julie Au, "The Effect of Print Access on Reading Frequency," Reading Psychology, vol. 22, 2001, pp. 225-48. Of course, simply providing access is not always enough. Sam Pack, in a study of children's after-school activities, identified a group of children he labeled "library latch-key kids," children whose parents used the public library from one to six hours a day as a "free source of after-school care." Pack reported that the children did "little more than 'hang out' at the library." They did not read but passed the entire time socializing with other children and play ing on the computer (Sam Pack, "Public Library Use, School Performance, and the Parental X-Factor: A Bio-Documentary Approach to Children's Snapshots," Reading Improvement, vol. 37, 2000, p. 166). Modest interventions on the part of teachers and librarians, however, can remedy this indifference. Many readers report that one positive experience with reading - a "home run" book experience - was enough to make them dedicated pleasure readers. See Jim Trelease, The Read-Aloud Handbook, 5th ed. (New York: Penguin, 2001). For empirical evidence, see Debra Von Sprecken, Jiyoung Kim, and Stephen Krashen, "The Home Run Book: Can One Positive Reading Experience Create a Reader?," California School Library Journal, vol. 23, no. 2, 2000, pp. 8-9; and Jiyoung Kim and Stephen Krashen, "Another Home Run," California English, vol. 6, no. 2, 2000, p. 25. There are many ways to help ensure that home run experiences will happen, among them, conducting read-alouds (Trelease, op. cit.); modeling reading (see, for example, Kevin Wheldall and Judy Entwhistle, "Back in the USSR: The Effect of Teacher Modeling of Silent Reading on Pupils' Reading Behavior in the Primary School Classroom," Educational Psychology, vol. 8, 1988, pp. 51-56); holding interesting book discussions; and just providing time to read. There is consistent evidence showing that, when students are provided time to read, they will take advantage of it. When observations of sustained silent reading classes are made in the middle of the school year and when students have adequate access to interesting reading material, the vast majority of students are involved in reading during the designated time (see Debra Von Sprecken and Stephen Krashen, "Do Students Read During Sustained Silent Reading?," California Reader, vol. 32, no. 1, 1998, pp. 11-13; Kera Cohen, "Reluctant Eighth-Grade Readers Enjoy Sustained Silent Reading," California Reader, vol. 33, no. 1, 1999, pp. 22-25; and Rene Herda and Francisco Ramos, "How Consistently Do Students Read During Sustained Silent Reading?," California School Library Journal, vol. 24, no. 2, 2001, pp. 29-31).
    • (1998) California Reader , vol.32 , Issue.1 , pp. 11-13
    • Von Sprecken, D.1    Krashen, S.2
  • 29
    • 0039923835 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Reluctant eighth-grade readers enjoy sustained silent reading
    • Jeff McQuillan and Julie Au, "The Effect of Print Access on Reading Frequency," Reading Psychology, vol. 22, 2001, pp. 225-48. Of course, simply providing access is not always enough. Sam Pack, in a study of children's after-school activities, identified a group of children he labeled "library latch-key kids," children whose parents used the public library from one to six hours a day as a "free source of after-school care." Pack reported that the children did "little more than 'hang out' at the library." They did not read but passed the entire time socializing with other children and play ing on the computer (Sam Pack, "Public Library Use, School Performance, and the Parental X-Factor: A Bio-Documentary Approach to Children's Snapshots," Reading Improvement, vol. 37, 2000, p. 166). Modest interventions on the part of teachers and librarians, however, can remedy this indifference. Many readers report that one positive experience with reading - a "home run" book experience - was enough to make them dedicated pleasure readers. See Jim Trelease, The Read-Aloud Handbook, 5th ed. (New York: Penguin, 2001). For empirical evidence, see Debra Von Sprecken, Jiyoung Kim, and Stephen Krashen, "The Home Run Book: Can One Positive Reading Experience Create a Reader?," California School Library Journal, vol. 23, no. 2, 2000, pp. 8-9; and Jiyoung Kim and Stephen Krashen, "Another Home Run," California English, vol. 6, no. 2, 2000, p. 25. There are many ways to help ensure that home run experiences will happen, among them, conducting read-alouds (Trelease, op. cit.); modeling reading (see, for example, Kevin Wheldall and Judy Entwhistle, "Back in the USSR: The Effect of Teacher Modeling of Silent Reading on Pupils' Reading Behavior in the Primary School Classroom," Educational Psychology, vol. 8, 1988, pp. 51-56); holding interesting book discussions; and just providing time to read. There is consistent evidence showing that, when students are provided time to read, they will take advantage of it. When observations of sustained silent reading classes are made in the middle of the school year and when students have adequate access to interesting reading material, the vast majority of students are involved in reading during the designated time (see Debra Von Sprecken and Stephen Krashen, "Do Students Read During Sustained Silent Reading?," California Reader, vol. 32, no. 1, 1998, pp. 11-13; Kera Cohen, "Reluctant Eighth-Grade Readers Enjoy Sustained Silent Reading," California Reader, vol. 33, no. 1, 1999, pp. 22-25; and Rene Herda and Francisco Ramos, "How Consistently Do Students Read During Sustained Silent Reading?," California School Library Journal, vol. 24, no. 2, 2001, pp. 29-31).
    • (1999) California Reader , vol.33 , Issue.1 , pp. 22-25
    • Cohen, K.1
  • 30
    • 0039331438 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • How consistently do students read during sustained silent reading?
    • Jeff McQuillan and Julie Au, "The Effect of Print Access on Reading Frequency," Reading Psychology, vol. 22, 2001, pp. 225-48. Of course, simply providing access is not always enough. Sam Pack, in a study of children's after-school activities, identified a group of children he labeled "library latch-key kids," children whose parents used the public library from one to six hours a day as a "free source of after-school care." Pack reported that the children did "little more than 'hang out' at the library." They did not read but passed the entire time socializing with other children and play ing on the computer (Sam Pack, "Public Library Use, School Performance, and the Parental X-Factor: A Bio-Documentary Approach to Children's Snapshots," Reading Improvement, vol. 37, 2000, p. 166). Modest interventions on the part of teachers and librarians, however, can remedy this indifference. Many readers report that one positive experience with reading - a "home run" book experience - was enough to make them dedicated pleasure readers. See Jim Trelease, The Read-Aloud Handbook, 5th ed. (New York: Penguin, 2001). For empirical evidence, see Debra Von Sprecken, Jiyoung Kim, and Stephen Krashen, "The Home Run Book: Can One Positive Reading Experience Create a Reader?," California School Library Journal, vol. 23, no. 2, 2000, pp. 8-9; and Jiyoung Kim and Stephen Krashen, "Another Home Run," California English, vol. 6, no. 2, 2000, p. 25. There are many ways to help ensure that home run experiences will happen, among them, conducting read-alouds (Trelease, op. cit.); modeling reading (see, for example, Kevin Wheldall and Judy Entwhistle, "Back in the USSR: The Effect of Teacher Modeling of Silent Reading on Pupils' Reading Behavior in the Primary School Classroom," Educational Psychology, vol. 8, 1988, pp. 51-56); holding interesting book discussions; and just providing time to read. There is consistent evidence showing that, when students are provided time to read, they will take advantage of it. When observations of sustained silent reading classes are made in the middle of the school year and when students have adequate access to interesting reading material, the vast majority of students are involved in reading during the designated time (see Debra Von Sprecken and Stephen Krashen, "Do Students Read During Sustained Silent Reading?," California Reader, vol. 32, no. 1, 1998, pp. 11-13; Kera Cohen, "Reluctant Eighth-Grade Readers Enjoy Sustained Silent Reading," California Reader, vol. 33, no. 1, 1999, pp. 22-25; and Rene Herda and Francisco Ramos, "How Consistently Do Students Read During Sustained Silent Reading?," California School Library Journal, vol. 24, no. 2, 2001, pp. 29-31).
    • (2001) California School Library Journal , vol.24 , Issue.2 , pp. 29-31
    • Herda, R.1    Ramos, F.2
  • 32
    • 0003723671 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Englewood, Colo.: Libraries Unlimited
    • These cases are reviewed in Stephen Krashen, The Power of Reading (Englewood, Colo.: Libraries Unlimited, 1993).
    • (1993) The Power of Reading
    • Krashen, S.1
  • 33
    • 0011664844 scopus 로고
    • Do we learn to read by reading? the relationship between free reading and reading ability
    • Deborah Tannen, ed., Norwood, N.J.: Ablex
    • Stephen Krashen, "Do We Learn to Read by Reading? The Relationship Between Free Reading and Reading Ability," in Deborah Tannen, ed., Linguistics in Context: Connecting Observation and Understanding (Norwood, N.J.: Ablex, 1988), pp. 269-98.
    • (1988) Linguistics in Context: Connecting Observation and Understanding , pp. 269-298
    • Krashen, S.1
  • 35
    • 0347487388 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • More smoke and mirrors: A critique of the national reading panel report on fluency
    • October
    • Stephen Krashen, "More Smoke and Mirrors: A Critique of the National Reading Panel Report on Fluency," Phi Delta Kappan, October 2001, pp. 119-23.
    • (2001) Phi Delta Kappan , pp. 119-123
    • Krashen, S.1
  • 38
    • 0041110792 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The national reading panel comparison of whole language and phonics: Ignoring the crucial variable in reading
    • in press
    • Stephen Krashen, "The National Reading Panel Comparison of Whole Language and Phonics: Ignoring the Crucial Variable in Reading," Talking Points, in press.
    • Talking Points
    • Krashen, S.1
  • 40
    • 0041192602 scopus 로고
    • School libraries, public libraries, and the NAEP reading scores
    • Stephen Krashen, "School Libraries, Public Libraries, and the NAEP Reading Scores," School Library Media Quarterly, vol. 23, 1995, pp. 235-38; and McQuillan, op. cit.
    • (1995) School Library Media Quarterly , vol.23 , pp. 235-238
    • Krashen, S.1
  • 41
    • 0041192602 scopus 로고
    • School libraries, public libraries, and the NAEP reading scores
    • Stephen Krashen, "School Libraries, Public Libraries, and the NAEP Reading Scores," School Library Media Quarterly, vol. 23, 1995, pp. 235-38; and McQuillan, op. cit.
    • (1995) School Library Media Quarterly , vol.23 , pp. 235-238
    • McQuillan1
  • 42
    • 0003839453 scopus 로고
    • Hamburg: International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement
    • Warwick Elley, How in the World Do Children Read? (Hamburg: International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement, 1992).
    • (1992) How in the World Do Children Read?
    • Elley, W.1
  • 43
    • 0038715599 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • San Jose, Calif.: Hi Willow Research and Publishing
    • Replication studies by Lance and his colleagues include a second Colorado study (Keith Curry Lance, Marcia Rodney, and Christine Hamilton-Pennell, How School Librarians Help Kids Achieve Standards: The Second Colorado Study [San Jose, Calif.: Hi Willow Research and Publishing, 2000]); and replications in Alaska (Keith Curry Lance et al., Information Empowered: The School Librarian as an Agent of Academic Achievement in Alaska Schools [Juneau: Alaska State Library, 1999]), Oregon (Keith Curry Lance, Marcia Rodney, and Christine Hamilton-Pennell, Good Schools Have Good Librarians [Terrebone: Oregon Educational Media Association, 2001]), and Pennsylvania (Keith Curry Lance et al., Measuring to Standards: The Impact of School Library Programs and Information Literacy in Pennsylvania Schools [Greensburg: Pennsylvania Citizens for Better Libraries, 2000]). Similar studies have been done in Texas (Ester Smith, Texas School Libraries: Standards, Resources, Services, and Students' Performance, Texas State Library and Archives Commission, 2001, available at www.tsl.state.tx.us/ld/pubs/schlibsurvey/ index.html): Massachusetts (James Baughman, School Libraries and MCAS Scores, 2000, available at http://web.simmons. edu/∼baughman/mcas-school-libraries/); and Indiana (A Study of the Differences Between Higher- and Lower-Performing Indiana Schools in Reading and Mathematics [Oak Brook, III.: North Central Regional Educational Laboratory, 2000]). In all these studies, poverty was a predictor of achievement. The number of books per student and the level of library staffing were also consistent predictors of achievement. In most studies, these relationships held even when poverty was controlled (the Colorado replication and the Oregon, Indiana, Massachusetts, and Texas studies); in the Alaska and Pennsylvania studies, only staffing predicted reading scores, and in the Alaska study this was the case only when poverty was not controlled. The Colorado replication and the Pennsylvania, Texas, and Oregon studies used tests of reading comprehension only. In Alaska and Indiana, math was included, and in Massachusetts, the measure included language arts, math, and science.
    • (2000) How School Librarians Help Kids Achieve Standards: The Second Colorado Study
    • Lance, K.C.1    Rodney, M.2    Hamilton-Pennell, C.3
  • 44
    • 0038715563 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Juneau: Alaska State Library
    • Replication studies by Lance and his colleagues include a second Colorado study (Keith Curry Lance, Marcia Rodney, and Christine Hamilton-Pennell, How School Librarians Help Kids Achieve Standards: The Second Colorado Study [San Jose, Calif.: Hi Willow Research and Publishing, 2000]); and replications in Alaska (Keith Curry Lance et al., Information Empowered: The School Librarian as an Agent of Academic Achievement in Alaska Schools [Juneau: Alaska State Library, 1999]), Oregon (Keith Curry Lance, Marcia Rodney, and Christine Hamilton-Pennell, Good Schools Have Good Librarians [Terrebone: Oregon Educational Media Association, 2001]), and Pennsylvania (Keith Curry Lance et al., Measuring to Standards: The Impact of School Library Programs and Information Literacy in Pennsylvania Schools [Greensburg: Pennsylvania Citizens for Better Libraries, 2000]). Similar studies have been done in Texas (Ester Smith, Texas School Libraries: Standards, Resources, Services, and Students' Performance, Texas State Library and Archives Commission, 2001, available at www.tsl.state.tx.us/ld/pubs/schlibsurvey/ index.html): Massachusetts (James Baughman, School Libraries and MCAS Scores, 2000, available at http://web.simmons. edu/∼baughman/mcas-school-libraries/); and Indiana (A Study of the Differences Between Higher- and Lower-Performing Indiana Schools in Reading and Mathematics [Oak Brook, III.: North Central Regional Educational Laboratory, 2000]). In all these studies, poverty was a predictor of achievement. The number of books per student and the level of library staffing were also consistent predictors of achievement. In most studies, these relationships held even when poverty was controlled (the Colorado replication and the Oregon, Indiana, Massachusetts, and Texas studies); in the Alaska and Pennsylvania studies, only staffing predicted reading scores, and in the Alaska study this was the case only when poverty was not controlled. The Colorado replication and the Pennsylvania, Texas, and Oregon studies used tests of reading comprehension only. In Alaska and Indiana, math was included, and in Massachusetts, the measure included language arts, math, and science.
    • (1999) Information Empowered: The School Librarian as an Agent of Academic Achievement in Alaska Schools
    • Lance, K.C.1
  • 45
    • 23244437304 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Terrebone: Oregon Educational Media Association
    • Replication studies by Lance and his colleagues include a second Colorado study (Keith Curry Lance, Marcia Rodney, and Christine Hamilton-Pennell, How School Librarians Help Kids Achieve Standards: The Second Colorado Study [San Jose, Calif.: Hi Willow Research and Publishing, 2000]); and replications in Alaska (Keith Curry Lance et al., Information Empowered: The School Librarian as an Agent of Academic Achievement in Alaska Schools [Juneau: Alaska State Library, 1999]), Oregon (Keith Curry Lance, Marcia Rodney, and Christine Hamilton-Pennell, Good Schools Have Good Librarians [Terrebone: Oregon Educational Media Association, 2001]), and Pennsylvania (Keith Curry Lance et al., Measuring to Standards: The Impact of School Library Programs and Information Literacy in Pennsylvania Schools [Greensburg: Pennsylvania Citizens for Better Libraries, 2000]). Similar studies have been done in Texas (Ester Smith, Texas School Libraries: Standards, Resources, Services, and Students' Performance, Texas State Library and Archives Commission, 2001, available at www.tsl.state.tx.us/ld/pubs/schlibsurvey/ index.html): Massachusetts (James Baughman, School Libraries and MCAS Scores, 2000, available at http://web.simmons. edu/∼baughman/mcas-school-libraries/); and Indiana (A Study of the Differences Between Higher- and Lower-Performing Indiana Schools in Reading and Mathematics [Oak Brook, III.: North Central Regional Educational Laboratory, 2000]). In all these studies, poverty was a predictor of achievement. The number of books per student and the level of library staffing were also consistent predictors of achievement. In most studies, these relationships held even when poverty was controlled (the Colorado replication and the Oregon, Indiana, Massachusetts, and Texas studies); in the Alaska and Pennsylvania studies, only staffing predicted reading scores, and in the Alaska study this was the case only when poverty was not controlled. The Colorado replication and the Pennsylvania, Texas, and Oregon studies used tests of reading comprehension only. In Alaska and Indiana, math was included, and in Massachusetts, the measure included language arts, math, and science.
    • (2001) Good Schools Have Good Librarians
    • Lance, K.C.1    Rodney, M.2    Hamilton-Pennell, C.3
  • 46
    • 0038038991 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Greensburg: Pennsylvania Citizens for Better Libraries
    • Replication studies by Lance and his colleagues include a second Colorado study (Keith Curry Lance, Marcia Rodney, and Christine Hamilton-Pennell, How School Librarians Help Kids Achieve Standards: The Second Colorado Study [San Jose, Calif.: Hi Willow Research and Publishing, 2000]); and replications in Alaska (Keith Curry Lance et al., Information Empowered: The School Librarian as an Agent of Academic Achievement in Alaska Schools [Juneau: Alaska State Library, 1999]), Oregon (Keith Curry Lance, Marcia Rodney, and Christine Hamilton-Pennell, Good Schools Have Good Librarians [Terrebone: Oregon Educational Media Association, 2001]), and Pennsylvania (Keith Curry Lance et al., Measuring to Standards: The Impact of School Library Programs and Information Literacy in Pennsylvania Schools [Greensburg: Pennsylvania Citizens for Better Libraries, 2000]). Similar studies have been done in Texas (Ester Smith, Texas School Libraries: Standards, Resources, Services, and Students' Performance, Texas State Library and Archives Commission, 2001, available at www.tsl.state.tx.us/ld/pubs/schlibsurvey/ index.html): Massachusetts (James Baughman, School Libraries and MCAS Scores, 2000, available at http://web.simmons. edu/∼baughman/mcas-school-libraries/); and Indiana (A Study of the Differences Between Higher- and Lower-Performing Indiana Schools in Reading and Mathematics [Oak Brook, III.: North Central Regional Educational Laboratory, 2000]). In all these studies, poverty was a predictor of achievement. The number of books per student and the level of library staffing were also consistent predictors of achievement. In most studies, these relationships held even when poverty was controlled (the Colorado replication and the Oregon, Indiana, Massachusetts, and Texas studies); in the Alaska and Pennsylvania studies, only staffing predicted reading scores, and in the Alaska study this was the case only when poverty was not controlled. The Colorado replication and the Pennsylvania, Texas, and Oregon studies used tests of reading comprehension only. In Alaska and Indiana, math was included, and in Massachusetts, the measure included language arts, math, and science.
    • (2000) Measuring to Standards: The Impact of School Library Programs and Information Literacy in Pennsylvania Schools
    • Lance, K.C.1
  • 47
    • 0038377237 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Texas State Library and Archives Commission
    • Replication studies by Lance and his colleagues include a second Colorado study (Keith Curry Lance, Marcia Rodney, and Christine Hamilton-Pennell, How School Librarians Help Kids Achieve Standards: The Second Colorado Study [San Jose, Calif.: Hi Willow Research and Publishing, 2000]); and replications in Alaska (Keith Curry Lance et al., Information Empowered: The School Librarian as an Agent of Academic Achievement in Alaska Schools [Juneau: Alaska State Library, 1999]), Oregon (Keith Curry Lance, Marcia Rodney, and Christine Hamilton-Pennell, Good Schools Have Good Librarians [Terrebone: Oregon Educational Media Association, 2001]), and Pennsylvania (Keith Curry Lance et al., Measuring to Standards: The Impact of School Library Programs and Information Literacy in Pennsylvania Schools [Greensburg: Pennsylvania Citizens for Better Libraries, 2000]). Similar studies have been done in Texas (Ester Smith, Texas School Libraries: Standards, Resources, Services, and Students' Performance, Texas State Library and Archives Commission, 2001, available at www.tsl.state.tx.us/ld/pubs/schlibsurvey/ index.html): Massachusetts (James Baughman, School Libraries and MCAS Scores, 2000, available at http://web.simmons. edu/∼baughman/mcas-school-libraries/); and Indiana (A Study of the Differences Between Higher- and Lower-Performing Indiana Schools in Reading and Mathematics [Oak Brook, III.: North Central Regional Educational Laboratory, 2000]). In all these studies, poverty was a predictor of achievement. The number of books per student and the level of library staffing were also consistent predictors of achievement. In most studies, these relationships held even when poverty was controlled (the Colorado replication and the Oregon, Indiana, Massachusetts, and Texas studies); in the Alaska and Pennsylvania studies, only staffing predicted reading scores, and in the Alaska study this was the case only when poverty was not controlled. The Colorado replication and the Pennsylvania, Texas, and Oregon studies used tests of reading comprehension only. In Alaska and Indiana, math was included, and in Massachusetts, the measure included language arts, math, and science.
    • (2001) Texas School Libraries: Standards, Resources, Services, and Students' Performance
    • Smith, E.1
  • 48
    • 0039923831 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Replication studies by Lance and his colleagues include a second Colorado study (Keith Curry Lance, Marcia Rodney, and Christine Hamilton-Pennell, How School Librarians Help Kids Achieve Standards: The Second Colorado Study [San Jose, Calif.: Hi Willow Research and Publishing, 2000]); and replications in Alaska (Keith Curry Lance et al., Information Empowered: The School Librarian as an Agent of Academic Achievement in Alaska Schools [Juneau: Alaska State Library, 1999]), Oregon (Keith Curry Lance, Marcia Rodney, and Christine Hamilton-Pennell, Good Schools Have Good Librarians [Terrebone: Oregon Educational Media Association, 2001]), and Pennsylvania (Keith Curry Lance et al., Measuring to Standards: The Impact of School Library Programs and Information Literacy in Pennsylvania Schools [Greensburg: Pennsylvania Citizens for Better Libraries, 2000]). Similar studies have been done in Texas (Ester Smith, Texas School Libraries: Standards, Resources, Services, and Students' Performance, Texas State Library and Archives Commission, 2001, available at www.tsl.state.tx.us/ld/pubs/schlibsurvey/ index.html): Massachusetts (James Baughman, School Libraries and MCAS Scores, 2000, available at http://web.simmons. edu/∼baughman/mcas-school-libraries/); and Indiana (A Study of the Differences Between Higher- and Lower-Performing Indiana Schools in Reading and Mathematics [Oak Brook, III.: North Central Regional Educational Laboratory, 2000]). In all these studies, poverty was a predictor of achievement. The number of books per student and the level of library staffing were also consistent predictors of achievement. In most studies, these relationships held even when poverty was controlled (the Colorado replication and the Oregon, Indiana, Massachusetts, and Texas studies); in the Alaska and Pennsylvania studies, only staffing predicted reading scores, and in the Alaska study this was the case only when poverty was not controlled. The Colorado replication and the Pennsylvania, Texas, and Oregon studies used tests of reading comprehension only. In Alaska and Indiana, math was included, and in Massachusetts, the measure included language arts, math, and science.
    • (2000) School Libraries and MCAS Scores
    • Baughman, J.1
  • 49
    • 0039331435 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Oak Brook, III.: North Central Regional Educational Laboratory
    • Replication studies by Lance and his colleagues include a second Colorado study (Keith Curry Lance, Marcia Rodney, and Christine Hamilton-Pennell, How School Librarians Help Kids Achieve Standards: The Second Colorado Study [San Jose, Calif.: Hi Willow Research and Publishing, 2000]); and replications in Alaska (Keith Curry Lance et al., Information Empowered: The School Librarian as an Agent of Academic Achievement in Alaska Schools [Juneau: Alaska State Library, 1999]), Oregon (Keith Curry Lance, Marcia Rodney, and Christine Hamilton-Pennell, Good Schools Have Good Librarians [Terrebone: Oregon Educational Media Association, 2001]), and Pennsylvania (Keith Curry Lance et al., Measuring to Standards: The Impact of School Library Programs and Information Literacy in Pennsylvania Schools [Greensburg: Pennsylvania Citizens for Better Libraries, 2000]). Similar studies have been done in Texas (Ester Smith, Texas School Libraries: Standards, Resources, Services, and Students' Performance, Texas State Library and Archives Commission, 2001, available at www.tsl.state.tx.us/ld/pubs/schlibsurvey/ index.html): Massachusetts (James Baughman, School Libraries and MCAS Scores, 2000, available at http://web.simmons. edu/∼baughman/mcas-school-libraries/); and Indiana (A Study of the Differences Between Higher- and Lower-Performing Indiana Schools in Reading and Mathematics [Oak Brook, III.: North Central Regional Educational Laboratory, 2000]). In all these studies, poverty was a predictor of achievement. The number of books per student and the level of library staffing were also consistent predictors of achievement. In most studies, these relationships held even when poverty was controlled (the Colorado replication and the Oregon, Indiana, Massachusetts, and Texas studies); in the Alaska and Pennsylvania studies, only staffing predicted reading scores, and in the Alaska study this was the case only when poverty was not controlled. The Colorado replication and the Pennsylvania, Texas, and Oregon studies used tests of reading comprehension only. In Alaska and Indiana, math was included, and in Massachusetts, the measure included language arts, math, and science.
    • (2000) A Study of the Differences Between Higher- and Lower-Performing Indiana Schools in Reading and Mathematics
  • 51
    • 0003619530 scopus 로고
    • Washington, D.C.: National Institute of Education
    • Richard Anderson et al., Becoming a Nation of Readers (Washington, D.C.: National Institute of Education, 1985), p. 38.
    • (1985) Becoming a Nation of Readers , pp. 38
    • Anderson, R.1
  • 56
    • 0007323762 scopus 로고
    • Sacramento: California Department of Education
    • Meanwhile, SAT 9 (Stanford Achievement Test) scores have increased in California since 1998. Does the increased emphasis on phonics deserve the credit? Recall that NAEP scores up to 1998 reveal no increases for fourth-graders in California and that there have been no NAEP results analyzed for individual states since that time. One could argue that the impact of phonics became apparent only after 1998, because it took time for changes to be made. The new approaches to reading were announced only in 1995 (see Every Child a Reader [Sacramento: California Department of Education, 1995]).
    • (1995) Every Child a Reader
  • 57
    • 0002823683 scopus 로고
    • Comparing state and district test results to national norms: The validity of claims that 'everyone is above average'
    • It is premature to grant credit for SAT 9 increases to intensive phonics instruction, however. There are other plausible reasons for the gains. The SAT 9 was introduced in California in 1998. Research has shown that, after new tests are introduced, test scores rise, which is why commercial tests need to be recalibrated every few years (see Robert Linn, Elizabeth Graue, and Nancy Sanders, "Comparing State and District Test Results to National Norms: The Validity of Claims That 'Everyone Is Above Average,'" Educational Measurement: Issues and Practice, vol. 10, 1990, pp. 5-14).
    • (1990) Educational Measurement: Issues and Practice , vol.10 , pp. 5-14
    • Linn, R.1    Graue, E.2    Sanders, N.3
  • 58
    • 11144304734 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Test scores up,test-takers down: Link between participation, improvement on school exam promotes concern
    • 22 July
    • Typical test score inflation is about 1.5 to 2 points per year, which accounts for a great deal of the gain seen in California. "Test inflation" is especially prevalent in California where the same test has now been given for four years in a row, punishments for lower scores are severe, and rewards for higher scores are generous. This pressure has resulted in districts' using unusual and extraordinary means for raising test scores, some of which have nothing to do with increased competence. Among the bogus means of increasing test scores are extensive training in certain test-taking skills and selective testing -that is, excluding low-scoring children from the tested group. San Francisco Chronicle reporter Nanette Asimov reported that selective testing may have occurred in California (Nanette Asimov, "Test Scores Up,Test-Takers Down: Link Between Participation, Improvement on School Exam Promotes Concern," San Francisco Chronicle, 22 July 2000).
    • (2000) San Francisco Chronicle
    • Asimov, N.1
  • 59
    • 0039374454 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Beyond the smoke and mirrors: A critique of the national reading panel report on phonics
    • March
    • Asimov reported that in many cases in which SAT 9 scores increased from year to year, the number of students tested decreased. According to Asimov, "questionable pairings" appeared in 22 San Francisco area school districts. And of course some test-taking skills will raise scores without an increase in competence. If there is no penalty for guessing, for example, simply encouraging guessing will raise scores. Using these means to raise scores is like claiming to raise the temperature of the room by lighting a match under the thermometer. No study has been conducted of teaching practices before and after the new phonics emphasis, and no attempt has been made to search for a specific link between increased phonics teaching and improvement in test scores. A detailed look at experimental research comparing intensive versus "regular" phonics teaching shows that the phonics advantage is limited only to phonetically regular words presented in isolation - with only a weak impact on tests of reading comprehension for older children (grades 2-6). SAT 9 tests are given only to grades 2 and higher (see Elaine M. Garan, "Beyond the Smoke and Mirrors: A Critique of the National Reading Panel Report on Phonics," Phi Delta Kappan, March 2001, pp. 500-506).
    • (2001) Phi Delta Kappan , pp. 500-506
    • Garan, E.M.1


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.