메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 11, Issue 3, 2002, Pages 243-257

Attributions in explanations of risk estimates

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords

ANIMAL; ARTICLE; HISTORY; PARASITE; PUBLIC HEALTH; RISK ASSESSMENT; UNITED STATES; WATER POLLUTION;

EID: 0036634139     PISSN: 09636625     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1088/0963-6625/11/3/303     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (17)

References (75)
  • 1
    • 0004329876 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
    • See, for example, James Voss, David Perkins, and Judith Segal, Informal Reasoning and Education (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1991); Kathleen Galotti, "Approaches to studying informal and everyday reasoning," Psychological Bulletin 105, no. 3 (1989): 331-351; Brian Wynne, "Public understanding of science," in Handbook of Science and Technology Studies, ed. Sheila Jasanoff, Gerald E. Markle, James C. Petersen, and Trevor Pinch (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 1995): 361-388; See, for example, Herbert Simon, "Rational choice and the structure of the environment," Psychological Review 63 (1956): 129-138; Herbert Simon, Models of Bounded Rationality (Cambridge: MIT Press, 1982); Shelly Chaiken, "The heuristic model of persuasion," in Social Influence: The Ontario Symposium, vol. 5, ed. Mark Zanna, James Olson, and C. Peter Herman (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,1980: 3-39; Shelly Chaiken, Akiva Liberman, and Alice Eagly, "Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context," in Unintended Thought, ed. James Uleman and John Bargh (New York: Guilford, 1989) 212-252; Alice Eagly and Shelly Chaiken, The Psychology of Attitudes (San Diego: Harcourt Brace, 1993); Gerd Gigerenzer and Daniel Goldstein, "Reasoning the fast and frugal way: models of bounded rationality," Psychological Review 103, no. 4 (1996): 650-669.
    • (1991) Informal Reasoning and Education
    • Voss, J.1    Perkins, D.2    Segal, J.3
  • 2
    • 0000003554 scopus 로고
    • Approaches to studying informal and everyday reasoning
    • See, for example, James Voss, David Perkins, and Judith Segal, Informal Reasoning and Education (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1991); Kathleen Galotti, "Approaches to studying informal and everyday reasoning," Psychological Bulletin 105, no. 3 (1989): 331-351; Brian Wynne, "Public understanding of science," in Handbook of Science and Technology Studies, ed. Sheila Jasanoff, Gerald E. Markle, James C. Petersen, and Trevor Pinch (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 1995): 361-388; See, for example, Herbert Simon, "Rational choice and the structure of the environment," Psychological Review 63 (1956): 129-138; Herbert Simon, Models of Bounded Rationality (Cambridge: MIT Press, 1982); Shelly Chaiken, "The heuristic model of persuasion," in Social Influence: The Ontario Symposium, vol. 5, ed. Mark Zanna, James Olson, and C. Peter Herman (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,1980: 3-39; Shelly Chaiken, Akiva Liberman, and Alice Eagly, "Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context," in Unintended Thought, ed. James Uleman and John Bargh (New York: Guilford, 1989) 212-252; Alice Eagly and Shelly Chaiken, The Psychology of Attitudes (San Diego: Harcourt Brace, 1993); Gerd Gigerenzer and Daniel Goldstein, "Reasoning the fast and frugal way: models of bounded rationality," Psychological Review 103, no. 4 (1996): 650-669.
    • (1989) Psychological Bulletin , vol.105 , Issue.3 , pp. 331-351
    • Galotti, K.1
  • 3
    • 0002572453 scopus 로고
    • Public understanding of science
    • ed. Sheila Jasanoff, Gerald E. Markle, James C. Petersen, and Trevor Pinch Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
    • See, for example, James Voss, David Perkins, and Judith Segal, Informal Reasoning and Education (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1991); Kathleen Galotti, "Approaches to studying informal and everyday reasoning," Psychological Bulletin 105, no. 3 (1989): 331-351; Brian Wynne, "Public understanding of science," in Handbook of Science and Technology Studies, ed. Sheila Jasanoff, Gerald E. Markle, James C. Petersen, and Trevor Pinch (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 1995): 361-388; See, for example, Herbert Simon, "Rational choice and the structure of the environment," Psychological Review 63 (1956): 129-138; Herbert Simon, Models of Bounded Rationality (Cambridge: MIT Press, 1982); Shelly Chaiken, "The heuristic model of persuasion," in Social Influence: The Ontario Symposium, vol. 5, ed. Mark Zanna, James Olson, and C. Peter Herman (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,1980: 3-39; Shelly Chaiken, Akiva Liberman, and Alice Eagly, "Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context," in Unintended Thought, ed. James Uleman and John Bargh (New York: Guilford, 1989) 212-252; Alice Eagly and Shelly Chaiken, The Psychology of Attitudes (San Diego: Harcourt Brace, 1993); Gerd Gigerenzer and Daniel Goldstein, "Reasoning the fast and frugal way: models of bounded rationality," Psychological Review 103, no. 4 (1996): 650-669.
    • (1995) Handbook of Science and Technology Studies , pp. 361-388
    • Wynne, B.1
  • 4
    • 58149433367 scopus 로고
    • Rational choice and the structure of the environment
    • See, for example, James Voss, David Perkins, and Judith Segal, Informal Reasoning and Education (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1991); Kathleen Galotti, "Approaches to studying informal and everyday reasoning," Psychological Bulletin 105, no. 3 (1989): 331-351; Brian Wynne, "Public understanding of science," in Handbook of Science and Technology Studies, ed. Sheila Jasanoff, Gerald E. Markle, James C. Petersen, and Trevor Pinch (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 1995): 361-388; See, for example, Herbert Simon, "Rational choice and the structure of the environment," Psychological Review 63 (1956): 129-138; Herbert Simon, Models of Bounded Rationality (Cambridge: MIT Press, 1982); Shelly Chaiken, "The heuristic model of persuasion," in Social Influence: The Ontario Symposium, vol. 5, ed. Mark Zanna, James Olson, and C. Peter Herman (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,1980: 3-39; Shelly Chaiken, Akiva Liberman, and Alice Eagly, "Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context," in Unintended Thought, ed. James Uleman and John Bargh (New York: Guilford, 1989) 212-252; Alice Eagly and Shelly Chaiken, The Psychology of Attitudes (San Diego: Harcourt Brace, 1993); Gerd Gigerenzer and Daniel Goldstein, "Reasoning the fast and frugal way: models of bounded rationality," Psychological Review 103, no. 4 (1996): 650-669.
    • (1956) Psychological Review , vol.63 , pp. 129-138
    • Simon, H.1
  • 5
    • 0004077471 scopus 로고
    • Cambridge: MIT Press
    • See, for example, James Voss, David Perkins, and Judith Segal, Informal Reasoning and Education (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1991); Kathleen Galotti, "Approaches to studying informal and everyday reasoning," Psychological Bulletin 105, no. 3 (1989): 331-351; Brian Wynne, "Public understanding of science," in Handbook of Science and Technology Studies, ed. Sheila Jasanoff, Gerald E. Markle, James C. Petersen, and Trevor Pinch (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 1995): 361-388; See, for example, Herbert Simon, "Rational choice and the structure of the environment," Psychological Review 63 (1956): 129-138; Herbert Simon, Models of Bounded Rationality (Cambridge: MIT Press, 1982); Shelly Chaiken, "The heuristic model of persuasion," in Social Influence: The Ontario Symposium, vol. 5, ed. Mark Zanna, James Olson, and C. Peter Herman (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,1980: 3-39; Shelly Chaiken, Akiva Liberman, and Alice Eagly, "Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context," in Unintended Thought, ed. James Uleman and John Bargh (New York: Guilford, 1989) 212-252; Alice Eagly and Shelly Chaiken, The Psychology of Attitudes (San Diego: Harcourt Brace, 1993); Gerd Gigerenzer and Daniel Goldstein, "Reasoning the fast and frugal way: models of bounded rationality," Psychological Review 103, no. 4 (1996): 650-669.
    • (1982) Models of Bounded Rationality
    • Simon, H.1
  • 6
    • 0001662062 scopus 로고
    • The heuristic model of persuasion
    • ed. Mark Zanna, James Olson, and C. Peter Herman Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
    • See, for example, James Voss, David Perkins, and Judith Segal, Informal Reasoning and Education (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1991); Kathleen Galotti, "Approaches to studying informal and everyday reasoning," Psychological Bulletin 105, no. 3 (1989): 331-351; Brian Wynne, "Public understanding of science," in Handbook of Science and Technology Studies, ed. Sheila Jasanoff, Gerald E. Markle, James C. Petersen, and Trevor Pinch (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 1995): 361-388; See, for example, Herbert Simon, "Rational choice and the structure of the environment," Psychological Review 63 (1956): 129-138; Herbert Simon, Models of Bounded Rationality (Cambridge: MIT Press, 1982); Shelly Chaiken, "The heuristic model of persuasion," in Social Influence: The Ontario Symposium, vol. 5, ed. Mark Zanna, James Olson, and C. Peter Herman (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,1980: 3-39; Shelly Chaiken, Akiva Liberman, and Alice Eagly, "Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context," in Unintended Thought, ed. James Uleman and John Bargh (New York: Guilford, 1989) 212-252; Alice Eagly and Shelly Chaiken, The Psychology of Attitudes (San Diego: Harcourt Brace, 1993); Gerd Gigerenzer and Daniel Goldstein, "Reasoning the fast and frugal way: models of bounded rationality," Psychological Review 103, no. 4 (1996): 650-669.
    • (1980) Social Influence: The Ontario Symposium , vol.5 , pp. 3-39
    • Chaiken, S.1
  • 7
    • 0002016719 scopus 로고
    • Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context
    • ed. James Uleman and John Bargh New York: Guilford
    • See, for example, James Voss, David Perkins, and Judith Segal, Informal Reasoning and Education (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1991); Kathleen Galotti, "Approaches to studying informal and everyday reasoning," Psychological Bulletin 105, no. 3 (1989): 331-351; Brian Wynne, "Public understanding of science," in Handbook of Science and Technology Studies, ed. Sheila Jasanoff, Gerald E. Markle, James C. Petersen, and Trevor Pinch (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 1995): 361-388; See, for example, Herbert Simon, "Rational choice and the structure of the environment," Psychological Review 63 (1956): 129-138; Herbert Simon, Models of Bounded Rationality (Cambridge: MIT Press, 1982); Shelly Chaiken, "The heuristic model of persuasion," in Social Influence: The Ontario Symposium, vol. 5, ed. Mark Zanna, James Olson, and C. Peter Herman (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,1980: 3-39; Shelly Chaiken, Akiva Liberman, and Alice Eagly, "Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context," in Unintended Thought, ed. James Uleman and John Bargh (New York: Guilford, 1989) 212-252; Alice Eagly and Shelly Chaiken, The Psychology of Attitudes (San Diego: Harcourt Brace, 1993); Gerd Gigerenzer and Daniel Goldstein, "Reasoning the fast and frugal way: models of bounded rationality," Psychological Review 103, no. 4 (1996): 650-669.
    • (1989) Unintended Thought , pp. 212-252
    • Chaiken, S.1    Liberman, A.2    Eagly, A.3
  • 8
    • 0003714701 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • San Diego: Harcourt Brace
    • See, for example, James Voss, David Perkins, and Judith Segal, Informal Reasoning and Education (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1991); Kathleen Galotti, "Approaches to studying informal and everyday reasoning," Psychological Bulletin 105, no. 3 (1989): 331-351; Brian Wynne, "Public understanding of science," in Handbook of Science and Technology Studies, ed. Sheila Jasanoff, Gerald E. Markle, James C. Petersen, and Trevor Pinch (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 1995): 361-388; See, for example, Herbert Simon, "Rational choice and the structure of the environment," Psychological Review 63 (1956): 129-138; Herbert Simon, Models of Bounded Rationality (Cambridge: MIT Press, 1982); Shelly Chaiken, "The heuristic model of persuasion," in Social Influence: The Ontario Symposium, vol. 5, ed. Mark Zanna, James Olson, and C. Peter Herman (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,1980: 3-39; Shelly Chaiken, Akiva Liberman, and Alice Eagly, "Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context," in Unintended Thought, ed. James Uleman and John Bargh (New York: Guilford, 1989) 212-252; Alice Eagly and Shelly Chaiken, The Psychology of Attitudes (San Diego: Harcourt Brace, 1993); Gerd Gigerenzer and Daniel Goldstein, "Reasoning the fast and frugal way: models of bounded rationality," Psychological Review 103, no. 4 (1996): 650-669.
    • (1993) The Psychology of Attitudes
    • Eagly, A.1    Chaiken, S.2
  • 9
    • 0030266119 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Reasoning the fast and frugal way: Models of bounded rationality
    • See, for example, James Voss, David Perkins, and Judith Segal, Informal Reasoning and Education (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1991); Kathleen Galotti, "Approaches to studying informal and everyday reasoning," Psychological Bulletin 105, no. 3 (1989): 331-351; Brian Wynne, "Public understanding of science," in Handbook of Science and Technology Studies, ed. Sheila Jasanoff, Gerald E. Markle, James C. Petersen, and Trevor Pinch (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 1995): 361-388; See, for example, Herbert Simon, "Rational choice and the structure of the environment," Psychological Review 63 (1956): 129-138; Herbert Simon, Models of Bounded Rationality (Cambridge: MIT Press, 1982); Shelly Chaiken, "The heuristic model of persuasion," in Social Influence: The Ontario Symposium, vol. 5, ed. Mark Zanna, James Olson, and C. Peter Herman (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,1980: 3-39; Shelly Chaiken, Akiva Liberman, and Alice Eagly, "Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context," in Unintended Thought, ed. James Uleman and John Bargh (New York: Guilford, 1989) 212-252; Alice Eagly and Shelly Chaiken, The Psychology of Attitudes (San Diego: Harcourt Brace, 1993); Gerd Gigerenzer and Daniel Goldstein, "Reasoning the fast and frugal way: models of bounded rationality," Psychological Review 103, no. 4 (1996): 650-669.
    • (1996) Psychological Review , vol.103 , Issue.4 , pp. 650-669
    • Gigerenzer, G.1    Goldstein, D.2
  • 12
    • 1542479100 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Chaiken et al., "Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context;" Eagly et al., The Psychology of Attitudes; Akiva Liberman and Shelly Chaiken, "Defensive processing of personally relevant health messages," Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 18, no. 6 (1992): 669-679. Gigerenzer and Goldstein, "Reasoning the fast and frugal way."
    • Heuristic and Systematic Processing Within and Beyond the Persuasion Context
    • Chaiken1
  • 13
    • 0003714701 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Chaiken et al., "Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context;" Eagly et al., The Psychology of Attitudes; Akiva Liberman and Shelly Chaiken, "Defensive processing of personally relevant health messages," Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 18, no. 6 (1992): 669-679. Gigerenzer and Goldstein, "Reasoning the fast and frugal way."
    • The Psychology of Attitudes
    • Eagly1
  • 14
    • 21144471327 scopus 로고
    • Defensive processing of personally relevant health messages
    • Chaiken et al., "Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context;" Eagly et al., The Psychology of Attitudes; Akiva Liberman and Shelly Chaiken, "Defensive processing of personally relevant health messages," Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 18, no. 6 (1992): 669-679. Gigerenzer and Goldstein, "Reasoning the fast and frugal way."
    • (1992) Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin , vol.18 , Issue.6 , pp. 669-679
    • Liberman, A.1    Chaiken, S.2
  • 15
    • 0043255433 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Chaiken et al., "Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context;" Eagly et al., The Psychology of Attitudes; Akiva Liberman and Shelly Chaiken, "Defensive processing of personally relevant health messages," Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 18, no. 6 (1992): 669-679. Gigerenzer and Goldstein, "Reasoning the fast and frugal way."
    • Reasoning the Fast and Frugal Way
    • Gigerenzer1    Goldstein2
  • 16
    • 0002795580 scopus 로고
    • Facts versus fears: Understanding perceived risk
    • ed. Daniel Kuhneman, Paul Slovic, and Amos Tversky Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
    • Paul Slovic, Baruch Fischoff, und Sarah Lichtenstein, "Facts versus fears: understanding perceived risk," in Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases, ed. Daniel Kuhneman, Paul Slovic, and Amos Tversky (Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 1982): 463-489.
    • (1982) Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , pp. 463-489
    • Slovic, P.1    Fischoff, B.2    Lichtenstein, S.3
  • 19
    • 0042252973 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Characteristics of individual risk perception
    • ed. Bayerische Ruck Munchen: Knesebeck
    • Helmut Jungermann and Paul Slovic, "Characteristics of individual risk perception," in Risk is a Construct, ed. Bayerische Ruck (Munchen: Knesebeck, 1993): 85-102.
    • (1993) Risk Is a Construct , pp. 85-102
    • Jungermann, H.1    Slovic, P.2
  • 20
    • 0042252982 scopus 로고
    • Thinking aloud about impersonal impact
    • Atlanta GA, 1984. Department of Communication Paper 15-94-02
    • See, for example, Michael Shapiro and Miejeong Han, "Thinking aloud about impersonal impact." Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the Association for Education in Journalism and Mass Communication, Atlanta GA, 1984. Department of Communication Paper 15-94-02 (1994); Neil Weinstein, "Why it won't happen to me: perceptions of risk factors and susceptibility," Health Psychology 3, no. 5 (1984): 431-457; Bethan Henderson and Bryan Maguire, "Three lay models of disease inheritance," Social Science and Medicine 50 (2000): 293- 301; Sharon Dunwoody, Kurt Neuwirth, Robert Griffin, and Marilee Long, "The impact of risk message content and construction on comments about risks embedded in 'letters to friends,"' Journal of Language and Social Psychology 11, no. 2 (1992): 9-33.
    • (1994) Annual Conference of the Association for Education in Journalism and Mass Communication
    • Shapiro, M.1    Han, M.2
  • 21
    • 0021667965 scopus 로고
    • Why it won't happen to me: Perceptions of risk factors and susceptibility
    • See, for example, Michael Shapiro and Miejeong Han, "Thinking aloud about impersonal impact." Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the Association for Education in Journalism and Mass Communication, Atlanta GA, 1984. Department of Communication Paper 15-94-02 (1994); Neil Weinstein, "Why it won't happen to me: perceptions of risk factors and susceptibility," Health Psychology 3, no. 5 (1984): 431-457; Bethan Henderson and Bryan Maguire, "Three lay models of disease inheritance," Social Science and Medicine 50 (2000): 293- 301; Sharon Dunwoody, Kurt Neuwirth, Robert Griffin, and Marilee Long, "The impact of risk message content and construction on comments about risks embedded in 'letters to friends,"' Journal of Language and Social Psychology 11, no. 2 (1992): 9-33.
    • (1984) Health Psychology , vol.3 , Issue.5 , pp. 431-457
    • Weinstein, N.1
  • 22
    • 0033992566 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Three lay models of disease inheritance
    • See, for example, Michael Shapiro and Miejeong Han, "Thinking aloud about impersonal impact." Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the Association for Education in Journalism and Mass Communication, Atlanta GA, 1984. Department of Communication Paper 15-94-02 (1994); Neil Weinstein, "Why it won't happen to me: perceptions of risk factors and susceptibility," Health Psychology 3, no. 5 (1984): 431-457; Bethan Henderson and Bryan Maguire, "Three lay models of disease inheritance," Social Science and Medicine 50 (2000): 293-301; Sharon Dunwoody, Kurt Neuwirth, Robert Griffin, and Marilee Long, "The impact of risk message content and construction on comments about risks embedded in 'letters to friends,"' Journal of Language and Social Psychology 11, no. 2 (1992): 9-33.
    • (2000) Social Science and Medicine , vol.50 , pp. 293-301
    • Henderson, B.1    Maguire, B.2
  • 23
    • 0006854265 scopus 로고
    • The impact of risk message content and construction on comments about risks embedded in 'letters to friends'
    • See, for example, Michael Shapiro and Miejeong Han, "Thinking aloud about impersonal impact." Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the Association for Education in Journalism and Mass Communication, Atlanta GA, 1984. Department of Communication Paper 15-94-02 (1994); Neil Weinstein, "Why it won't happen to me: perceptions of risk factors and susceptibility," Health Psychology 3, no. 5 (1984): 431-457; Bethan Henderson and Bryan Maguire, "Three lay models of disease inheritance," Social Science and Medicine 50 (2000): 293- 301; Sharon Dunwoody, Kurt Neuwirth, Robert Griffin, and Marilee Long, "The impact of risk message content and construction on comments about risks embedded in 'letters to friends,"' Journal of Language and Social Psychology 11, no. 2 (1992): 9-33.
    • (1992) Journal of Language and Social Psychology , vol.11 , Issue.2 , pp. 9-33
    • Dunwoody, S.1    Neuwirth, K.2    Griffin, R.3    Long, M.4
  • 34
    • 0000656889 scopus 로고
    • Popular induction: Information is not necessarily informative
    • ed. John S. Carroll and John Payne Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
    • Richard Nisbett, Eugene Borgida, Rick Crandall, and Harvey Reed, "Popular induction: information is not necessarily informative," in Cognition and Social Behavior, ed. John S. Carroll and John Payne (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1982), 101; Harold Kelley, "Attribution theory in social psychology," in Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: 1967, ed. D. Levine (Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1967), 192-241; Nisbett et al., "Popular induction: information is not necessarily informative," p. 101.
    • (1982) Cognition and Social Behavior , vol.101
    • Nisbett, R.1    Borgida, E.2    Crandall, R.3    Reed, H.4
  • 35
    • 0002395197 scopus 로고
    • Attribution theory in social psychology
    • ed. D. Levine Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press
    • Richard Nisbett, Eugene Borgida, Rick Crandall, and Harvey Reed, "Popular induction: information is not necessarily informative," in Cognition and Social Behavior, ed. John S. Carroll and John Payne (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1982), 101; Harold Kelley, "Attribution theory in social psychology," in Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: 1967, ed. D. Levine (Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1967), 192-241; Nisbett et al., "Popular induction: information is not necessarily informative," p. 101.
    • (1967) Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: 1967 , pp. 192-241
    • Kelley, H.1
  • 36
    • 0002944790 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Richard Nisbett, Eugene Borgida, Rick Crandall, and Harvey Reed, "Popular induction: information is not necessarily informative," in Cognition and Social Behavior, ed. John S. Carroll and John Payne (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1982), 101; Harold Kelley, "Attribution theory in social psychology," in Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: 1967, ed. D. Levine (Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1967), 192-241; Nisbett et al., "Popular induction: information is not necessarily informative," p. 101.
    • Popular Induction: Information Is Not Necessarily Informative , pp. 101
    • Nisbett1
  • 37
    • 0004220038 scopus 로고
    • Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall
    • Stuart Oskamp, Attitudes and Opinions (Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall, 1977); Weiner, Judgments Of Responsibility: A Foundation For A Theory Of Social Conduct.
    • (1977) Attitudes and Opinions
    • Oskamp, S.1
  • 39
    • 0003103997 scopus 로고
    • Shortcomings in the attributional process: On the origins and maintenance of erroneous social assessments
    • ed. Daniel Kahneman, Paul Slovic, and Amos Tversky Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
    • Lee Ross and Craig Andersen, "Shortcomings in the attributional process: on the origins and maintenance of erroneous social assessments," in Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases, ed. Daniel Kahneman, Paul Slovic, and Amos Tversky (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, (1982): 129-152.
    • (1982) Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , pp. 129-152
    • Ross, L.1    Andersen, C.2
  • 43
    • 0042252976 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Ross and Andersen, "Shortcomings in the attributional process"; Patricia Cheng, "From covariates to causation: a causal power theory," Psychological Review 104 (1997): 367-405; Weiner, Judgments of Responsibility.
    • Shortcomings in the Attributional Process
    • Ross1    Andersen2
  • 44
    • 0039630447 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • From covariates to causation: A causal power theory
    • Ross and Andersen, "Shortcomings in the attributional process"; Patricia Cheng, "From covariates to causation: a causal power theory," Psychological Review 104 (1997): 367-405; Weiner, Judgments of Responsibility.
    • (1997) Psychological Review , vol.104 , pp. 367-405
    • Cheng, P.1
  • 45
    • 0004242277 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Ross and Andersen, "Shortcomings in the attributional process"; Patricia Cheng, "From covariates to causation: a causal power theory," Psychological Review 104 (1997): 367-405; Weiner, Judgments of Responsibility.
    • Judgments of Responsibility
    • Weiner1
  • 46
    • 0001442380 scopus 로고
    • Motivated inference: Self-serving generation and evaluation of causal theories
    • Ziva Kunda, "Motivated inference: self-serving generation and evaluation of causal theories," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 53, no. 4 (1987): 636-647.
    • (1987) Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , vol.53 , Issue.4 , pp. 636-647
    • Kunda, Z.1
  • 47
    • 85047682849 scopus 로고
    • What constitutes a self-serving attributional bias?
    • Dale T. Miller, "What constitutes a self-serving attributional bias?" Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 36 (1978): 1221-1223.
    • (1978) Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , vol.36 , pp. 1221-1223
    • Miller, D.T.1
  • 48
    • 0034404840 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Self-efficacy and causal attributions: Direct and reciprocal links
    • Alexander Stajkovic and Steven Sommer, "Self-efficacy and causal attributions: direct and reciprocal links," Journal of Applied Social Psychology 30, no. 4 (2000): 707-737.
    • (2000) Journal of Applied Social Psychology , vol.30 , Issue.4 , pp. 707-737
    • Stajkovic, A.1    Sommer, S.2
  • 49
    • 0000562052 scopus 로고
    • Blaming others for threatening events
    • Howard Tennan and Glenn Affleck, "Blaming others for threatening events," Psychological Bulletin 108, no. 2 (1990): 209-232.
    • (1990) Psychological Bulletin , vol.108 , Issue.2 , pp. 209-232
    • Tennan, H.1    Affleck, G.2
  • 50
    • 0022905674 scopus 로고
    • Informing and educating the public about risk
    • Paul Slovic, "Informing and educating the public about risk," Risk Analysis 6, no. 4 (1986): 403-415. Baruch Fischhoff, Paul Slovic, and Sarah Lichtenstein, "Lay foibles and expert fables in judgments about risk," The America Statistician 36, no. 3 (1982): 240-255. Ulrich Hoffrage and Gerd Gigerenzer, "Using natural frequencies to improve diagnostic inferences," Academic Medicine 73, no. 5 (1998): 538-540. Ulrich Hoffrage, Samuel Lindsey, Ralph Hertwig, and Gerd Gigerenzer, "Communicating statistical information," Science 290 (22 December 2000): 2261-2262.
    • (1986) Risk Analysis , vol.6 , Issue.4 , pp. 403-415
    • Slovic, P.1
  • 51
    • 0022905674 scopus 로고
    • Lay foibles and expert fables in judgments about risk
    • Paul Slovic, "Informing and educating the public about risk," Risk Analysis 6, no. 4 (1986): 403-415. Baruch Fischhoff, Paul Slovic, and Sarah Lichtenstein, "Lay foibles and expert fables in judgments about risk," The America Statistician 36, no. 3 (1982): 240-255. Ulrich Hoffrage and Gerd Gigerenzer, "Using natural frequencies to improve diagnostic inferences," Academic Medicine 73, no. 5 (1998): 538-540. Ulrich Hoffrage, Samuel Lindsey, Ralph Hertwig, and Gerd Gigerenzer, "Communicating statistical information," Science 290 (22 December 2000): 2261-2262.
    • (1982) The America Statistician , vol.36 , Issue.3 , pp. 240-255
    • Fischhoff, B.1    Slovic, P.2    Lichtenstein, S.3
  • 52
    • 0031777047 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Using natural frequencies to improve diagnostic inferences
    • Paul Slovic, "Informing and educating the public about risk," Risk Analysis 6, no. 4 (1986): 403-415. Baruch Fischhoff, Paul Slovic, and Sarah Lichtenstein, "Lay foibles and expert fables in judgments about risk," The America Statistician 36, no. 3 (1982): 240-255. Ulrich Hoffrage and Gerd Gigerenzer, "Using natural frequencies to improve diagnostic inferences," Academic Medicine 73, no. 5 (1998): 538-540. Ulrich Hoffrage, Samuel Lindsey, Ralph Hertwig, and Gerd Gigerenzer, "Communicating statistical information," Science 290 (22 December 2000): 2261-2262.
    • (1998) Academic Medicine , vol.73 , Issue.5 , pp. 538-540
    • Hoffrage, U.1    Gigerenzer, G.2
  • 53
    • 0034704224 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Communicating statistical information
    • 22 December
    • Paul Slovic, "Informing and educating the public about risk," Risk Analysis 6, no. 4 (1986): 403-415. Baruch Fischhoff, Paul Slovic, and Sarah Lichtenstein, "Lay foibles and expert fables in judgments about risk," The America Statistician 36, no. 3 (1982): 240-255. Ulrich Hoffrage and Gerd Gigerenzer, "Using natural frequencies to improve diagnostic inferences," Academic Medicine 73, no. 5 (1998): 538-540. Ulrich Hoffrage, Samuel Lindsey, Ralph Hertwig, and Gerd Gigerenzer, "Communicating statistical information," Science 290 (22 December 2000): 2261-2262.
    • (2000) Science , vol.290 , pp. 2261-2262
    • Hoffrage, U.1    Lindsey, S.2    Hertwig, R.3    Gigerenzer, G.4
  • 54
    • 0030666187 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The role of numeracy in understanding the benefit of screening mammography
    • Lisa Schwartz, Steven Woloshin, William Black, and Gilbert Welch, "The role of numeracy in understanding the benefit of screening mammography," Annals of International Medicine 127 (1997): 966-972.
    • (1997) Annals of International Medicine , vol.127 , pp. 966-972
    • Schwartz, L.1    Woloshin, S.2    Black, W.3    Welch, G.4
  • 56
    • 0042252975 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Although we initially summed these behaviors to create an index, the index was unreliable.
  • 58
    • 0043255434 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Accuracy was originally intended to be a variable in this study; however, attempts to code the responses for accurate and inaccurate information proved too subjective for the coders.
  • 60
    • 0003726404 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage University Paper Series, Sage Publications, Odds ratios are presented in the SPSS output as Exp (B)
    • Scott Menard, Applied Logistic Regression (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage University Paper Series, Sage Publications, 1995), 21; Odds ratios are presented in the SPSS output as Exp (B).
    • (1995) Applied Logistic Regression , vol.21
    • Menard, S.1
  • 64
    • 0042252972 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • See Shapiro and Han, "Thinking aloud about impersonal impact," Weinstein, "Why it won't happen to me: perceptions of risk factors and susceptibility," Henderson and Maguire, "Three lay models of disease inheritance," Dunwoody, Neuwirth, Griffin, and Long, "The impact of risk message content and construction on comments about risks embedded in 'letters to friends."'
    • Thinking Aloud about Impersonal Impact
    • Shapiro1    Han2
  • 65
    • 0042252977 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • See Shapiro and Han, "Thinking aloud about impersonal impact," Weinstein, "Why it won't happen to me: perceptions of risk factors and susceptibility," Henderson and Maguire, "Three lay models of disease inheritance," Dunwoody, Neuwirth, Griffin, and Long, "The impact of risk message content and construction on comments about risks embedded in 'letters to friends."'
    • Why it Won't Happen to Me: Perceptions of Risk Factors and Susceptibility
    • Weinstein1
  • 66
    • 0041752410 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • See Shapiro and Han, "Thinking aloud about impersonal impact," Weinstein, "Why it won't happen to me: perceptions of risk factors and susceptibility," Henderson and Maguire, "Three lay models of disease inheritance," Dunwoody, Neuwirth, Griffin, and Long, "The impact of risk message content and construction on comments about risks embedded in 'letters to friends."'
    • Three Lay Models of Disease Inheritance
    • Henderson1    Maguire2
  • 68
    • 0000265336 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The knowledge gap hypothesis: Twenty-five years later
    • ed. B.R. Burleson Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
    • See, for example, K. Viswanath and John R. Finnegan, Jr., "The knowledge gap hypothesis: twenty-five years later," in Communication Yearbook 19, ed. B.R. Burleson (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 1996): 187-227.
    • (1996) Communication Yearbook 19 , vol.19 , pp. 187-227
    • Viswanath, K.1    Finnegan J.R., Jr.2
  • 69
    • 0031497668 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Community structure and science framing of news about local environmental risks
    • Robert Griffin and Sharon Dunwoody, "Community structure and science framing of news about local environmental risks," Science Communication 18 (1997): 362-384.
    • (1997) Science Communication , vol.18 , pp. 362-384
    • Griffin, R.1    Dunwoody, S.2
  • 70
    • 0004179736 scopus 로고
    • New York: Knopf
    • Martin Seligman, Learned Optimism (New York: Knopf, 1990); Bernard Weiner, Judgments of Responsibility: A Foundation for the Theory of Social Conduct (New York: Guilford Press, 1995).
    • (1990) Learned Optimism
    • Seligman, M.1
  • 75
    • 0033043069 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Proposed model of the relationship of risk information seeking and processing to the development of preventive behaviors
    • Robert Griffin, Sharon Dunwoody, and Kurt Neuwirth, "Proposed model of the relationship of risk information seeking and processing to the development of preventive behaviors," Environmental Rersearch 80 (1999): 230-245.
    • (1999) Environmental Rersearch , vol.80 , pp. 230-245
    • Griffin, R.1    Dunwoody, S.2    Neuwirth, K.3


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.