메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 287, Issue 5460, 2000, Pages 2036-2038

Motion integration and postdiction in visual awareness

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords

ARTICLE; AWARENESS; HUMAN; HUMAN EXPERIMENT; MOTION; NORMAL HUMAN; PRIORITY JOURNAL; PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY; VISUAL MASKING; VISUAL NERVOUS SYSTEM; VISUAL STIMULATION;

EID: 0034678028     PISSN: 00368075     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1126/science.287.5460.2036     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (424)

References (32)
  • 1
    • 0001364747 scopus 로고
    • D. MacKay, Nature 181, 507 (1958).
    • (1958) Nature , vol.181 , pp. 507
    • MacKay, D.1
  • 3
    • 0028852013 scopus 로고
    • M. V. Baldo and S. A. Klein, Nature 378, 565 (1995); G. Purushothaman, S. S. Patel, H. E. Bedell, H. Ogmen, Nature 396, 424 (1998).
    • (1995) Nature , vol.378 , pp. 565
    • Baldo, M.V.1    Klein, S.A.2
  • 5
    • 0032242606 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • D. Whitney and I. Murakami, Nature Neurosci. 1, 656 (1998); expanded upon in D. Whitney, I. Murakami, P. Cavanagh, Vision Res. 40, 137 (2000).
    • (1998) Nature Neurosci. , vol.1 , pp. 656
    • Whitney, D.1    Murakami, I.2
  • 8
    • 0343104060 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • -1. The trajectory of the stimulus covered 4° of the visual angle; the inner diameter of the moving ring and outer diameter of the flashed disk were 1°. All stimuli were programmed in C on a Silicon Graphics workstation, with a monitor refresh rate of 72 Hz = 13.3 ms/frame.
  • 9
    • 0343975783 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • -1) with proportionally scaled results (displacement for the continuous case, 3.3° ± 1.3°; for the stopped case, -0.3° ± 0.6°; for the reversed case, -4.5° ± 0.6°; n = 8).
  • 10
    • 36849146056 scopus 로고
    • This paradigm is analogous to the "flash-initiated cycle" used
    • This paradigm is analogous to the "flash-initiated cycle" used by B. Khurana and R. Nijhawan, Nature 378, 566 (1995).
    • (1995) Nature , vol.378 , pp. 566
    • Khurana, B.1    Nijhawan, R.2
  • 11
    • 0021505081 scopus 로고
    • S. Yantis and J. Jonides, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 10, 601 (1984); E. Weichselgartner and G. Sperling, Science 238, 778 (1987); J. Musseler and G. Aschersleben, Percept. Psychophys. 60, 683 (1998); K. Watanabe and S. Shimojo, Perception 27, 1041 (1998).
    • (1984) J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. , vol.10 , pp. 601
    • Yantis, S.1    Jonides, J.2
  • 12
    • 0023483354 scopus 로고
    • S. Yantis and J. Jonides, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 10, 601 (1984); E. Weichselgartner and G. Sperling, Science 238, 778 (1987); J. Musseler and G. Aschersleben, Percept. Psychophys. 60, 683 (1998); K. Watanabe and S. Shimojo, Perception 27, 1041 (1998).
    • (1987) Science , vol.238 , pp. 778
    • Weichselgartner, E.1    Sperling, G.2
  • 13
    • 0032066907 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • S. Yantis and J. Jonides, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 10, 601 (1984); E. Weichselgartner and G. Sperling, Science 238, 778 (1987); J. Musseler and G. Aschersleben, Percept. Psychophys. 60, 683 (1998); K. Watanabe and S. Shimojo, Perception 27, 1041 (1998).
    • (1998) Percept. Psychophys. , vol.60 , pp. 683
    • Musseler, J.1    Aschersleben, G.2
  • 14
    • 0032235228 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • S. Yantis and J. Jonides, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 10, 601 (1984); E. Weichselgartner and G. Sperling, Science 238, 778 (1987); J. Musseler and G. Aschersleben, Percept. Psychophys. 60, 683 (1998); K. Watanabe and S. Shimojo, Perception 27, 1041 (1998).
    • (1998) Perception , vol.27 , pp. 1041
    • Watanabe, K.1    Shimojo, S.2
  • 15
    • 0343975782 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Results are the same when the ring appears for only six frames after the appearance of the flash, as opposed to remaining on screen until the end of the trial (n = 2 of the 6 participants).
  • 16
    • 0024432436 scopus 로고
    • R. J. Snowden and O. J. Braddick, Vision Res. 29, 1621 (1989); Vision Res. 31, 907 (1991); B. Krekelberg and M. Lappe, Vision Res. 39, 2669 (1999).
    • (1989) Vision Res. , vol.29 , pp. 1621
    • Snowden, R.J.1    Braddick, O.J.2
  • 17
    • 0026023105 scopus 로고
    • R. J. Snowden and O. J. Braddick, Vision Res. 29, 1621 (1989); Vision Res. 31, 907 (1991); B. Krekelberg and M. Lappe, Vision Res. 39, 2669 (1999).
    • (1991) Vision Res. , vol.31 , pp. 907
  • 18
    • 0032999097 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • R. J. Snowden and O. J. Braddick, Vision Res. 29, 1621 (1989); Vision Res. 31, 907 (1991); B. Krekelberg and M. Lappe, Vision Res. 39, 2669 (1999).
    • (1999) Vision Res. , vol.39 , pp. 2669
    • Krekelberg, B.1    Lappe, M.2
  • 20
    • 0033602375 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A model for the flash-lag effect based on motion extrapolation from retinal mechanisms has been proposed [M. J. Berry II, I. H. Brivanlou, T. A. Jordan, M. Meister, Nature 398, 334 (1999)]; however, that model does not predict our results (for example, compare predictions from our stopped and reversed conditions). Because we propose that motion extrapolation does not explain the flash-tag effect, we do not interpret the retinal data from Berry et al. as directly relevant to the psychophysics. Further, a recent paper shows that the flash-lag illusion is not limited to low-level visual mechanisms but instead integrates other modalities such as the vestibular systems [J. Schlag, R. H. Cai, A. Dorfman, A. Mohempour, M. Schlag-Rey, Nature 403, 38 (2000)]. Such cross-modal interactions may require more processing time, just the sort of need for which a perceptual delay could make allowances.
    • (1999) Nature , vol.398 , pp. 334
    • Berry M.J. II1    Brivanlou, I.H.2    Jordan, T.A.3    Meister, M.4
  • 21
    • 0034610794 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Such cross-modal interactions may require more processing time, just the sort of need for which a perceptual delay could make allowances
    • A model for the flash-lag effect based on motion extrapolation from retinal mechanisms has been proposed [M. J. Berry II, I. H. Brivanlou, T. A. Jordan, M. Meister, Nature 398, 334 (1999)]; however, that model does not predict our results (for example, compare predictions from our stopped and reversed conditions). Because we propose that motion extrapolation does not explain the flash-tag effect, we do not interpret the retinal data from Berry et al. as directly relevant to the psychophysics. Further, a recent paper shows that the flash-lag illusion is not limited to low-level visual mechanisms but instead integrates other modalities such as the vestibular systems [J. Schlag, R. H. Cai, A. Dorfman, A. Mohempour, M. Schlag-Rey, Nature 403, 38 (2000)]. Such cross-modal interactions may require more processing time, just the sort of need for which a perceptual delay could make allowances.
    • (2000) Nature , vol.403 , pp. 38
    • Schlag, J.1    Cai, R.H.2    Dorfman, A.3    Mohempour, A.4    Schlag-Rey, M.5
  • 22
    • 0018352296 scopus 로고
    • B. Libet, E. W. Wright, B. Feinstein, D. K. Pearl, Brain 102, 193 (1979); D. C. Dennet, Consciousness Explained (Little, Brown, Boston, 1992); D. C. Dennet and M. Kinsbourne, Behav. Brain Sci. 15, 183 (1992); M. Kinsbourne, Ciba Found. Symp. 174, 43 (1993); L. Pessoa, E. Thompson, A. Noe, Behav. Brain Sci. 21, 723 (1998).
    • (1979) Brain , vol.102 , pp. 193
    • Libet, B.1    Wright, E.W.2    Feinstein, B.3    Pearl, D.K.4
  • 23
    • 0018352296 scopus 로고
    • Little, Brown, Boston
    • B. Libet, E. W. Wright, B. Feinstein, D. K. Pearl, Brain 102, 193 (1979); D. C. Dennet, Consciousness Explained (Little, Brown, Boston, 1992); D. C. Dennet and M. Kinsbourne, Behav. Brain Sci. 15, 183 (1992); M. Kinsbourne, Ciba Found. Symp. 174, 43 (1993); L. Pessoa, E. Thompson, A. Noe, Behav. Brain Sci. 21, 723 (1998).
    • (1992) Consciousness Explained
    • Dennet, D.C.1
  • 24
    • 0001136358 scopus 로고
    • B. Libet, E. W. Wright, B. Feinstein, D. K. Pearl, Brain 102, 193 (1979); D. C. Dennet, Consciousness Explained (Little, Brown, Boston, 1992); D. C. Dennet and M. Kinsbourne, Behav. Brain Sci. 15, 183 (1992); M. Kinsbourne, Ciba Found. Symp. 174, 43 (1993); L. Pessoa, E. Thompson, A. Noe, Behav. Brain Sci. 21, 723 (1998).
    • (1992) Behav. Brain Sci. , vol.15 , pp. 183
    • Dennet, D.C.1    Kinsbourne, M.2
  • 25
    • 0027345318 scopus 로고
    • B. Libet, E. W. Wright, B. Feinstein, D. K. Pearl, Brain 102, 193 (1979); D. C. Dennet, Consciousness Explained (Little, Brown, Boston, 1992); D. C. Dennet and M. Kinsbourne, Behav. Brain Sci. 15, 183 (1992); M. Kinsbourne, Ciba Found. Symp. 174, 43 (1993); L. Pessoa, E. Thompson, A. Noe, Behav. Brain Sci. 21, 723 (1998).
    • (1993) Ciba Found. Symp. , vol.174 , pp. 43
    • Kinsbourne, M.1
  • 26
    • 0032444477 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • B. Libet, E. W. Wright, B. Feinstein, D. K. Pearl, Brain 102, 193 (1979); D. C. Dennet, Consciousness Explained (Little, Brown, Boston, 1992); D. C. Dennet and M. Kinsbourne, Behav. Brain Sci. 15, 183 (1992); M. Kinsbourne, Ciba Found. Symp. 174, 43 (1993); L. Pessoa, E. Thompson, A. Noe, Behav. Brain Sci. 21, 723 (1998).
    • (1998) Behav. Brain Sci. , vol.21 , pp. 723
    • Pessoa, L.1    Thompson, E.2    Noe, A.3
  • 29
    • 0343975780 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • This method of estimating a value in a time series by using values from the future is known as smoothing in the engineering literature and has recently been appreciated as a useful strategy for many systems (R. P. Rao, D. M. Eagleman, T. J. Sejnowski, in preparation).
  • 31
    • 0343975779 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Demonstrations of the stimuli and supplemental information can be found at www.cnl.salk.edu/ ~eagleman/flashlag.
  • 32
    • 0343539934 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • We thank R. Rao, T. Wachtler, F. Crick, O. Braddick, T. Albright, and A. Messinger for helpful discussions. Supported by the Sloan Center for Theoretical Neurobiology and by an National Institute of Mental Health training grant in cognitive neuroscience.


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.