메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 281, Issue 5381, 1998, Pages 1363-1366

Optimizing gaze control in three dimensions

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords

ARTICLE; COMPUTER SIMULATION; EYE MOVEMENT; GAZE; HEAD MOVEMENT; PRIORITY JOURNAL; RETINA IMAGE; VISUAL FIELD;

EID: 0032575728     PISSN: 00368075     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1126/science.281.5381.1363     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (74)

References (28)
  • 3
    • 0001658292 scopus 로고
    • P. A. Merton, J. Physiol. (Proceedings) 132, 25 (1956); E. F. Miller, Acta Oto-Laryngologica 54, 479 (1962).
    • (1962) Acta Oto-Laryngologica , vol.54 , pp. 479
    • Miller, E.F.1
  • 5
    • 0025060338 scopus 로고
    • D. Tweed and T. Vilis, ibid. 30, 111 (1990); A. W. H. Minken, A. J. Van Opstal, J. A. M. Van Gisbergen, Exp. Brain Res. 93, 521 (1993); D. Tweed, H. Misslisch, M. Fetter, J. Neurophysiol. 72, 1425 (1994); D. Straumann, D. S. Zee, D. Solomon, A. G. Lasker, D. Roberts, Vision Res. 35, 3321 (1995).
    • (1990) Vision Res. , vol.30 , pp. 111
    • Tweed, D.1    Vilis, T.2
  • 6
    • 0027280388 scopus 로고
    • D. Tweed and T. Vilis, ibid. 30, 111 (1990); A. W. H. Minken, A. J. Van Opstal, J. A. M. Van Gisbergen, Exp. Brain Res. 93, 521 (1993); D. Tweed, H. Misslisch, M. Fetter, J. Neurophysiol. 72, 1425 (1994); D. Straumann, D. S. Zee, D. Solomon, A. G. Lasker, D. Roberts, Vision Res. 35, 3321 (1995).
    • (1993) Exp. Brain Res. , vol.93 , pp. 521
    • Minken, A.W.H.1    Van Opstal, A.J.2    Van Gisbergen, J.A.M.3
  • 7
    • 0027990322 scopus 로고
    • D. Tweed and T. Vilis, ibid. 30, 111 (1990); A. W. H. Minken, A. J. Van Opstal, J. A. M. Van Gisbergen, Exp. Brain Res. 93, 521 (1993); D. Tweed, H. Misslisch, M. Fetter, J. Neurophysiol. 72, 1425 (1994); D. Straumann, D. S. Zee, D. Solomon, A. G. Lasker, D. Roberts, Vision Res. 35, 3321 (1995).
    • (1994) J. Neurophysiol. , vol.72 , pp. 1425
    • Tweed, D.1    Misslisch, H.2    Fetter, M.3
  • 8
    • 0028829661 scopus 로고
    • D. Tweed and T. Vilis, ibid. 30, 111 (1990); A. W. H. Minken, A. J. Van Opstal, J. A. M. Van Gisbergen, Exp. Brain Res. 93, 521 (1993); D. Tweed, H. Misslisch, M. Fetter, J. Neurophysiol. 72, 1425 (1994); D. Straumann, D. S. Zee, D. Solomon, A. G. Lasker, D. Roberts, Vision Res. 35, 3321 (1995).
    • (1995) Vision Res. , vol.35 , pp. 3321
    • Straumann, D.1    Zee, D.S.2    Solomon, D.3    Lasker, A.G.4    Roberts, D.5
  • 10
    • 0022557244 scopus 로고
    • A. Roucoux, M. Crommelinck, J. M. Guerit, M. Meulders, in Progress in Oculomotor Research, A. F. Fuchs and W. Becker, Eds. (Elsevier, Amsterdam, 1980), pp. 309-315; V. P. Laurutis and D. A. Robinson, J. Physiol. London 373, 209 (1986).
    • (1986) J. Physiol. London , vol.373 , pp. 209
    • Laurutis, V.P.1    Robinson, D.A.2
  • 11
  • 13
    • 3542995176 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Our four participants were healthy, aged 30 to 38, and gave informed consent. They were instructed to look to the target spot using a fast combined motion of eyes and head, with the head ending up 30° CW or 30° CCW. Similar results were obtained for torsional gaze shifts while looking up and for "natural" gaze shifts, in which the person was given no instructions about final head position but simply looked between four targets - up-right, down-right, down-left, and up-left - at 90° eccentricity. These latter gaze shifts were made in pseudorandom order and followed spoken commands.
  • 14
    • 0344806451 scopus 로고
    • The position of the left eye was recorded at 375 samples per second with the scleral-search-coil technique devised by D. A. Robinson [IEEE Trans. Biomed. Electron. BME-10, 137 (1963)]. Head position was recorded at the same rate with the use of search coils fastened to a lightweight bar molded to fit the person's teeth. Electrical signals from the eye and head search coils were converted to quaternions with the use of algorithms from D. Tweed, W. Cadera, and T. Vilis [Vision Res. 30, 97 (1990)]. For an oculomotor introduction to quaternions, see G. Westheimer, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 47, 967(1957); D. Tweed and T. Vilis, J. Neurophysiol. 58, 832 (1987); or T. Haslwanter, Vision Res. 35, 1727 (1995).
    • (1963) IEEE Trans. Biomed. Electron. BME-10 , pp. 137
    • Robinson, D.A.1
  • 15
    • 0025013006 scopus 로고
    • The position of the left eye was recorded at 375 samples per second with the scleral-search-coil technique devised by D. A. Robinson [IEEE Trans. Biomed. Electron. BME-10, 137 (1963)]. Head position was recorded at the same rate with the use of search coils fastened to a lightweight bar molded to fit the person's teeth. Electrical signals from the eye and head search coils were converted to quaternions with the use of algorithms from D. Tweed, W. Cadera, and T. Vilis [Vision Res. 30, 97 (1990)]. For an oculomotor introduction to quaternions, see G. Westheimer, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 47, 967(1957); D. Tweed and T. Vilis, J. Neurophysiol. 58, 832 (1987); or T. Haslwanter, Vision Res. 35, 1727 (1995).
    • (1990) Vision Res. , vol.30 , pp. 97
    • Tweed, D.1    Cadera, W.2    Vilis, T.3
  • 16
    • 0001565660 scopus 로고
    • The position of the left eye was recorded at 375 samples per second with the scleral-search-coil technique devised by D. A. Robinson [IEEE Trans. Biomed. Electron. BME-10, 137 (1963)]. Head position was recorded at the same rate with the use of search coils fastened to a lightweight bar molded to fit the person's teeth. Electrical signals from the eye and head search coils were converted to quaternions with the use of algorithms from D. Tweed, W. Cadera, and T. Vilis [Vision Res. 30, 97 (1990)]. For an oculomotor introduction to quaternions, see G. Westheimer, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 47, 967(1957); D. Tweed and T. Vilis, J. Neurophysiol. 58, 832 (1987); or T. Haslwanter, Vision Res. 35, 1727 (1995).
    • (1957) J. Opt. Soc. Am. , vol.47 , pp. 967
    • Westheimer, G.1
  • 17
    • 0023513576 scopus 로고
    • The position of the left eye was recorded at 375 samples per second with the scleral-search-coil technique devised by D. A. Robinson [IEEE Trans. Biomed. Electron. BME-10, 137 (1963)]. Head position was recorded at the same rate with the use of search coils fastened to a lightweight bar molded to fit the person's teeth. Electrical signals from the eye and head search coils were converted to quaternions with the use of algorithms from D. Tweed, W. Cadera, and T. Vilis [Vision Res. 30, 97 (1990)]. For an oculomotor introduction to quaternions, see G. Westheimer, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 47, 967(1957); D. Tweed and T. Vilis, J. Neurophysiol. 58, 832 (1987); or T. Haslwanter, Vision Res. 35, 1727 (1995).
    • (1987) J. Neurophysiol. , vol.58 , pp. 832
    • Tweed, D.1    Vilis, T.2
  • 18
    • 0029022686 scopus 로고
    • The position of the left eye was recorded at 375 samples per second with the scleral-search-coil technique devised by D. A. Robinson [IEEE Trans. Biomed. Electron. BME-10, 137 (1963)]. Head position was recorded at the same rate with the use of search coils fastened to a lightweight bar molded to fit the person's teeth. Electrical signals from the eye and head search coils were converted to quaternions with the use of algorithms from D. Tweed, W. Cadera, and T. Vilis [Vision Res. 30, 97 (1990)]. For an oculomotor introduction to quaternions, see G. Westheimer, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 47, 967(1957); D. Tweed and T. Vilis, J. Neurophysiol. 58, 832 (1987); or T. Haslwanter, Vision Res. 35, 1727 (1995).
    • (1995) Vision Res. , vol.35 , pp. 1727
    • Haslwanter, T.1
  • 19
    • 0015767487 scopus 로고
    • Our best performing participant showed a range of 16° CCW to 18° CW, tying the world record for human ocular torsion. A. P. Petrov and G. M. Zenkin [Vision Res. 13, 2465 (1973)] also had a study participant who reached 18°, but by a different mechanism: Their participant was rotated torsionally in the dark; the eye movements were not gaze shifts to visual targets but vestibular responses to the head rotation, and they opposed the head's motion; whereas in our study the eye rolled the same way as the head and before it. R. Balliet and K. Nakayama [Invest. Ophthalmol. 17, 303 (1978)] trained people to rotate their eyes torsionally while keeping their heads still. The training, which involved torsional visual feedback, allowed them to increase their torsional range by about 0.8°/hour, so that after 35 hours of training, the best performing person could move over a range of 26.5°. That result showed that the oculomotor system has torsional plasticity, whereas the present findings show that circuits for torsional gaze control are active in normal untrained humans. Our participants practiced the gaze task in Fig. 1 at most five times (that is, for only a few seconds) before recording began, and they showed the full torsional range of eye motion, or very close to it, on the first recorded trial.
    • (1973) Vision Res. , vol.13 , pp. 2465
    • Petrov, A.P.1    Zenkin, G.M.2
  • 20
    • 0017842262 scopus 로고
    • Our best performing participant showed a range of 16° CCW to 18° CW, tying the world record for human ocular torsion. A. P. Petrov and G. M. Zenkin [Vision Res. 13, 2465 (1973)] also had a study participant who reached 18°, but by a different mechanism: Their participant was rotated torsionally in the dark; the eye movements were not gaze shifts to visual targets but vestibular responses to the head rotation, and they opposed the head's motion; whereas in our study the eye rolled the same way as the head and before it. R. Balliet and K. Nakayama [Invest. Ophthalmol. 17, 303 (1978)] trained people to rotate their eyes torsionally while keeping their heads still. The training, which involved torsional visual feedback, allowed them to increase their torsional range by about 0.8°/hour, so that after 35 hours of training, the best performing person could move over a range of 26.5°. That result showed that the oculomotor system has torsional plasticity, whereas the present findings show that circuits for torsional gaze control are active in normal untrained humans. Our participants practiced the gaze task in Fig. 1 at most five times (that is, for only a few seconds) before recording began, and they showed the full torsional range of eye motion, or very close to it, on the first recorded trial.
    • (1978) Invest. Ophthalmol. , vol.17 , pp. 303
    • Balliet, R.1    Nakayama, K.2
  • 21
    • 3543042580 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • The eye's return motion, when it came back toward Listing's plane while staying locked on the visual target, likely was vestibularly driven; but the initial outbound torsion, which anticipated the upcoming head turn, was not. In the outbound stage, the three-dimensional gaze system rotated the eye to its desired position in space and then locked on by activating the vestibuloocular reflex. This reflex uses vestibular information to monitor head velocity and counterrotate the eye, holding it steady in space as the head completes its motion (5, 6).
  • 23
    • 0031033512 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • For model equations and predictions see D. Tweed, ibid. 77, 654 (1997). In this model, eye and head are driven by two separate feedback loops to goal positions defined in space-fixed coordinates.
    • (1997) J. Neurophysiol. , vol.77 , pp. 654
    • Tweed, D.1
  • 24
    • 0004262763 scopus 로고
    • Pion, London
    • The loss of acuity caused by horizontal and vertical image slip depends on the spatial frequency of the image, which means that regions with a lot of fine detail are usually degraded more than coarse patches; see R. H. S. Carpenter, The Movements of the Eyes (Pion, London, 1988). Something analogous likely holds for the effects of torsional image slip on orientation detectors; radially oriented contours are degraded more than circumferential contours, so that the spokes of a spinning wheel are hard to make out, while the rim is clear.
    • (1988) The Movements of the Eyes
    • Carpenter, R.H.S.1
  • 25
    • 0026512482 scopus 로고
    • J.-R. Duhamel, C. Colby, and M. Goldberg [Science 255, 90 (1992)] reported that some visual cells in the parietal cortex adjust their receptive fields in anticipation of gaze shifts. Such cells will, for example, shift their receptive fields to the right immediately before rightward eye movements. Based on our results, one might also expect to find cells that adjust their receptive fields in the torsional dimension. For instance, an orientation-sensitive cell that normally responds best to horizontal lines may prefer CW-rotated lines in the final milliseconds before a CW gaze shift.
    • (1992) Science , vol.255 , pp. 90
    • Duhamel, J.-R.1    Colby, C.2    Goldberg, M.3
  • 26
    • 0028812684 scopus 로고
    • Advantages of rezeroing torsion are that the eye muscles probably require less force to hold an eye position near Listing's plane and that the eye, at the center of its torsional range, is optimally placed for the next gaze shift, which may go either CW or CCW [see K. Hepp. Vision Res. 35, 3237 (1995) and D. Tweed, ibid. 37, 1939 (1997)].
    • (1995) Vision Res. , vol.35 , pp. 3237
    • Hepp, K.1
  • 27
    • 0030789591 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Advantages of rezeroing torsion are that the eye muscles probably require less force to hold an eye position near Listing's plane and that the eye, at the center of its torsional range, is optimally placed for the next gaze shift, which may go either CW or CCW [see K. Hepp. Vision Res. 35, 3237 (1995) and D. Tweed, ibid. 37, 1939 (1997)].
    • (1997) Vision Res. , vol.37 , pp. 1939
    • Tweed, D.1
  • 28
    • 3542999977 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • for comments on the manuscript
    • We thank J. Dichgans, S. Ferber, V. Happe, C. Hawkins, M. Niemeier, and T. Vilis for comments on the manuscript. This work was supported by the Medical Research Council of Canada (grant MT-12847) and the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (grant SFB 307-A10).
    • Dichgans, J.1    Ferber, S.2    Happe, V.3    Hawkins, C.4    Niemeier, M.5    Vilis, T.6


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.