메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 279, Issue 5347, 1998, Pages 91-95

Dissociated pattern of activity in visual cortices and their projections during human rapid eye movement sleep

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords

OXYGEN 15;

EID: 0032472267     PISSN: 00368075     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1126/science.279.5347.91     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (414)

References (52)
  • 3
    • 0028813647 scopus 로고
    • Some studies suggest that activation of only the higher levels of the visual processing hierarchy is sufficient to evoke and sustain visual imagery [P. E. Roland and B. Gulyas, Cereb. Cortex 5, 79 (1995)].
    • (1995) Cereb. Cortex , vol.5 , pp. 79
    • Roland, P.E.1    Gulyas, B.2
  • 4
    • 0028867946 scopus 로고
    • Others [S. M. Kosslyn, W. L. Thompson, I. J. Kim, N. M. Alpert, Nature 378, 496 (1995)] indicate that contribution of the primary visual processing areas - possibly activated through reentrant pathways [D. J. Felleman and D. C. Van Essen, Cereb. Cortex 1, 1 (1991)] - is also required.
    • (1995) Nature , vol.378 , pp. 496
    • Kosslyn, S.M.1    Thompson, W.L.2    Kim, I.J.3    Alpert, N.M.4
  • 5
    • 0025718412 scopus 로고
    • Others [S. M. Kosslyn, W. L. Thompson, I. J. Kim, N. M. Alpert, Nature 378, 496 (1995)] indicate that contribution of the primary visual processing areas - possibly activated through reentrant pathways [D. J. Felleman and D. C. Van Essen, Cereb. Cortex 1, 1 (1991)] - is also required.
    • (1991) Cereb. Cortex , vol.1 , pp. 1
    • Felleman, D.J.1    Van Essen, D.C.2
  • 7
    • 14444282002 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Informed consent was obtained from 10 healthy male volunteers (23.6 ± 1.7 years of age, mean ± SD; range, 22 to 27 years). Eight subjects were right-handed, and two were left-handed.
  • 8
    • 14444271806 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • There were 8 hours of sleep with selective REM deprivation on day 1 ; 2 hours of sleep with REM deprivation on day 2; and 15.5 to 23.5 hours of continuous wakefulness (22.2 ± 2.4 hours, mean ± SD) on day 3. Subjects were monitored polysomnographically throughout this period to verify the absence of sleep and facilitate selective REM deprivation.
  • 9
    • 14444271087 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • PET scans were performed on a Scanditronix PC2048-15B tomograph (Uppsala, Sweden), which acquires 15 contiguous planes. A transmission scan was performed for attenuation correction.
  • 10
    • 0003409637 scopus 로고
    • Government Printing Office, Washington, DC
    • 15O were injected and scans were initiated automatically. For inclusion in these analyses, a sleep stage had to be uniformly maintained (free of arousals and stage shifts) from 40 s before to 40 s after scan initiation. It was necessary to use partial sleep-deprivation methods to help ensure that subjects would sleep in the scanner. Such procedures, however, may affect sleep architecture (that is, the relative amounts and timing of the various stages within the sleep period). To evaluate these potential effects, we compared our subjects' sleep architecture with published normative data [R. L. Williams, I. Karacan, C. J. Hursch, Electroencephalography (EEG) of Human Sleep: Clinical Applications (Wiley, New York, 1974)]. Except for increased SWS time during the first third of the sleep period (an expected consequence of the deprivation procedure), we found no qualitative or quantitative abnormalities in the sleep architecture of our subjects. Furthermore, the scans selected for analysis were, in each instance, associated with sleep samples that conformed explicitly and unambiguously to the standard scoring criteria of Rechtschaffen and Kales throughout the period of data acquisition. Therefore, although sleep deprivation may have affected duration and timing of sleep stages, the procedure produced no observable effects on the physiological characteristics of the sleep stages themselves (that is, at the level of analysis most critical to this study).
    • (1968) A Manual of Standardized Terminology, Techniques and Scoring System for Sleep Stages of Human Subjects
    • Rechtschaffen, A.1    Kales, A.E.2
  • 11
    • 0003855652 scopus 로고
    • Wiley, New York
    • 15O were injected and scans were initiated automatically. For inclusion in these analyses, a sleep stage had to be uniformly maintained (free of arousals and stage shifts) from 40 s before to 40 s after scan initiation. It was necessary to use partial sleep-deprivation methods to help ensure that subjects would sleep in the scanner. Such procedures, however, may affect sleep architecture (that is, the relative amounts and timing of the various stages within the sleep period). To evaluate these potential effects, we compared our subjects' sleep architecture with published normative data [R. L. Williams, I. Karacan, C. J. Hursch, Electroencephalography (EEG) of Human Sleep: Clinical Applications (Wiley, New York, 1974)]. Except for increased SWS time during the first third of the sleep period (an expected consequence of the deprivation procedure), we found no qualitative or quantitative abnormalities in the sleep architecture of our subjects. Furthermore, the scans selected for analysis were, in each instance, associated with sleep samples that conformed explicitly and unambiguously to the standard scoring criteria of Rechtschaffen and Kales throughout the period of data acquisition. Therefore, although sleep deprivation may have affected duration and timing of sleep stages, the procedure produced no observable effects on the physiological characteristics of the sleep stages themselves (that is, at the level of analysis most critical to this study).
    • (1974) Electroencephalography (EEG) of Human Sleep: Clinical Applications
    • Williams, R.L.1    Karacan, I.2    Hursch, C.J.3
  • 12
    • 0003415335 scopus 로고
    • Thieme, New York
    • 2 values. Of the 10 subjects in whom REM sleep scans were acquired, 7 had SWS scans, and 7 had waking postsleep studies. Stage contrasts were performed in a repeated measures design; thus, N = 7 for both REM-waking and REM-SWS contrasts. To evaluate hemispheral effects, differences in normalized rCBF for each contrast were compared between homologous pixels in right and left hemispheres by using a voxel-wise error variance; differences exceeding a Z-threshold of 3.09 were considered significant.
    • (1988) Co-planar Stereotactic Atlas of the Human Brain
    • Talairach, J.1    Tournoux, P.2
  • 13
    • 0025773346 scopus 로고
    • 2 values. Of the 10 subjects in whom REM sleep scans were acquired, 7 had SWS scans, and 7 had waking postsleep studies. Stage contrasts were performed in a repeated measures design; thus, N = 7 for both REM-waking and REM-SWS contrasts. To evaluate hemispheral effects, differences in normalized rCBF for each contrast were compared between homologous pixels in right and left hemispheres by using a voxel-wise error variance; differences exceeding a Z-threshold of 3.09 were considered significant.
    • (1991) J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. , vol.11 , pp. 690
    • Friston, K.J.1    Frith, C.D.2    Liddle, P.F.3    Frackowiak, R.S.J.4
  • 14
    • 12344294700 scopus 로고
    • 2 values. Of the 10 subjects in whom REM sleep scans were acquired, 7 had SWS scans, and 7 had waking postsleep studies. Stage contrasts were performed in a repeated measures design; thus, N = 7 for both REM-waking and REM-SWS contrasts. To evaluate hemispheral effects, differences in normalized rCBF for each contrast were compared between homologous pixels in right and left hemispheres by using a voxel-wise error variance; differences exceeding a Z-threshold of 3.09 were considered significant.
    • (1994) Hum. Brain Mapp. , vol.1 , pp. 210
    • Friston, K.J.1    Worsley, K.J.2    Frackowiak, R.S.J.3    Mazziotta, J.C.4    Evans, A.C.5
  • 15
    • 14444284346 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • The frequency of rapid conjugate eye movements, identified as positive or negative deflections on the electrooculogram (EOG) during the 180 s after the start of the scan, were computed. Normalized rCBF images were correlated with each subject's REM counts on a pixel-by-pixel basis (N = 10 for these analyses) (Fig. 1C). Correlation coefficients were thresholded at a level of 0.6 (P < 0.05, n = 10) for tabulation (Table 1).
  • 16
    • 0003843310 scopus 로고
    • Chapman & Hall, New York
    • Coordinates for 20 visual cortical areas in each hemisphere (7 striate, 6 lateral occipital, 7 fusiform/infero-temporal) were selected by using the Talairach atlas, adjusted with the SPM PET template, and used to extract rCBF values from the stereotaxically normalized PET images. These values were normalized by using mean CBF rates for the visual cortex as a whole; rCBF values in homologous regions of right and left hemispheres were averaged and used to generate difference matrices (REM-SWS and REM-wake; N = 7); a principal component analysis was then applied. Significance of the extracted component vector was assessed by using permutation tests [B. F. J. Manly, Randomization and Monte Carlo Methods in Biology (Chapman & Hall, New York, 1990)] by randomly permuting the sign of data entries 19 times; P values were derived by determining the proportion of permuted trials with higher first eigenvalues than the original data. Stability of regional loadings was assessed by the bootstrap procedure [B. Efron, SIAM Monogr. 38-1 (1982)]. Fit values of the original loadings across 10,000 bootstrap samples, expressed as Z-scores, were calculated; absolute values > 2.4 were considered stable.
    • (1990) Randomization and Monte Carlo Methods in Biology
    • Manly, B.F.J.1
  • 17
    • 14444283123 scopus 로고
    • Coordinates for 20 visual cortical areas in each hemisphere (7 striate, 6 lateral occipital, 7 fusiform/infero-temporal) were selected by using the Talairach atlas, adjusted with the SPM PET template, and used to extract rCBF values from the stereotaxically normalized PET images. These values were normalized by using mean CBF rates for the visual cortex as a whole; rCBF values in homologous regions of right and left hemispheres were averaged and used to generate difference matrices (REM-SWS and REM-wake; N = 7); a principal component analysis was then applied. Significance of the extracted component vector was assessed by using permutation tests [B. F. J. Manly, Randomization and Monte Carlo Methods in Biology (Chapman & Hall, New York, 1990)] by randomly permuting the sign of data entries 19 times; P values were derived by determining the proportion of permuted trials with higher first eigenvalues than the original data. Stability of regional loadings was assessed by the bootstrap procedure [B. Efron, SIAM Monogr. 38-1 (1982)]. Fit values of the original loadings across 10,000 bootstrap samples, expressed as Z-scores, were calculated; absolute values > 2.4 were considered stable.
    • (1982) SIAM Monogr. , vol.38 , Issue.1
    • Efron, B.1
  • 18
    • 14444269680 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • -7; centered x = 21.9, y = -51.8, z = -6.8.
  • 21
    • 14444287520 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • -6; centered x = 2.8, y = -83.0, z = 0.4.
  • 22
    • 14444276276 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • 2 corrected flow rates were consistently lower during REM sleep than during SWS throughout the striate cortex [ΔrCBF = -6.40 ± 5.9 ml/100 g/min (mean ± SD), x = 4, y = -82, z = 0], and absolute rCBF rates were consistently elevated in extrastriate regions (in the fusiform cortex, ΔrCBF = +8.85 ± 2.79 ml/100g/min, x = -34, y = -56, z = -8), although variances in absolute values were large.
  • 23
    • 14444276054 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • max > k), corrected = 3.55E - 15; centered x = -3.9, y = 0.3, z = 1.2.
  • 25
    • 0027938818 scopus 로고
    • D. A. Chavis and D. N. Pandya, Trans. Am. Neurol. Assoc. 99, 192 (1974); W. A. Suzuki and D. G. Amaral, J. Comp. Neurol. 350, 497 (1994).
    • (1994) J. Comp. Neurol. , vol.350 , pp. 497
    • Suzuki, W.A.1    Amaral, D.G.2
  • 26
    • 14444283603 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • The frontal eye fields (superior dorsolateral prefrontal and premotor cortices) were incompletely sampled in many subjects and are not included in this analysis.
  • 27
    • 14444277010 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • The bootstrapping procedures indicated that most regional loadings were stable. In the REM-SWS contrast, all the fusiform-interotemporal regions - but none of the lateral occipital regions - were associated with Z-scores exceeding threshold (local maximum = 5.44, x = ±48, y = -62, z = 0). In the REM-wake contrast, six of the seven fusiform-inferotemporal regions (maximum 4.18, x = ±48, y = -62, z = 4), and four of the six lateral occipital regions (maximum 3.58, x = ±32, y = -86, z = 0) were stable; in the latter case, all were located in ventral rather than dorsal portions of the lateral occipital cortex (z ≤ 8 mm). In the REM-SWS contrast, five of the seven striate loadings were stable (local minimum = -3.79, x = ±4, y = -82, z = 0), and in the REM-wake contrast, six of seven loadings were stable (minimum = -9.89, x = ±4, y = -82, z = 0).
  • 28
    • 14444273885 scopus 로고
    • S. Zela et al., J. Neurosci. 11, 641 (1991); D. Watson et al., Cereb. Cortex 3, 79 (1993); J. V. Haxby et al., J. Neurosci. 14, 6336 (1994); I. Sereno et al., Science 268, 889 (1995).
    • (1991) J. Neurosci. , vol.11 , pp. 641
    • Zela, S.1
  • 29
    • 0027460558 scopus 로고
    • S. Zela et al., J. Neurosci. 11, 641 (1991); D. Watson et al., Cereb. Cortex 3, 79 (1993); J. V. Haxby et al., J. Neurosci. 14, 6336 (1994); I. Sereno et al., Science 268, 889 (1995).
    • (1993) Cereb. Cortex , vol.3 , pp. 79
    • Watson, D.1
  • 30
    • 0028125345 scopus 로고
    • S. Zela et al., J. Neurosci. 11, 641 (1991); D. Watson et al., Cereb. Cortex 3, 79 (1993); J. V. Haxby et al., J. Neurosci. 14, 6336 (1994); I. Sereno et al., Science 268, 889 (1995).
    • (1994) J. Neurosci. , vol.14 , pp. 6336
    • Haxby, J.V.1
  • 31
    • 0029018377 scopus 로고
    • S. Zela et al., J. Neurosci. 11, 641 (1991); D. Watson et al., Cereb. Cortex 3, 79 (1993); J. V. Haxby et al., J. Neurosci. 14, 6336 (1994); I. Sereno et al., Science 268, 889 (1995).
    • (1995) Science , vol.268 , pp. 889
    • Sereno, I.1
  • 32
    • 0028874363 scopus 로고
    • These results could reflect the central effects of saccadic eye movements per se. Voluntary saccades during wakefulness previously have been found to be associated with reductions in visual CBF [T. Paus, S. Marrett, K. J. Worsley, A. C. Evans, J. Neurophysiol. 74, 2179 (1995)]. However, saccades in that study were continuous and rapid (40 to 140 in 60 s) but were intermittent and lower in frequency in our own study [0 to 11 in 60 s (5.5 ± 4.0, mean ± SD]. Furthermore, reductions in rCBF were more widely distributed throughout the visual cortex, and no positive correlations between extrastriate activity and saccadic eye movements were observed, which suggests that the striate-extrastriate dissociation evident in this study may be a unique characteristic of REM sleep.
    • (1995) J. Neurophysiol. , vol.74 , pp. 2179
    • Paus, T.1    Marrett, S.2    Worsley, K.J.3    Evans, A.C.4
  • 33
    • 0020556494 scopus 로고
    • R. W. McCarley, J. W. Winkelman, F. H. Duffy, Brain Res. 274, 359 (1983); C. W. Callaway, R. Lydic, H. A. Baghdoyan, J. A. Hobson, Cell Mol. Neurobiol. 7, 105 (1987).
    • (1983) Brain Res. , vol.274 , pp. 359
    • McCarley, R.W.1    Winkelman, J.W.2    Duffy, F.H.3
  • 35
    • 0014559875 scopus 로고
    • The effects of the PGO wave generator on the occipital cortex do not appear to be mediated through the lateral geniculate nucleus [J. A. Hobson, J. Alexander, C. J. Frederickson, Brain Res. 14, 607 (1969)] and therefore are not expected to be exclusively associated with striate projections. Early studies also demonstrated that PGO wave amplitudes in the lateral association areas of the cat visual cortex exceed those in primary visual cortices [D. C. Brooks, Exp. Neurol. 22, 603 (1968)], and single unit studies have shown that the facilitatory effects of PGO waves on neuronal firing rates appear to be manifest in extrastriate but not in striate cortices [T. Kasamatsu and W. R. Adey, Brain Res. 55, 323 (1973)].
    • (1969) Brain Res. , vol.14 , pp. 607
    • Hobson, J.A.1    Alexander, J.2    Frederickson, C.J.3
  • 36
    • 0014383786 scopus 로고
    • The effects of the PGO wave generator on the occipital cortex do not appear to be mediated through the lateral geniculate nucleus [J. A. Hobson, J. Alexander, C. J. Frederickson, Brain Res. 14, 607 (1969)] and therefore are not expected to be exclusively associated with striate projections. Early studies also demonstrated that PGO wave amplitudes in the lateral association areas of the cat visual cortex exceed those in primary visual cortices [D. C. Brooks, Exp. Neurol. 22, 603 (1968)], and single unit studies have shown that the facilitatory effects of PGO waves on neuronal firing rates appear to be manifest in extrastriate but not in striate cortices [T. Kasamatsu and W. R. Adey, Brain Res. 55, 323 (1973)].
    • (1968) Exp. Neurol. , vol.22 , pp. 603
    • Brooks, D.C.1
  • 37
    • 0015904195 scopus 로고
    • The effects of the PGO wave generator on the occipital cortex do not appear to be mediated through the lateral geniculate nucleus [J. A. Hobson, J. Alexander, C. J. Frederickson, Brain Res. 14, 607 (1969)] and therefore are not expected to be exclusively associated with striate projections. Early studies also demonstrated that PGO wave amplitudes in the lateral association areas of the cat visual cortex exceed those in primary visual cortices [D. C. Brooks, Exp. Neurol. 22, 603 (1968)], and single unit studies have shown that the facilitatory effects of PGO waves on neuronal firing rates appear to be manifest in extrastriate but not in striate cortices [T. Kasamatsu and W. R. Adey, Brain Res. 55, 323 (1973)].
    • (1973) Brain Res. , vol.55 , pp. 323
    • Kasamatsu, T.1    Adey, W.R.2
  • 38
    • 0001116040 scopus 로고
    • D. R. Goodenough, H. B. Lewis, A. Shapiro, I. Sleser, J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 140, 365 (1965); P. Verdone, Percept. Mot. Skills 20, 1253 (1965); T. Pivik and D. Foulkes, Science 153, 1282 (1966); W. Dement and E. A. Wolpert, J. Exp. Psychol. 55, 543 (1958). See also (2, 14).
    • (1965) J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. , vol.140 , pp. 365
    • Goodenough, D.R.1    Lewis, H.B.2    Shapiro, A.3    Sleser, I.4
  • 39
    • 0000753491 scopus 로고
    • D. R. Goodenough, H. B. Lewis, A. Shapiro, I. Sleser, J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 140, 365 (1965); P. Verdone, Percept. Mot. Skills 20, 1253 (1965); T. Pivik and D. Foulkes, Science 153, 1282 (1966); W. Dement and E. A. Wolpert, J. Exp. Psychol. 55, 543 (1958). See also (2, 14).
    • (1965) Percept. Mot. Skills , vol.20 , pp. 1253
    • Verdone, P.1
  • 40
    • 0014009863 scopus 로고
    • D. R. Goodenough, H. B. Lewis, A. Shapiro, I. Sleser, J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 140, 365 (1965); P. Verdone, Percept. Mot. Skills 20, 1253 (1965); T. Pivik and D. Foulkes, Science 153, 1282 (1966); W. Dement and E. A. Wolpert, J. Exp. Psychol. 55, 543 (1958). See also (2, 14).
    • (1966) Science , vol.153 , pp. 1282
    • Pivik, T.1    Foulkes, D.2
  • 41
    • 58149425740 scopus 로고
    • See also (2, 14)
    • D. R. Goodenough, H. B. Lewis, A. Shapiro, I. Sleser, J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 140, 365 (1965); P. Verdone, Percept. Mot. Skills 20, 1253 (1965); T. Pivik and D. Foulkes, Science 153, 1282 (1966); W. Dement and E. A. Wolpert, J. Exp. Psychol. 55, 543 (1958). See also (2, 14).
    • (1958) J. Exp. Psychol. , vol.55 , pp. 543
    • Dement, W.1    Wolpert, E.A.2
  • 42
    • 14444278750 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • This appears to be consistent with the finding of Roland and Gulyas (3). However, our results may not be directly comparable with studies of conscious visual imagery; task-elicited activation of the primary visual cortex seen by Kosslyn et al. (4) might be a special feature of conscious visual imagery or retrieval processes that occur during the waking state.
  • 43
    • 0029828471 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • P. Maquet et al. [Nature 383, 163 (1996)] did not list rCBF changes in visual cortices; however, they compared REM sleep with an amalgam of wake and non-REM stages, whereas our contrasts were stage specific (for example, REM-SWS) and may be more sensitive.
    • (1996) Nature , vol.383 , pp. 163
    • Maquet, P.1
  • 44
    • 0025827911 scopus 로고
    • P. L. Madsen et al. [J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 11, 502 (1991)] reported REM-associated increases in rCBF in visual association cortices; functional relationships between activity in striate and extrastriate cortices were not evaluated.
    • (1991) J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. , vol.11 , pp. 502
    • Madsen, P.L.1
  • 45
    • 0029114170 scopus 로고
    • C. C. Hong et al. [Sleep 18, 570 (1995)] reported a positive correlation between REM density and glucose metabolic rates in lateral occipital cortex, but results for primary visual cortex were not reported.
    • (1995) Sleep , vol.18 , pp. 570
    • Hong, C.C.1
  • 46
    • 0004127556 scopus 로고
    • Blackwell, Oxford, See also (3, 4)
    • S. Zeki, A Vision of the Brain (Blackwell, Oxford, 1993). See also (3, 4).
    • (1993) A Vision of the Brain
    • Zeki, S.1
  • 47
    • 0029072740 scopus 로고
    • A pattern of extrastriate activity in the absence of striate function also characterizes clinical states associated with unusual and often bizarre perturbations of visual awareness, such as blindsight [L. Weiskrantz, J. L. Barbur, A. Sahraie, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 92, 6122 (1995)] and confabulatory denial of blindness [G. Goldenberg, W. Mullbacher, A. Nowak, Neuropsychologia 33, 1373 (1995)]. The features of both syndromes suggest that synchronous activation of primary and extrastriate cortices may be essential for "normal" visual awareness. That such coherence appears to be breached during REM sleep is not inconsistent with the altered awareness that characterizes this sleep stage.
    • (1995) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. , vol.92 , pp. 6122
    • Weiskrantz, L.1    Barbur, J.L.2    Sahraie, A.3
  • 48
    • 0028789776 scopus 로고
    • A pattern of extrastriate activity in the absence of striate function also characterizes clinical states associated with unusual and often bizarre perturbations of visual awareness, such as blindsight [L. Weiskrantz, J. L. Barbur, A. Sahraie, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 92, 6122 (1995)] and confabulatory denial of blindness [G. Goldenberg, W. Mullbacher, A. Nowak, Neuropsychologia 33, 1373 (1995)]. The features of both syndromes suggest that synchronous activation of primary and extrastriate cortices may be essential for "normal" visual awareness. That such coherence appears to be breached during REM sleep is not inconsistent with the altered awareness that characterizes this sleep stage.
    • (1995) Neuropsychologia , vol.33 , pp. 1373
    • Goldenberg, G.1    Mullbacher, W.2    Nowak, A.3
  • 49
    • 14444272524 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Decreases in activity in the prefrontal cortex during REM sleep have been noted previously by Madsen et al. (29) and by Maquet et al. (28). On the other hand, Hong et al. (30) reported positive correlations between REM density and absolute glucose metabolic rates in the (right) dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; however, this measure, which reflects physiological responses over a longer time period, may not be directly comparable with normalized rCBF values evaluated in this study.
  • 50
    • 0018225935 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A. Rechtschaffen, Sleep 1, 97 (1978); J. A. Hobson, Endeavour 20, 86 (1996).
    • (1978) Sleep , vol.1 , pp. 97
    • Rechtschaffen, A.1
  • 51
    • 0018225935 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • A. Rechtschaffen, Sleep 1, 97 (1978); J. A. Hobson, Endeavour 20, 86 (1996).
    • (1996) Endeavour , vol.20 , pp. 86
    • Hobson, J.A.1
  • 52
    • 14444284345 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • The authors wish to thank Dr. Alex Martin for his expertise, insight, and critical suggestions, which were essential in the preparation of this manuscript, and Dr. Scott Selbie for his valuable help in preparing Fig. 1.


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.