메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 275, Issue 5301, 1997, Pages 835-838

Two modulatory effects of attention that mediate object categorization in human cortex

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords

ARTICLE; ATTENTION; BRAIN CORTEX; CEREBELLUM; NEUROMODULATION; PREMOTOR CORTEX; PRIORITY JOURNAL; REGULATORY MECHANISM; VISUAL STIMULATION;

EID: 0031024562     PISSN: 00368075     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1126/science.275.5301.835     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (147)

References (58)
  • 1
    • 0003891643 scopus 로고
    • Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, MA
    • W. James, The Principles of Psychology (Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, MA, 1976); M. I. Posner et al., Psychobiology 15, 107 (1987); J. Duncan, J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 113, 501 (1984); S. P. Vecera and M. J. Farah, ibid. 123, 146 (1994); M. I. Posner and S. Dehaene, Trends Neurosci. 17, 75 (1994).
    • (1976) The Principles of Psychology
    • James, W.1
  • 2
    • 80053504802 scopus 로고
    • W. James, The Principles of Psychology (Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, MA, 1976); M. I. Posner et al., Psychobiology 15, 107 (1987); J. Duncan, J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 113, 501 (1984); S. P. Vecera and M. J. Farah, ibid. 123, 146 (1994); M. I. Posner and S. Dehaene, Trends Neurosci. 17, 75 (1994).
    • (1987) Psychobiology , vol.15 , pp. 107
    • Posner, M.I.1
  • 3
    • 0021696833 scopus 로고
    • W. James, The Principles of Psychology (Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, MA, 1976); M. I. Posner et al., Psychobiology 15, 107 (1987); J. Duncan, J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 113, 501 (1984); S. P. Vecera and M. J. Farah, ibid. 123, 146 (1994); M. I. Posner and S. Dehaene, Trends Neurosci. 17, 75 (1994).
    • (1984) J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. , vol.113 , pp. 501
    • Duncan, J.1
  • 4
    • 0028453988 scopus 로고
    • W. James, The Principles of Psychology (Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, MA, 1976); M. I. Posner et al., Psychobiology 15, 107 (1987); J. Duncan, J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 113, 501 (1984); S. P. Vecera and M. J. Farah, ibid. 123, 146 (1994); M. I. Posner and S. Dehaene, Trends Neurosci. 17, 75 (1994).
    • (1994) J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. , vol.123 , pp. 146
    • Vecera, S.P.1    Farah, M.J.2
  • 5
    • 0028054928 scopus 로고
    • W. James, The Principles of Psychology (Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, MA, 1976); M. I. Posner et al., Psychobiology 15, 107 (1987); J. Duncan, J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 113, 501 (1984); S. P. Vecera and M. J. Farah, ibid. 123, 146 (1994); M. I. Posner and S. Dehaene, Trends Neurosci. 17, 75 (1994).
    • (1994) Trends Neurosci. , vol.17 , pp. 75
    • Posner, M.I.1    Dehaene, S.2
  • 6
    • 0003843553 scopus 로고
    • Pergamon, London
    • D. E. Broadbent, Perception and Communication (Pergamon, London, 1958); U. Neisser, Cognitive Psychology (Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, 1967); M. R. Harter and C. J. Aine, in Varieties of Attention, R. Parasuraman and D. R. Davies, Eds. (Academic Press, Orlando, FL, 1984), pp. 293-321.
    • (1958) Perception and Communication
    • Broadbent, D.E.1
  • 7
    • 0004243597 scopus 로고
    • Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York
    • D. E. Broadbent, Perception and Communication (Pergamon, London, 1958); U. Neisser, Cognitive Psychology (Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, 1967); M. R. Harter and C. J. Aine, in Varieties of Attention, R. Parasuraman and D. R. Davies, Eds. (Academic Press, Orlando, FL, 1984), pp. 293-321.
    • (1967) Cognitive Psychology
    • Neisser, U.1
  • 8
    • 0000718970 scopus 로고
    • R. Parasuraman and D. R. Davies, Eds. Academic Press, Orlando, FL
    • D. E. Broadbent, Perception and Communication (Pergamon, London, 1958); U. Neisser, Cognitive Psychology (Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, 1967); M. R. Harter and C. J. Aine, in Varieties of Attention, R. Parasuraman and D. R. Davies, Eds. (Academic Press, Orlando, FL, 1984), pp. 293-321.
    • (1984) Varieties of Attention , pp. 293-321
    • Harter, M.R.1    Aine, C.J.2
  • 11
    • 0002651020 scopus 로고
    • D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, R. J. W. Mansfield, Eds. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA
    • L. G. Ungerleider and M. Mishkin, in Analysis of Visual Behaviour, D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, R. J. W. Mansfield, Eds. (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1982), pp. 549-586.
    • (1982) Analysis of Visual Behaviour , pp. 549-586
    • Ungerleider, L.G.1    Mishkin, M.2
  • 14
    • 0025029346 scopus 로고
    • M. Eglin, L. C. Robertson, R. T. Knight., Cereb. Cortex. 1, 263 (1991); M. Marsel Mesulam, Ann. Neurol. 28, 597 (1990).
    • (1990) Ann. Neurol. , vol.28 , pp. 597
    • Marsel Mesulam, M.1
  • 15
    • 0027422152 scopus 로고
    • K. Tanaka, Science 262, 685 (1993); J. V. Haxby et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 88, 1621 (1991).
    • (1993) Science , vol.262 , pp. 685
    • Tanaka, K.1
  • 20
    • 0021991105 scopus 로고
    • P. T. Fox and M. E. Raichle, Ann. Neurol. 17, 303 (1985); C. Price et al., Neurosci. Lett. 146, 179 (1992); C. Price, C. Moore, R. S. J. Frackowiak, Neuroimage 3, 40 (1996).
    • (1985) Ann. Neurol. , vol.17 , pp. 303
    • Fox, P.T.1    Raichle, M.E.2
  • 21
    • 0026455209 scopus 로고
    • P. T. Fox and M. E. Raichle, Ann. Neurol. 17, 303 (1985); C. Price et al., Neurosci. Lett. 146, 179 (1992); C. Price, C. Moore, R. S. J. Frackowiak, Neuroimage 3, 40 (1996).
    • (1992) Neurosci. Lett. , vol.146 , pp. 179
    • Price, C.1
  • 22
    • 0030004989 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • P. T. Fox and M. E. Raichle, Ann. Neurol. 17, 303 (1985); C. Price et al., Neurosci. Lett. 146, 179 (1992); C. Price, C. Moore, R. S. J. Frackowiak, Neuroimage 3, 40 (1996).
    • (1996) Neuroimage , vol.3 , pp. 40
    • Price, C.1    Moore, C.2    Frackowiak, R.S.J.3
  • 23
    • 84889530173 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • This analogy is purely illustrative. The neural instantiation of phasic modulation might, for example, represent modulation of the spiking frequency, number of spiking neurons, or any number of other effects. Functional imaging reveals the activity in populations of neurons, so the theoretical analysis developed here should not be thought of as making predictions about the spiking behavior of individual neurons.
  • 24
    • 84889513373 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • All participants were right-handed males aged 31 to 76 (mean 40.5 years) who gave their informed consent for participation in this study. Isoluminant ellipses subtending ∼7° of visual angle were presented at regular intervals (30, 50, 70, and 90 ellipses per minute) for 500 ms at fixation on a computer monitor ∼57 cm from the participant. The ellipses could differ in color (red, green, or blue) and orientation (horizontal or vertical). A participant indicated a categorization choice of target or nontarget by pressing a button with either the right or left index finger. A response was required for every ellipse presented. The proportion of targets was kept constant across conditions at 50%; in all other respects the stimuli and presentation were identical in all conditions. The assignment of fingers was made before the scanning session and was the same for all scans for any one participant but counterbalanced across participants for the experiment. The order of task presentation was counterbalanced across participants.
  • 25
    • 0003415335 scopus 로고
    • Thieme, New York
    • 15O administrations was 8 min. The data were acquired in one 90-s frame, beginning 5 s before the rising phase of the head curve. Correction for attenuation was made with a transmission scan collected at the beginning of each study. Images were reconstructed by filtered back projection (Hanning filter, cut off frequency of 0.5 cycles per pixel) into 63 image planes (separation 2.4 mm) and into a 128 by 128 pixel image matrix (size 2.1 mm). For data analysis we used the general linear model and theory of Gaussian fields, as implemented in SPM96 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK). Images were realigned with the first as a reference, and then stereotactically transformed into the space of Talairach and Tournoux [J. Talairach and P. Tournoux, Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain (Thieme, New York, 1988)]. The images were smoothed with a Gaussian filter of 12-mm full-width half maximum. Statistical parametric maps were derived with prespecified contrasts between images as discussed in the text [K. J. Friston et al., Hum. Brain Mapp. 2, 165 (1994); K. J. Friston et al., ibid. 3, 189 (1995)].
    • (1988) Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain
    • Talairach, J.1    Tournoux, P.2
  • 26
    • 0000051967 scopus 로고
    • 15O administrations was 8 min. The data were acquired in one 90-s frame, beginning 5 s before the rising phase of the head curve. Correction for attenuation was made with a transmission scan collected at the beginning of each study. Images were reconstructed by filtered back projection (Hanning filter, cut off frequency of 0.5 cycles per pixel) into 63 image planes (separation 2.4 mm) and into a 128 by 128 pixel image matrix (size 2.1 mm). For data analysis we used the general linear model and theory of Gaussian fields, as implemented in SPM96 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK). Images were realigned with the first as a reference, and then stereotactically transformed into the space of Talairach and Tournoux [J. Talairach and P. Tournoux, Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain (Thieme, New York, 1988)]. The images were smoothed with a Gaussian filter of 12-mm full-width half maximum. Statistical parametric maps were derived with prespecified contrasts between images as discussed in the text [K. J. Friston et al., Hum. Brain Mapp. 2, 165 (1994); K. J. Friston et al., ibid. 3, 189 (1995)].
    • (1994) Hum. Brain Mapp. , vol.2 , pp. 165
    • Friston, K.J.1
  • 27
    • 0000051967 scopus 로고
    • 15O administrations was 8 min. The data were acquired in one 90-s frame, beginning 5 s before the rising phase of the head curve. Correction for attenuation was made with a transmission scan collected at the beginning of each study. Images were reconstructed by filtered back projection (Hanning filter, cut off frequency of 0.5 cycles per pixel) into 63 image planes (separation 2.4 mm) and into a 128 by 128 pixel image matrix (size 2.1 mm). For data analysis we used the general linear model and theory of Gaussian fields, as implemented in SPM96 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK). Images were realigned with the first as a reference, and then stereotactically transformed into the space of Talairach and Tournoux [J. Talairach and P. Tournoux, Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain (Thieme, New York, 1988)]. The images were smoothed with a Gaussian filter of 12-mm full-width half maximum. Statistical parametric maps were derived with prespecified contrasts between images as discussed in the text [K. J. Friston et al., Hum. Brain Mapp. 2, 165 (1994); K. J. Friston et al., ibid. 3, 189 (1995)].
    • (1995) Hum. Brain Mapp. , vol.3 , pp. 189
    • Friston, K.J.1
  • 28
    • 84889524398 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • The reaction time (RT) for correct responses for the color, orientation, and conjunction tasks was 352 ms (standard deviation 86 ms), 350 ms (standard deviation 95 ms), and 368 ms (standard deviation 104 ms), respectively. This significant difference in RT between conjunction and feature tasks is not in accord with Treisman's feature integration theory, if one regards our task as a conjunction search without distractors. However, similar elevated RTs in a conjunction search without distractors have been seen in the only functional imaging study previously reported of conjunction search (6). The RT declined with increasing rate of presentation, being 378 ms, 358 ms, 346 ms, and 357 ms for rates of 30, 50, 70, and 90 ellipses per minute, respectively. This may reflect participants finding it easier to stay "on task" at higher rates, keeping relevant behavioral goals more active. Error rates were highest (15.5%) with the color condition; for the orientation condition there were 5.05% errors, and for the conjunction condition 6.98% errors averaged over all participants. There is thus no suggestion from the error data that increased difficulty of the conjunction task might account for our results.
  • 29
    • 84889531068 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Tonic effects were indexed as an increase in baseline activity without an alteration of the correlation between activity and presentation rate. Phasic effects were indexed as a change in the correlation of activity and presentation rate. See Fig. 1 and Table 2.
  • 30
    • 84889512261 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • The Z score was 4.22, Talairach coordinates (42 22 40), and P < 0.001 uncorrected for multiple comparisons because of our prior hypothesis.
  • 31
    • 0024593108 scopus 로고
    • B. Seltzer and D. N. Pandya, J. Comp. Neurol. 281, 97 (1989); C. Cavada and P. S. Goldman-Rakic, ibid. 287, 422 (1989); H. Barbas, ibid. 276, 313 (1988); M. Petrides and D. N. Pandya, ibid. 273, 52 (1988); H. Barbas and M.-M. Mesulam, ibid. 200, 407 (1981).
    • (1989) J. Comp. Neurol. , vol.281 , pp. 97
    • Seltzer, B.1    Pandya, D.N.2
  • 32
    • 0024434384 scopus 로고
    • B. Seltzer and D. N. Pandya, J. Comp. Neurol. 281, 97 (1989); C. Cavada and P. S. Goldman-Rakic, ibid. 287, 422 (1989); H. Barbas, ibid. 276, 313 (1988); M. Petrides and D. N. Pandya, ibid. 273, 52 (1988); H. Barbas and M.-M. Mesulam, ibid. 200, 407 (1981).
    • (1989) J. Comp. Neurol. , vol.287 , pp. 422
    • Cavada, C.1    Goldman-Rakic, P.S.2
  • 33
    • 0023779537 scopus 로고
    • B. Seltzer and D. N. Pandya, J. Comp. Neurol. 281, 97 (1989); C. Cavada and P. S. Goldman-Rakic, ibid. 287, 422 (1989); H. Barbas, ibid. 276, 313 (1988); M. Petrides and D. N. Pandya, ibid. 273, 52 (1988); H. Barbas and M.-M. Mesulam, ibid. 200, 407 (1981).
    • (1988) J. Comp. Neurol. , vol.276 , pp. 313
    • Barbas, H.1
  • 34
    • 0023676198 scopus 로고
    • B. Seltzer and D. N. Pandya, J. Comp. Neurol. 281, 97 (1989); C. Cavada and P. S. Goldman-Rakic, ibid. 287, 422 (1989); H. Barbas, ibid. 276, 313 (1988); M. Petrides and D. N. Pandya, ibid. 273, 52 (1988); H. Barbas and M.-M. Mesulam, ibid. 200, 407 (1981).
    • (1988) J. Comp. Neurol. , vol.273 , pp. 52
    • Petrides, M.1    Pandya, D.N.2
  • 35
    • 0019507294 scopus 로고
    • B. Seltzer and D. N. Pandya, J. Comp. Neurol. 281, 97 (1989); C. Cavada and P. S. Goldman-Rakic, ibid. 287, 422 (1989); H. Barbas, ibid. 276, 313 (1988); M. Petrides and D. N. Pandya, ibid. 273, 52 (1988); H. Barbas and M.-M. Mesulam, ibid. 200, 407 (1981).
    • (1981) J. Comp. Neurol. , vol.200 , pp. 407
    • Barbas, H.1    Mesulam, M.-M.2
  • 36
    • 0028296177 scopus 로고
    • For example, M. Sakagami and H. Niki, Exp. Brain Res. 97, 423 (1994); E. B. Pragay et al., Exp. Neurol. 96, 481 (1987).
    • (1994) Exp. Brain Res. , vol.97 , pp. 423
    • Sakagami, M.1    Niki, H.2
  • 37
    • 0023235254 scopus 로고
    • For example, M. Sakagami and H. Niki, Exp. Brain Res. 97, 423 (1994); E. B. Pragay et al., Exp. Neurol. 96, 481 (1987).
    • (1987) Exp. Neurol. , vol.96 , pp. 481
    • Pragay, E.B.1
  • 39
    • 0016700568 scopus 로고
    • M. Petrides and S. D. Iversen, Neuropsychologia 16, 527 (1978); A. Sahgal et al., Nature 257, 672 (1975).
    • (1975) Nature , vol.257 , pp. 672
    • Sahgal, A.1
  • 40
    • 0029885193 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • BA 8 in monkey contains the frontal eye fields, and an alternate explanation for our results is that eye movements were responsible for the differences in neural activity between conjunction and feature tasks. However all stimuli were presented in an identical fashion over all tasks at fixation, and there is no reason to suspect differential eye movements in the conjunction condition. Furthermore, in humans the reported coordinates for the frontal eye fields are not located within BA 8 [T. Paus, Neuropsychologia 34, 475 (1996); G. A. O'Driscoll et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 92, 925 (1995); P. T. Fox et al., Neurophysiology 54, 349 (1985).
    • (1996) Neuropsychologia , vol.34 , pp. 475
    • Paus, T.1
  • 41
    • 0028837936 scopus 로고
    • BA 8 in monkey contains the frontal eye fields, and an alternate explanation for our results is that eye movements were responsible for the differences in neural activity between conjunction and feature tasks. However all stimuli were presented in an identical fashion over all tasks at fixation, and there is no reason to suspect differential eye movements in the conjunction condition. Furthermore, in humans the reported coordinates for the frontal eye fields are not located within BA 8 [T. Paus, Neuropsychologia 34, 475 (1996); G. A. O'Driscoll et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 92, 925 (1995); P. T. Fox et al., Neurophysiology 54, 349 (1985).
    • (1995) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. , vol.92 , pp. 925
    • O'Driscoll, G.A.1
  • 42
    • 0029885193 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • BA 8 in monkey contains the frontal eye fields, and an alternate explanation for our results is that eye movements were responsible for the differences in neural activity between conjunction and feature tasks. However all stimuli were presented in an identical fashion over all tasks at fixation, and there is no reason to suspect differential eye movements in the conjunction condition. Furthermore, in humans the reported coordinates for the frontal eye fields are not located within BA 8 [T. Paus, Neuropsychologia 34, 475 (1996); G. A. O'Driscoll et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 92, 925 (1995); P. T. Fox et al., Neurophysiology 54, 349 (1985).
    • (1985) Neurophysiology , vol.54 , pp. 349
    • Fox, P.T.1
  • 43
    • 0026414940 scopus 로고
    • J. V. Pardo et al., Nature 349, 61 (1991).
    • (1991) Nature , vol.349 , pp. 61
    • Pardo, J.V.1
  • 45
    • 0028265478 scopus 로고
    • E. K. Miller and R. Desimone, Science 263, 520 (1994); E. K. Miller, L. Li, R. Desimone, J. Neurosci. 13, 1460 (1993); Science 254, 1377 (1991). "Adaptive mnemonic filtering" refers to the reduction in the response from a cell to a stimulus, which has been observed independently of task. However an extrapolation from single-cell data to the evoked hemodynamic response from activity in a population of neurons should be made with caution. Other effects are also seen in inferior temporal cells; for example, an enhancement of response to a stimulus when it matches a searched for target. Our data suggest that the aggregate outcome of activity in several subsets of neurons with different response characteristics is an overall decrease in inferior temporal activity.
    • (1994) Science , vol.263 , pp. 520
    • Miller, E.K.1    Desimone, R.2
  • 46
    • 0027394621 scopus 로고
    • E. K. Miller and R. Desimone, Science 263, 520 (1994); E. K. Miller, L. Li, R. Desimone, J. Neurosci. 13, 1460 (1993); Science 254, 1377 (1991). "Adaptive mnemonic filtering" refers to the reduction in the response from a cell to a stimulus, which has been observed independently of task. However an extrapolation from single-cell data to the evoked hemodynamic response from activity in a population of neurons should be made with caution. Other effects are also seen in inferior temporal cells; for example, an enhancement of response to a stimulus when it matches a searched for target. Our data suggest that the aggregate outcome of activity in several subsets of neurons with different response characteristics is an overall decrease in inferior temporal activity.
    • (1993) J. Neurosci. , vol.13 , pp. 1460
    • Miller, E.K.1    Li, L.2    Desimone, R.3
  • 47
    • 0026352252 scopus 로고
    • E. K. Miller and R. Desimone, Science 263, 520 (1994); E. K. Miller, L. Li, R. Desimone, J. Neurosci. 13, 1460 (1993); Science 254, 1377 (1991). "Adaptive mnemonic filtering" refers to the reduction in the response from a cell to a stimulus, which has been observed independently of task. However an extrapolation from single-cell data to the evoked hemodynamic response from activity in a population of neurons should be made with caution. Other effects are also seen in inferior temporal cells; for example, an enhancement of response to a stimulus when it matches a searched for target. Our data suggest that the aggregate outcome of activity in several subsets of neurons with different response characteristics is an overall decrease in inferior temporal activity.
    • (1991) Science , vol.254 , pp. 1377
  • 48
    • 84913299946 scopus 로고
    • There are intriguing parallels with single-cell electrophysiology work, where both tonic and phasic signals are seen in inferior temporal cortex cells when monkeys attend to stimuli [(25); B. J. Richmond and T. Sato, Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 8, 812 (1982); J. Neurophysiol. 58, 1292 (1987)]. However, there are important differences with our data; in terms of modulation by attention our data suggest only a tonic modulation of inferior temporal cortical activity. There is no phasic modulation by attention (Fig. 1). Rather, we see a correlation between the rate of presentation of stimuli and inferior temporal cortex activity in addition to the attention-related modulation of this activity.
    • (1982) Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. , vol.8 , pp. 812
    • Richmond, B.J.1    Sato, T.2
  • 49
    • 0023594985 scopus 로고
    • There are intriguing parallels with single-cell electrophysiology work, where both tonic and phasic signals are seen in inferior temporal cortex cells when monkeys attend to stimuli [(25); B. J. Richmond and T. Sato, Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 8, 812 (1982); J. Neurophysiol. 58, 1292 (1987)]. However, there are important differences with our data; in terms of modulation by attention our data suggest only a tonic modulation of inferior temporal cortical activity. There is no phasic modulation by attention (Fig. 1). Rather, we see a correlation between the rate of presentation of stimuli and inferior temporal cortex activity in addition to the attention-related modulation of this activity.
    • (1987) J. Neurophysiol. , vol.58 , pp. 1292
  • 50
    • 84889539821 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • An alternate explanation for the modulatory effect seen in inferior temporal cortex might be that it reflects differences in the stimuli presented. Activity decreases in inferotemporal cortex units in monkey have been observed associated with target identification, and also with repetition of visual items [for example, (29)]. The probability of a target was kept constant at 50% in our study, and targets were physically identical in the conjunction condition but shared one feature in the feature tasks; therefore, there were more repetitions of physically identical targets per unit time in the conjunction task. However, if unequal repetition probabilities were to have an effect on our results, they should have led to increased activity with increasing rate of presentation. Such an effect would lead to a change in the slope relating rCBF to presentation rate in conjunction and feature tasks. We found no such evidence; the modulatory effect is tonic and so is task-dependent and stimulus-independent.
  • 52
    • 0015214110 scopus 로고
    • M. J. Webster, J. Bachevalier, L. G. Ungerleider, Cereb. Cortex 4, 470 (1994); D. N. Pandya et al., Brain Res. 31, 35 (1971); D. N. Pandya and H. G. J. M. Kuypers, ibid. 13, 13 (1969).
    • (1971) Brain Res. , vol.31 , pp. 35
    • Pandya, D.N.1
  • 53
    • 0014491084 scopus 로고
    • M. J. Webster, J. Bachevalier, L. G. Ungerleider, Cereb. Cortex 4, 470 (1994); D. N. Pandya et al., Brain Res. 31, 35 (1971); D. N. Pandya and H. G. J. M. Kuypers, ibid. 13, 13 (1969).
    • (1969) Brain Res. , vol.13 , pp. 13
    • Pandya, D.N.1    Kuypers, H.G.J.M.2
  • 54
    • 0017202048 scopus 로고
    • D. A. Chavis and D. N. Pandya, Brain Res. 117, 369 (1976); S. Jacobson and J. O. Trojanowski, ibid. 132, 209 (1977); E. G. Jones and T. P. S. Powell, Brain 93, 793 (1970).
    • (1976) Brain Res. , vol.117 , pp. 369
    • Chavis, D.A.1    Pandya, D.N.2
  • 55
    • 0017736050 scopus 로고
    • D. A. Chavis and D. N. Pandya, Brain Res. 117, 369 (1976); S. Jacobson and J. O. Trojanowski, ibid. 132, 209 (1977); E. G. Jones and T. P. S. Powell, Brain 93, 793 (1970).
    • (1977) Brain Res. , vol.132 , pp. 209
    • Jacobson, S.1    Trojanowski, J.O.2
  • 56
    • 0014890004 scopus 로고
    • D. A. Chavis and D. N. Pandya, Brain Res. 117, 369 (1976); S. Jacobson and J. O. Trojanowski, ibid. 132, 209 (1977); E. G. Jones and T. P. S. Powell, Brain 93, 793 (1970).
    • (1970) Brain , vol.93 , pp. 793
    • Jones, E.G.1    Powell, T.P.S.2
  • 57
    • 0345422229 scopus 로고
    • H. Heuer and A. F. Sanders, Eds. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ
    • For example, A. Allport, in Perspectives on Perception and Action, H. Heuer and A. F. Sanders, Eds. (Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1987).
    • (1987) Perspectives on Perception and Action
    • Allport, A.1
  • 58
    • 84889559549 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • We are grateful to the Wellcome Trust for their support, and to all our colleagues in the Functional Imaging Laboratory for their help in the conduct of this study. We also would like to thank D. Passingham, M. Husain, and our anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments and discussion.


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.