메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 274, Issue 5294, 1996, Pages 1926-1928

Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords

ARTICLE; AUDITORY STIMULATION; HUMAN; HUMAN EXPERIMENT; INFANT; LANGUAGE; LEARNING; NORMAL HUMAN; PRIORITY JOURNAL; SPEECH; SPEECH DISCRIMINATION; STATISTICS;

EID: 0030451408     PISSN: 00368075     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1126/science.274.5294.1926     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (3736)

References (39)
  • 1
    • 12644268186 scopus 로고
    • Certain species-specific skills develop without any experiential input, including bat echolocation [E. Gould, Dev. Psychobiol. 8, 33 (1975)] and cricket song [R. Hoy, Am. Zool. 14, 1067 (1974)].
    • (1975) Dev. Psychobiol. , vol.8 , pp. 33
    • Gould, E.1
  • 2
    • 77957214036 scopus 로고
    • Certain species-specific skills develop without any experiential input, including bat echolocation [E. Gould, Dev. Psychobiol. 8, 33 (1975)] and cricket song [R. Hoy, Am. Zool. 14, 1067 (1974)].
    • (1974) Am. Zool. , vol.14 , pp. 1067
    • Hoy, R.1
  • 3
    • 0003701549 scopus 로고
    • Van Nostrand, New York
    • Examples of behaviors mediated by early experience are imprinting [E. Hess, Imprinting (Van Nostrand, New York, 1973); M. Leon, Physiol. Behav. 14, 311 (1975)] and suckling responses in newborn rats [M. H. Teicher and E. M. Glass, Science 198, 635 (1977)].
    • (1973) Imprinting
    • Hess, E.1
  • 4
    • 0016707090 scopus 로고
    • Examples of behaviors mediated by early experience are imprinting [E. Hess, Imprinting (Van Nostrand, New York, 1973); M. Leon, Physiol. Behav. 14, 311 (1975)] and suckling responses in newborn rats [M. H. Teicher and E. M. Glass, Science 198, 635 (1977)].
    • (1975) Physiol. Behav. , vol.14 , pp. 311
    • Leon, M.1
  • 5
    • 0017706941 scopus 로고
    • Examples of behaviors mediated by early experience are imprinting [E. Hess, Imprinting (Van Nostrand, New York, 1973); M. Leon, Physiol. Behav. 14, 311 (1975)] and suckling responses in newborn rats [M. H. Teicher and E. M. Glass, Science 198, 635 (1977)].
    • (1977) Science , vol.198 , pp. 635
    • Teicher, M.H.1    Glass, E.M.2
  • 6
    • 0004075106 scopus 로고
    • Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, MA
    • These milestones have been well-documented both in English [for example, R. Brown, A First Language (Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, MA, 1973)] and cross-linguistically [for example, E. Lenneberg, Biological Foundations of Language (Wiley, New York, 1967); D. Slobin, Ed., vols. 1 to 3 of The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition (Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1985, 1987, 1992)].
    • (1973) A First Language
    • Brown, R.1
  • 7
    • 0003874961 scopus 로고
    • Wiley, New York
    • These milestones have been well-documented both in English [for example, R. Brown, A First Language (Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, MA, 1973)] and cross-linguistically [for example, E. Lenneberg, Biological Foundations of Language (Wiley, New York, 1967); D. Slobin, Ed., vols. 1 to 3 of The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition (Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1985, 1987, 1992)].
    • (1967) Biological Foundations of Language
    • Lenneberg, E.1
  • 8
    • 0003988640 scopus 로고
    • Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1987
    • These milestones have been well-documented both in English [for example, R. Brown, A First Language (Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, MA, 1973)] and cross-linguistically [for example, E. Lenneberg, Biological Foundations of Language (Wiley, New York, 1967); D. Slobin, Ed., vols. 1 to 3 of The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition (Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1985, 1987, 1992)].
    • (1985) The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition , vol.1-3
    • Slobin, D.1
  • 9
    • 0003647888 scopus 로고
    • MIT Press, Cambridge, MA
    • This "argument from the poverty of the stimulus" remains widely accepted [for example, N. Chomsky, Aspects of the Theory of Syntax (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1965); S. Crain, Behav. Brain Sci. 14, 597 (1991)].
    • (1965) Aspects of the Theory of Syntax
    • Chomsky, N.1
  • 10
    • 0346594072 scopus 로고
    • This "argument from the poverty of the stimulus" remains widely accepted [for example, N. Chomsky, Aspects of the Theory of Syntax (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1965); S. Crain, Behav. Brain Sci. 14, 597 (1991)].
    • (1991) Behav. Brain Sci. , vol.14 , pp. 597
    • Crain, S.1
  • 11
    • 84971195232 scopus 로고
    • D. Bickerton, Behav. Brain Sci. 7, 173 (1984); N. Chomsky, Rules and Representations (Columbia Univ. Press, New York, 1981); J. Fodor, Modularity of Mind (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1983); L. Gleitman and E. Newport, in Language: An Invitation to Cognitive Science, L Gleitman and M. Liberman, Eds. (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1995), pp. 1-24
    • (1984) Behav. Brain Sci. , vol.7 , pp. 173
    • Bickerton, D.1
  • 12
    • 0003838150 scopus 로고
    • Columbia Univ. Press, New York
    • D. Bickerton, Behav. Brain Sci. 7, 173 (1984); N. Chomsky, Rules and Representations (Columbia Univ. Press, New York, 1981); J. Fodor, Modularity of Mind (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1983); L. Gleitman and E. Newport, in Language: An Invitation to Cognitive Science, L Gleitman and M. Liberman, Eds. (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1995), pp. 1-24
    • (1981) Rules and Representations
    • Chomsky, N.1
  • 13
    • 0004213512 scopus 로고
    • MIT Press, Cambridge, MA
    • D. Bickerton, Behav. Brain Sci. 7, 173 (1984); N. Chomsky, Rules and Representations (Columbia Univ. Press, New York, 1981); J. Fodor, Modularity of Mind (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1983); L. Gleitman and E. Newport, in Language: An Invitation to Cognitive Science, L Gleitman and M. Liberman, Eds. (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1995), pp. 1-24
    • (1983) Modularity of Mind
    • Fodor, J.1
  • 14
    • 0037691844 scopus 로고
    • L Gleitman and M. Liberman, Eds. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA
    • D. Bickerton, Behav. Brain Sci. 7, 173 (1984); N. Chomsky, Rules and Representations (Columbia Univ. Press, New York, 1981); J. Fodor, Modularity of Mind (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1983); L. Gleitman and E. Newport, in Language: An Invitation to Cognitive Science, L Gleitman and M. Liberman, Eds. (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1995), pp. 1-24
    • (1995) Language: An Invitation to Cognitive Science , pp. 1-24
    • Gleitman, L.1    Newport, E.2
  • 15
    • 0026525165 scopus 로고
    • Examples include vowel structure [P. K. Kuhl, K. A. Williams, F. Lacerda, K. N. Stevens, B. Lindblom, Science 255, 606 (1992)], phonotactics [P. Jusczyk, A. Friederici, J. Wessels, V. Svenkerud, A. Jusczyk, J. Mem. Lang. 32, 401 (1993)], and prosodic structure [P. Jusczyk, A. Cutler, N. Redanz, Child Dev. 64, 675 (1993)].
    • (1992) Science , vol.255 , pp. 606
    • Kuhl, P.K.1    Williams, K.A.2    Lacerda, F.3    Stevens, K.N.4    Lindblom, B.5
  • 16
    • 12644310267 scopus 로고
    • Examples include vowel structure [P. K. Kuhl, K. A. Williams, F. Lacerda, K. N. Stevens, B. Lindblom, Science 255, 606 (1992)], phonotactics [P. Jusczyk, A. Friederici, J. Wessels, V. Svenkerud, A. Jusczyk, J. Mem. Lang. 32, 401 (1993)], and prosodic structure [P. Jusczyk, A. Cutler, N. Redanz, Child Dev. 64, 675 (1993)].
    • (1993) J. Mem. Lang. , vol.32 , pp. 401
    • Jusczyk, P.1    Friederici, A.2    Wessels, J.3    Svenkerud, V.4    Jusczyk, A.5
  • 17
    • 0027620110 scopus 로고
    • Examples include vowel structure [P. K. Kuhl, K. A. Williams, F. Lacerda, K. N. Stevens, B. Lindblom, Science 255, 606 (1992)], phonotactics [P. Jusczyk, A. Friederici, J. Wessels, V. Svenkerud, A. Jusczyk, J. Mem. Lang. 32, 401 (1993)], and prosodic structure [P. Jusczyk, A. Cutler, N. Redanz, Child Dev. 64, 675 (1993)].
    • (1993) Child Dev. , vol.64 , pp. 675
    • Jusczyk, P.1    Cutler, A.2    Redanz, N.3
  • 19
    • 0000319824 scopus 로고
    • Exceptions include research on prenatal exposure to maternal speech (A. DeCasper, J.-P. Lecanuet, M.-C. Busnel, C. Granier-Deferre, R. Maugeais, Infant Behav. Dev. 17, 159 (1994)] and early postnatal preferences [J. Mehler et al., Cognition 29, 149 (1988)].
    • (1988) Cognition , vol.29 , pp. 149
    • Mehler, J.1
  • 23
    • 0001479334 scopus 로고
    • A. Christophe, E. Dupoux, J. Bertoncini, J. Mehler, J. Acousf. Soc. Am. 95, 1570 (1994); A. Cutler and D. Carter, Comput. Speech Lang. 2, 133 (1987).
    • (1987) Comput. Speech Lang. , vol.2 , pp. 133
    • Cutler, A.1    Carter, D.2
  • 24
    • 0001074490 scopus 로고
    • Z. Harris, Language 31, 190 (1955); J. Hayes and H. Clark, in Cognition and the Development of Language, J. Hayes, Ed. (Wiley, New York, 1970). See M. Brent and T. Cartwright [Cognition 61, 93 (1996)] for a discussion of related statistical cues to word boundaries.
    • (1955) Language , vol.31 , pp. 190
    • Harris, Z.1
  • 25
    • 2942614508 scopus 로고
    • J. Hayes, Ed. Wiley, New York
    • Z. Harris, Language 31, 190 (1955); J. Hayes and H. Clark, in Cognition and the Development of Language, J. Hayes, Ed. (Wiley, New York, 1970). See M. Brent and T. Cartwright [Cognition 61, 93 (1996)] for a discussion of related statistical cues to word boundaries.
    • (1970) Cognition and the Development of Language
    • Hayes, J.1    Clark, H.2
  • 26
    • 0030256302 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Z. Harris, Language 31, 190 (1955); J. Hayes and H. Clark, in Cognition and the Development of Language, J. Hayes, Ed. (Wiley, New York, 1970). See M. Brent and T. Cartwright [Cognition 61, 93 (1996)] for a discussion of related statistical cues to word boundaries.
    • (1996) Cognition , vol.61 , pp. 93
    • Brent, M.1    Cartwright, T.2
  • 27
    • 12644317574 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • The transitional probability of Y|X = frequency of XY/frequency of X
  • 30
    • 12644298611 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Each infant was tested individually while seated on the parent's lap in a sound-attenuated booth. Synthetic speech was generated off-line by the Macin-Talk system and stored on disk at a sampling rate of 22 kHz for on-line playback through an Audiomedia board in an Apple Quadra 650 computer. An observer outside the testing booth monitored the infant's looking behavior with the use of a color video system, using a buttonbox connected to the computer to initiate trials and score head-turn responses. Both the parent and the observer listened to masking music over headphones to eliminate bias. During the 2-min familiarization phase, the infant's gaze was first directed to a blinking light located on the front wall of the testing booth, and then the sound sequence was presented from two loudspeakers located on the side walls. The infant's gaze was directed to one of two blinking lights on these side walls during familiarization, but there was no relation between lights and sound. Immediately after familiarization, 12 test trials were presented (six words and six nonwords). Each test trial began with the central blinking light. When the observer signaled with a button press that the infant had fixated on the central light, one of the two side blinking lights was turned on and the center light was extinguished. When the infant faced the side light (a head turn of at least 30° in the direction of the light), the three-syllable test string was played and repeated until the infant looked away from the light for 2 s or until 15 s of looking had occurred. The observer simply recorded the direction of the infant's head turn, and the computer measured looking times, determined when the 2-s lookaway criterion had been met, and controlled the randomization and presentation of stimuli. Cumulative looking time across each of the two types of test trials provided the measure of preference.
  • 31
    • 12644250052 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • The direction of the fixation preference depends on the degree of familiarity with the stimuli. If the infants have become highly familiar with the stimuli, they show dishabituation behavior, preferring the novel stimuli.
  • 32
    • 12644282492 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Two counterbalanced stimulus conditions were generated. For each condition, 45 tokens of each of four trisyllabic nonsense words (condition A: tupiro, golabu, bidaku, and padoti; condition B: dapiku, tilado, burobi, and pagotu) were spoken in random order to create a 2-min speech stream, with the stipulation that the same word never occurred twice in a row.
  • 33
    • 12644278809 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Test stimuli: tupiro, golabu, dapiku, and tilado. In condition A, the first two strings were words and the last two strings were nonwords (the transitional probabilities between the syllables in the nonwords were all zero relative to the exposure corpus, as these syllable pairs had never occurred during familiarization). In condition B, the first two strings were nonwords and the last two strings were words. This between-subjects counterbalanced design ensured that any observed preferences for words or nonwords across both conditions would not be artifacts of any general preferences for certain syllable strings. Each of the four test strings were presented (repeated win a 500-ms interval between test strings) on three different trials, resulting in a total of 12 test trials per infant.
  • 34
    • 12644292857 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • There were no significant differences between the infants in condition A and condition B: f(22) = 0.31. The data from the two groups were thus combined for the other analyses.
  • 35
    • 12644259453 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Condition A words: pabiku, tibudo, golatu, and daropi; condition B words: tudaro, pigola, bikuti, and budops.
  • 36
    • 12644306828 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Test stimuli: pabiku, tibudo, tudaro, and pigola. In condition A, the first two strings were words and the second two strings were part-words. For example, the part-word pigola spanned the word boundary between daropi#golatu and thus was heard during exposure. In condition B, the first two strings were part-words and the second two strings were words. The part-words were thus three-syllable sequences that the infants had heard during the course of the exposure period. The difficulty of this test discrimination can be seen by comparing the transitional probabilities between the syllables in the words (1.0 between syllables 1 and 2 and between syllables 2 and 3) to the transitional probabilities between the syllables in the part-words (0.33 between syllables 1 and 2 and 1.0 between syllables 2 and 3).
  • 37
    • 12644296948 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • There were no significant differences between the infants in condition A and condition B: t(22) = 0.49. The data from the two groups were thus combined for the other analyses.
  • 38
    • 12644294997 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • For example, this same general mechanism could be used to find an object, such as a human face, in the environment.
  • 39
    • 12644280786 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • We thank J. Gallipeau, J. Hooker, P. Jusczyk, A. Jusczyk, T. Mintz, K. Ruppert, and J. Sawusch for their help with various aspects of this research, and P. Jusczyk, S. Pollak, M. Spivey-Knowlton, and M. Tanenhaus for their helpful comments on a previous draft. Supported by an NSF predoctoral fellowship (J.R.S.), NSF grant SBR9421064 (R.N.A.), and NIH grant DC00167 (E.L.N.). The parents of all participants gave informed consent.


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.