-
1
-
-
0003041373
-
Perceiving layout and knowing distances: The integration, relative potency, and contextual use of different information about depth
-
W. Epstein and S. Rogers, Eds. (Academic Press, New York, in press)
-
For example, see J.E. Cutting and P.M. Vishton, Perceiving layout and knowing distances: The integration, relative potency, and contextual use of different information about depth, in Handbook of Perception and Cognition: Vol. 5. Perception of Space and Motion, W. Epstein and S. Rogers, Eds. (Academic Press, New York, in press); J.M. Foley, Binocular distance perception, Psychological Review, 87, 411-134 (1980); A.S. Gilinsky, Perceived size and distance in visual space, Psychological Review, 58, 460-482 (1951);
-
Handbook of Perception and Cognition: Vol. 5. Perception of Space and Motion
, vol.5
-
-
Cutting, J.E.1
Vishton, P.M.2
-
2
-
-
0019059602
-
Binocular distance perception
-
For example, see J.E. Cutting and P.M. Vishton, Perceiving layout and knowing distances: The integration, relative potency, and contextual use of different information about depth, in Handbook of Perception and Cognition: Vol. 5. Perception of Space and Motion, W. Epstein and S. Rogers, Eds. (Academic Press, New York, in press); J.M. Foley, Binocular distance perception, Psychological Review, 87, 411-134 (1980); A.S. Gilinsky, Perceived size and distance in visual space, Psychological Review, 58, 460-482 (1951);
-
(1980)
Psychological Review
, vol.87
, pp. 411-1134
-
-
Foley, J.M.1
-
3
-
-
0001014677
-
Perceived size and distance in visual space
-
For example, see J.E. Cutting and P.M. Vishton, Perceiving layout and knowing distances: The integration, relative potency, and contextual use of different information about depth, in Handbook of Perception and Cognition: Vol. 5. Perception of Space and Motion, W. Epstein and S. Rogers, Eds. (Academic Press, New York, in press); J.M. Foley, Binocular distance perception, Psychological Review, 87, 411-134 (1980); A.S. Gilinsky, Perceived size and distance in visual space, Psychological Review, 58, 460-482 (1951);
-
(1951)
Psychological Review
, vol.58
, pp. 460-482
-
-
Gilinsky, A.S.1
-
4
-
-
0011651749
-
The analysis of perceived space
-
S.C. Masin, Ed. Elsevier Science, Amsterdam
-
W. C. Gogel, The analysis of perceived space, in Foundations of Perceptual Theory, S.C. Masin, Ed. (Elsevier Science, Amsterdam, 1993);
-
(1993)
Foundations of Perceptual Theory
-
-
Gogel, W.C.1
-
5
-
-
0029198819
-
Distortions of 3-dimensional space in the perceptual analysis of motion and stereo
-
J.T. Todd, J.S. Tittle, and J.F. Norman, Distortions of 3-dimensional space in the perceptual analysis of motion and stereo, Perception, 24, 75-86 (1995).
-
(1995)
Perception
, vol.24
, pp. 75-86
-
-
Todd, J.T.1
Tittle, J.S.2
Norman, J.F.3
-
6
-
-
0003100713
-
Visuo-motor coordination in space-time
-
G.E. Stelmach and J. Requin, Eds. North-Holland, Amsterdam
-
For example, see D.N. Lee, Visuo-motor coordination in space-time, in Tutorials in Motor Behavior, G.E. Stelmach and J. Requin, Eds. (North-Holland, Amsterdam, 1980); M.K. McBeath, D.M. Shaffer, and M.K. Kaiser, How baseball outfielders determine where to run to catch fly balls, Science, 268, 569-573 (1995); W.H. Warren, The perception-action coupling, in Sensory-Motor Organizations and Development in Infancy and Early Childhood, H. Bloch and B.I. Bertenthal, Eds. (Kluwer Academic, the Netherlands, 1990).
-
(1980)
Tutorials in Motor Behavior
-
-
Lee, D.N.1
-
7
-
-
0029653405
-
How baseball outfielders determine where to run to catch fly balls
-
For example, see D.N. Lee, Visuo-motor coordination in space-time, in Tutorials in Motor Behavior, G.E. Stelmach and J. Requin, Eds. (North-Holland, Amsterdam, 1980); M.K. McBeath, D.M. Shaffer, and M.K. Kaiser, How baseball outfielders determine where to run to catch fly balls, Science, 268, 569-573 (1995); W.H. Warren, The perception-action coupling, in Sensory-Motor Organizations and Development in Infancy and Early Childhood, H. Bloch and B.I. Bertenthal, Eds. (Kluwer Academic, the Netherlands, 1990).
-
(1995)
Science
, vol.268
, pp. 569-573
-
-
McBeath, M.K.1
Shaffer, D.M.2
Kaiser, M.K.3
-
8
-
-
0004454641
-
The perception-action coupling
-
H. Bloch and B.I. Bertenthal, Eds. Kluwer Academic, the Netherlands
-
For example, see D.N. Lee, Visuo-motor coordination in space-time, in Tutorials in Motor Behavior, G.E. Stelmach and J. Requin, Eds. (North-Holland, Amsterdam, 1980); M.K. McBeath, D.M. Shaffer, and M.K. Kaiser, How baseball outfielders determine where to run to catch fly balls, Science, 268, 569-573 (1995); W.H. Warren, The perception-action coupling, in Sensory-Motor Organizations and Development in Infancy and Early Childhood, H. Bloch and B.I. Bertenthal, Eds. (Kluwer Academic, the Netherlands, 1990).
-
(1990)
Sensory-motor Organizations and Development in Infancy and Early Childhood
-
-
Warren, W.H.1
-
9
-
-
0026953714
-
Visual space perception and visually directed action
-
J.M. Loomis, J.A. Da Silva, N. Fujita, and S.S. Fukusima, Visual space perception and visually directed action, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 906-921 (1992); S.S. Fukusima, J.M. Loomis, and J.A. Da Silva, Visual perception of egocentric distance as assessed by triangulation, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance (in press).
-
(1992)
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
, vol.18
, pp. 906-921
-
-
Loomis, J.M.1
Da Silva, J.A.2
Fujita, N.3
Fukusima, S.S.4
-
10
-
-
0026953714
-
Visual perception of egocentric distance as assessed by triangulation
-
in press
-
J.M. Loomis, J.A. Da Silva, N. Fujita, and S.S. Fukusima, Visual space perception and visually directed action, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 906-921 (1992); S.S. Fukusima, J.M. Loomis, and J.A. Da Silva, Visual perception of egocentric distance as assessed by triangulation, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance (in press).
-
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
-
-
Fukusima, S.S.1
Loomis, J.M.2
Da Silva, J.A.3
-
12
-
-
4244094456
-
Measurement of visually perceived egocentric distance under reduced- and full-cue conditions
-
J.W. Philbeck, J.M. Loomis, and A.C. Beall, Measurement of visually perceived egocentric distance under reduced- and full-cue conditions, Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 36, S666 (1995).
-
(1995)
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science
, vol.36
-
-
Philbeck, J.W.1
Loomis, J.M.2
Beall, A.C.3
-
14
-
-
0025339796
-
Differential use of distance and location information for spatial localization
-
Other researchers have found evidence of a different type of dissociation, namely, one between conscious perception and action
-
Loomis, Da Silva, Fujita, and Fukusima, note 3. For other results supporting a dissociation between perceived location and perceived exocentric distance, see R.A. Abrams and J.Z. Landgraf, Differential use of distance and location information for spatial localization, Perception & Psychophysics, 47, 349-359 (1990). Other researchers have found evidence of a different type of dissociation, namely, one between conscious perception and action. See B. Bridgeman, M. Kirch, and A. Sperling, Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion, Perception & Psychophysics, 29, 336-342 (1981); M.A. Goodale and A.D. Milner, Separate visual pathways for perception and action, Trends in Neuroscience, 15, 20-25 (1992).
-
(1990)
Perception & Psychophysics
, vol.47
, pp. 349-359
-
-
Abrams, R.A.1
Landgraf, J.Z.2
-
15
-
-
0019461433
-
Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion
-
Loomis, Da Silva, Fujita, and Fukusima, note 3. For other results supporting a dissociation between perceived location and perceived exocentric distance, see R.A. Abrams and J.Z. Landgraf, Differential use of distance and location information for spatial localization, Perception & Psychophysics, 47, 349-359 (1990). Other researchers have found evidence of a different type of dissociation, namely, one between conscious perception and action. See B. Bridgeman, M. Kirch, and A. Sperling, Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion, Perception & Psychophysics, 29, 336-342 (1981); M.A. Goodale and A.D. Milner, Separate visual pathways for perception and action, Trends in Neuroscience, 15, 20-25 (1992).
-
(1981)
Perception & Psychophysics
, vol.29
, pp. 336-342
-
-
Bridgeman, B.1
Kirch, M.2
Sperling, A.3
-
16
-
-
0026565214
-
Separate visual pathways for perception and action
-
Loomis, Da Silva, Fujita, and Fukusima, note 3. For other results supporting a dissociation between perceived location and perceived exocentric distance, see R.A. Abrams and J.Z. Landgraf, Differential use of distance and location information for spatial localization, Perception & Psychophysics, 47, 349-359 (1990). Other researchers have found evidence of a different type of dissociation, namely, one between conscious perception and action. See B. Bridgeman, M. Kirch, and A. Sperling, Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion, Perception & Psychophysics, 29, 336-342 (1981); M.A. Goodale and A.D. Milner, Separate visual pathways for perception and action, Trends in Neuroscience, 15, 20-25 (1992).
-
(1992)
Trends in Neuroscience
, vol.15
, pp. 20-25
-
-
Goodale, M.A.1
Milner, A.D.2
-
18
-
-
85033652968
-
-
note
-
Binocular convergence is an egocentric distance cue that derives from the relative pointing directions of the two eyes. Binocular disparity is an exocentric distance cue and refers to the difference in the angular sizes of the left- and right-eye retinal images, both of which correspond to a single interval in three-dimensional space.
-
-
-
-
19
-
-
85033656005
-
-
note 1
-
Gilinsky, note 1.
-
-
-
Gilinsky1
|