메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 5, Issue 3, 1996, Pages 72-77

Visual perception of location and distance

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords


EID: 0030163322     PISSN: 09637214     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10772783     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (168)

References (19)
  • 1
    • 0003041373 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Perceiving layout and knowing distances: The integration, relative potency, and contextual use of different information about depth
    • W. Epstein and S. Rogers, Eds. (Academic Press, New York, in press)
    • For example, see J.E. Cutting and P.M. Vishton, Perceiving layout and knowing distances: The integration, relative potency, and contextual use of different information about depth, in Handbook of Perception and Cognition: Vol. 5. Perception of Space and Motion, W. Epstein and S. Rogers, Eds. (Academic Press, New York, in press); J.M. Foley, Binocular distance perception, Psychological Review, 87, 411-134 (1980); A.S. Gilinsky, Perceived size and distance in visual space, Psychological Review, 58, 460-482 (1951);
    • Handbook of Perception and Cognition: Vol. 5. Perception of Space and Motion , vol.5
    • Cutting, J.E.1    Vishton, P.M.2
  • 2
    • 0019059602 scopus 로고
    • Binocular distance perception
    • For example, see J.E. Cutting and P.M. Vishton, Perceiving layout and knowing distances: The integration, relative potency, and contextual use of different information about depth, in Handbook of Perception and Cognition: Vol. 5. Perception of Space and Motion, W. Epstein and S. Rogers, Eds. (Academic Press, New York, in press); J.M. Foley, Binocular distance perception, Psychological Review, 87, 411-134 (1980); A.S. Gilinsky, Perceived size and distance in visual space, Psychological Review, 58, 460-482 (1951);
    • (1980) Psychological Review , vol.87 , pp. 411-1134
    • Foley, J.M.1
  • 3
    • 0001014677 scopus 로고
    • Perceived size and distance in visual space
    • For example, see J.E. Cutting and P.M. Vishton, Perceiving layout and knowing distances: The integration, relative potency, and contextual use of different information about depth, in Handbook of Perception and Cognition: Vol. 5. Perception of Space and Motion, W. Epstein and S. Rogers, Eds. (Academic Press, New York, in press); J.M. Foley, Binocular distance perception, Psychological Review, 87, 411-134 (1980); A.S. Gilinsky, Perceived size and distance in visual space, Psychological Review, 58, 460-482 (1951);
    • (1951) Psychological Review , vol.58 , pp. 460-482
    • Gilinsky, A.S.1
  • 4
    • 0011651749 scopus 로고
    • The analysis of perceived space
    • S.C. Masin, Ed. Elsevier Science, Amsterdam
    • W. C. Gogel, The analysis of perceived space, in Foundations of Perceptual Theory, S.C. Masin, Ed. (Elsevier Science, Amsterdam, 1993);
    • (1993) Foundations of Perceptual Theory
    • Gogel, W.C.1
  • 5
    • 0029198819 scopus 로고
    • Distortions of 3-dimensional space in the perceptual analysis of motion and stereo
    • J.T. Todd, J.S. Tittle, and J.F. Norman, Distortions of 3-dimensional space in the perceptual analysis of motion and stereo, Perception, 24, 75-86 (1995).
    • (1995) Perception , vol.24 , pp. 75-86
    • Todd, J.T.1    Tittle, J.S.2    Norman, J.F.3
  • 6
    • 0003100713 scopus 로고
    • Visuo-motor coordination in space-time
    • G.E. Stelmach and J. Requin, Eds. North-Holland, Amsterdam
    • For example, see D.N. Lee, Visuo-motor coordination in space-time, in Tutorials in Motor Behavior, G.E. Stelmach and J. Requin, Eds. (North-Holland, Amsterdam, 1980); M.K. McBeath, D.M. Shaffer, and M.K. Kaiser, How baseball outfielders determine where to run to catch fly balls, Science, 268, 569-573 (1995); W.H. Warren, The perception-action coupling, in Sensory-Motor Organizations and Development in Infancy and Early Childhood, H. Bloch and B.I. Bertenthal, Eds. (Kluwer Academic, the Netherlands, 1990).
    • (1980) Tutorials in Motor Behavior
    • Lee, D.N.1
  • 7
    • 0029653405 scopus 로고
    • How baseball outfielders determine where to run to catch fly balls
    • For example, see D.N. Lee, Visuo-motor coordination in space-time, in Tutorials in Motor Behavior, G.E. Stelmach and J. Requin, Eds. (North-Holland, Amsterdam, 1980); M.K. McBeath, D.M. Shaffer, and M.K. Kaiser, How baseball outfielders determine where to run to catch fly balls, Science, 268, 569-573 (1995); W.H. Warren, The perception-action coupling, in Sensory-Motor Organizations and Development in Infancy and Early Childhood, H. Bloch and B.I. Bertenthal, Eds. (Kluwer Academic, the Netherlands, 1990).
    • (1995) Science , vol.268 , pp. 569-573
    • McBeath, M.K.1    Shaffer, D.M.2    Kaiser, M.K.3
  • 8
    • 0004454641 scopus 로고
    • The perception-action coupling
    • H. Bloch and B.I. Bertenthal, Eds. Kluwer Academic, the Netherlands
    • For example, see D.N. Lee, Visuo-motor coordination in space-time, in Tutorials in Motor Behavior, G.E. Stelmach and J. Requin, Eds. (North-Holland, Amsterdam, 1980); M.K. McBeath, D.M. Shaffer, and M.K. Kaiser, How baseball outfielders determine where to run to catch fly balls, Science, 268, 569-573 (1995); W.H. Warren, The perception-action coupling, in Sensory-Motor Organizations and Development in Infancy and Early Childhood, H. Bloch and B.I. Bertenthal, Eds. (Kluwer Academic, the Netherlands, 1990).
    • (1990) Sensory-motor Organizations and Development in Infancy and Early Childhood
    • Warren, W.H.1
  • 9
    • 0026953714 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Visual space perception and visually directed action
    • J.M. Loomis, J.A. Da Silva, N. Fujita, and S.S. Fukusima, Visual space perception and visually directed action, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 906-921 (1992); S.S. Fukusima, J.M. Loomis, and J.A. Da Silva, Visual perception of egocentric distance as assessed by triangulation, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance (in press).
    • (1992) Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , vol.18 , pp. 906-921
    • Loomis, J.M.1    Da Silva, J.A.2    Fujita, N.3    Fukusima, S.S.4
  • 10
    • 0026953714 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Visual perception of egocentric distance as assessed by triangulation
    • in press
    • J.M. Loomis, J.A. Da Silva, N. Fujita, and S.S. Fukusima, Visual space perception and visually directed action, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 906-921 (1992); S.S. Fukusima, J.M. Loomis, and J.A. Da Silva, Visual perception of egocentric distance as assessed by triangulation, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance (in press).
    • Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
    • Fukusima, S.S.1    Loomis, J.M.2    Da Silva, J.A.3
  • 14
    • 0025339796 scopus 로고
    • Differential use of distance and location information for spatial localization
    • Other researchers have found evidence of a different type of dissociation, namely, one between conscious perception and action
    • Loomis, Da Silva, Fujita, and Fukusima, note 3. For other results supporting a dissociation between perceived location and perceived exocentric distance, see R.A. Abrams and J.Z. Landgraf, Differential use of distance and location information for spatial localization, Perception & Psychophysics, 47, 349-359 (1990). Other researchers have found evidence of a different type of dissociation, namely, one between conscious perception and action. See B. Bridgeman, M. Kirch, and A. Sperling, Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion, Perception & Psychophysics, 29, 336-342 (1981); M.A. Goodale and A.D. Milner, Separate visual pathways for perception and action, Trends in Neuroscience, 15, 20-25 (1992).
    • (1990) Perception & Psychophysics , vol.47 , pp. 349-359
    • Abrams, R.A.1    Landgraf, J.Z.2
  • 15
    • 0019461433 scopus 로고
    • Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion
    • Loomis, Da Silva, Fujita, and Fukusima, note 3. For other results supporting a dissociation between perceived location and perceived exocentric distance, see R.A. Abrams and J.Z. Landgraf, Differential use of distance and location information for spatial localization, Perception & Psychophysics, 47, 349-359 (1990). Other researchers have found evidence of a different type of dissociation, namely, one between conscious perception and action. See B. Bridgeman, M. Kirch, and A. Sperling, Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion, Perception & Psychophysics, 29, 336-342 (1981); M.A. Goodale and A.D. Milner, Separate visual pathways for perception and action, Trends in Neuroscience, 15, 20-25 (1992).
    • (1981) Perception & Psychophysics , vol.29 , pp. 336-342
    • Bridgeman, B.1    Kirch, M.2    Sperling, A.3
  • 16
    • 0026565214 scopus 로고
    • Separate visual pathways for perception and action
    • Loomis, Da Silva, Fujita, and Fukusima, note 3. For other results supporting a dissociation between perceived location and perceived exocentric distance, see R.A. Abrams and J.Z. Landgraf, Differential use of distance and location information for spatial localization, Perception & Psychophysics, 47, 349-359 (1990). Other researchers have found evidence of a different type of dissociation, namely, one between conscious perception and action. See B. Bridgeman, M. Kirch, and A. Sperling, Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion, Perception & Psychophysics, 29, 336-342 (1981); M.A. Goodale and A.D. Milner, Separate visual pathways for perception and action, Trends in Neuroscience, 15, 20-25 (1992).
    • (1992) Trends in Neuroscience , vol.15 , pp. 20-25
    • Goodale, M.A.1    Milner, A.D.2
  • 18
    • 85033652968 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Binocular convergence is an egocentric distance cue that derives from the relative pointing directions of the two eyes. Binocular disparity is an exocentric distance cue and refers to the difference in the angular sizes of the left- and right-eye retinal images, both of which correspond to a single interval in three-dimensional space.
  • 19


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.