메뉴 건너뛰기




Volumn 74, Issue 3, 1997, Pages 541-556

Self-perceived knowledge of the O.J. Simpson trial: Third-person perception and perceptions of guilt

Author keywords

[No Author keywords available]

Indexed keywords


EID: 0001286053     PISSN: 10776990     EISSN: None     Source Type: Journal    
DOI: 10.1177/107769909707400308     Document Type: Article
Times cited : (75)

References (70)
  • 1
    • 85033077392 scopus 로고
    • Have a good time
    • NY: Warner Brothers compact disk, originally recorded in
    • Paul Simon, "Have a Good Time," in Negotiations and Love Songs, 1971-1986 (NY: Warner Brothers compact disk, 1988, originally recorded in 1975).
    • (1975) Negotiations and Love Songs, 1971-1986
    • Simon, P.1
  • 2
    • 34047260359 scopus 로고
    • The third-person effect in communication
    • spring A recent literature review reported that thirteen of fourteen studies at least partly support the third-person effect
    • The third-person effect was first enunciated by sociologist W. Phillips Davison in "The Third-Person Effect in Communication," Public Opinion Quarterly 47 (spring 1983): 1-15. A recent literature review reported that thirteen of fourteen studies at least partly support the third-person effect. Richard M. Perloff, "Third-Person Effect Research, 1983-1992: A Review and Synthesis," International Journal of Public Opinion Research 5 (summer 1993): 167-84. Also see Dominic L. Lasorsa, "Policy-Makers and the Third-Person Effect," in Public Opinion, the Press and Public Policy, ed. J. David Kennamer (NY: Praeger, 1992), 163-75; Klaus Schoenbach and Lee B. Becker, "Origins and Consequences of Mediated Public Opinion," in Public Opinion and the Communication of Consent, ed. Theodore L. Glasser and Charles T. Salmon (NY: The Guilford Press, 1995), 323-47.
    • (1983) Public Opinion Quarterly , vol.47 , pp. 1-15
    • Davison, W.P.1
  • 3
    • 0001552483 scopus 로고
    • Third-person effect research, 1983-1992: A review and synthesis
    • summer
    • The third-person effect was first enunciated by sociologist W. Phillips Davison in "The Third-Person Effect in Communication," Public Opinion Quarterly 47 (spring 1983): 1-15. A recent literature review reported that thirteen of fourteen studies at least partly support the third-person effect. Richard M. Perloff, "Third-Person Effect Research, 1983-1992: A Review and Synthesis," International Journal of Public Opinion Research 5 (summer 1993): 167-84. Also see Dominic L. Lasorsa, "Policy-Makers and the Third-Person Effect," in Public Opinion, the Press and Public Policy, ed. J. David Kennamer (NY: Praeger, 1992), 163-75; Klaus Schoenbach and Lee B. Becker, "Origins and Consequences of Mediated Public Opinion," in Public Opinion and the Communication of Consent, ed. Theodore L. Glasser and Charles T. Salmon (NY: The Guilford Press, 1995), 323-47.
    • (1993) International Journal of Public Opinion Research , vol.5 , pp. 167-184
    • Perloff, R.M.1
  • 4
    • 0002367359 scopus 로고
    • Policy-makers and the third-person effect
    • ed. J. David Kennamer NY: Praeger
    • The third-person effect was first enunciated by sociologist W. Phillips Davison in "The Third-Person Effect in Communication," Public Opinion Quarterly 47 (spring 1983): 1-15. A recent literature review reported that thirteen of fourteen studies at least partly support the third-person effect. Richard M. Perloff, "Third-Person Effect Research, 1983-1992: A Review and Synthesis," International Journal of Public Opinion Research 5 (summer 1993): 167-84. Also see Dominic L. Lasorsa, "Policy-Makers and the Third-Person Effect," in Public Opinion, the Press and Public Policy, ed. J. David Kennamer (NY: Praeger, 1992), 163-75; Klaus Schoenbach and Lee B. Becker, "Origins and Consequences of Mediated Public Opinion," in Public Opinion and the Communication of Consent, ed. Theodore L. Glasser and Charles T. Salmon (NY: The Guilford Press, 1995), 323-47.
    • (1992) Public Opinion, the Press and Public Policy , pp. 163-175
    • Lasorsa, D.L.1
  • 5
    • 0000534883 scopus 로고
    • Origins and consequences of mediated public opinion
    • ed. Theodore L. Glasser and Charles T. Salmon NY: The Guilford Press
    • The third-person effect was first enunciated by sociologist W. Phillips Davison in "The Third-Person Effect in Communication," Public Opinion Quarterly 47 (spring 1983): 1-15. A recent literature review reported that thirteen of fourteen studies at least partly support the third-person effect. Richard M. Perloff, "Third-Person Effect Research, 1983-1992: A Review and Synthesis," International Journal of Public Opinion Research 5 (summer 1993): 167-84. Also see Dominic L. Lasorsa, "Policy-Makers and the Third-Person Effect," in Public Opinion, the Press and Public Policy, ed. J. David Kennamer (NY: Praeger, 1992), 163-75; Klaus Schoenbach and Lee B. Becker, "Origins and Consequences of Mediated Public Opinion," in Public Opinion and the Communication of Consent, ed. Theodore L. Glasser and Charles T. Salmon (NY: The Guilford Press, 1995), 323-47.
    • (1995) Public Opinion and the Communication of Consent , pp. 323-347
    • Schoenbach, K.1    Becker, L.B.2
  • 7
    • 84937274046 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The third-person effect revisited
    • Davison recently wrote approvingly of Perloff's description of the effect as a form of perception - "third-person perception." summer
    • Davison recently wrote approvingly of Perloff's description of the effect as a form of perception - "third-person perception." W. Phillips Davison, "The Third-Person Effect Revisited," International Journal of Public Opinion Research 8 (summer 1996): 113-19.
    • (1996) International Journal of Public Opinion Research , vol.8 , pp. 113-119
    • Davison, W.P.1
  • 8
    • 0030304251 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The causes of third-person effects: Unrealistic optimism, impersonal impact, or generalized negative attitudes towards media influence
    • summer
    • Numerous explanations have been offered to explain this effect. See Hans-Bernd Brosius and Dirk Engel, "The Causes of Third-Person Effects: Unrealistic Optimism, Impersonal Impact, or Generalized Negative Attitudes Towards Media Influence," International Journal of Public Opinion Research 8 (summer 1996): 142-62; Julie M. Duck, Michael A. Hogg, and Deborah J. Terry, "Me, Us and Them: Political Identification and the Third-Person Effect in the 1993 Australian Federal Election," European Journal of Social Psychology 25 (March/April 1995): 195-215; Julie M. Duck, Deborah J. Terry, and Michael A. Hogg, "The Perceived Influence of AIDS Advertising: Third-Person Effects in the Context of Positive Media Content," Basic and Applied Social Psychology 17 (1995): 305-325; Carroll J. Glynn, Ronald E. Ostman, and Daniel G. McDonald, "Opinions, Perception, and Social Reality," in Public Opinion and the Communication of Consent, ed. Glasser and Salmon, 249-77; Albert C. Gunther, "What We Think Others Think: Cause and Consequence in the Third Person Effect," Communication Research 18 (June 1991): 355-72; Diana C. Mutz, "The Political Effects of Perceptions of Mass Opinion," Research in Micropolitics 4 (1994): 143-67.
    • (1996) International Journal of Public Opinion Research , vol.8 , pp. 142-162
    • Brosius, H.-B.1    Engel, D.2
  • 9
    • 84984474678 scopus 로고
    • Me, us and them: Political identification and the third-person effect in the 1993 Australian federal election
    • March/April
    • Numerous explanations have been offered to explain this effect. See Hans-Bernd Brosius and Dirk Engel, "The Causes of Third-Person Effects: Unrealistic Optimism, Impersonal Impact, or Generalized Negative Attitudes Towards Media Influence," International Journal of Public Opinion Research 8 (summer 1996): 142-62; Julie M. Duck, Michael A. Hogg, and Deborah J. Terry, "Me, Us and Them: Political Identification and the Third-Person Effect in the 1993 Australian Federal Election," European Journal of Social Psychology 25 (March/April 1995): 195-215; Julie M. Duck, Deborah J. Terry, and Michael A. Hogg, "The Perceived Influence of AIDS Advertising: Third-Person Effects in the Context of Positive Media Content," Basic and Applied Social Psychology 17 (1995): 305-325; Carroll J. Glynn, Ronald E. Ostman, and Daniel G. McDonald, "Opinions, Perception, and Social Reality," in Public Opinion and the Communication of Consent, ed. Glasser and Salmon, 249-77; Albert C. Gunther, "What We Think Others Think: Cause and Consequence in the Third Person Effect," Communication Research 18 (June 1991): 355-72; Diana C. Mutz, "The Political Effects of Perceptions of Mass Opinion," Research in Micropolitics 4 (1994): 143-67.
    • (1995) European Journal of Social Psychology , vol.25 , pp. 195-215
    • Duck, J.M.1    Hogg, M.A.2    Terry, D.J.3
  • 10
    • 84972949283 scopus 로고
    • The perceived influence of AIDS advertising: Third-person effects in the context of positive media content
    • Numerous explanations have been offered to explain this effect. See Hans-Bernd Brosius and Dirk Engel, "The Causes of Third-Person Effects: Unrealistic Optimism, Impersonal Impact, or Generalized Negative Attitudes Towards Media Influence," International Journal of Public Opinion Research 8 (summer 1996): 142-62; Julie M. Duck, Michael A. Hogg, and Deborah J. Terry, "Me, Us and Them: Political Identification and the Third-Person Effect in the 1993 Australian Federal Election," European Journal of Social Psychology 25 (March/April 1995): 195-215; Julie M. Duck, Deborah J. Terry, and Michael A. Hogg, "The Perceived Influence of AIDS Advertising: Third-Person Effects in the Context of Positive Media Content," Basic and Applied Social Psychology 17 (1995): 305-325; Carroll J. Glynn, Ronald E. Ostman, and Daniel G. McDonald, "Opinions, Perception, and Social Reality," in Public Opinion and the Communication of Consent, ed. Glasser and Salmon, 249-77; Albert C. Gunther, "What We Think Others Think: Cause and Consequence in the Third Person Effect," Communication Research 18 (June 1991): 355-72; Diana C. Mutz, "The Political Effects of Perceptions of Mass Opinion," Research in Micropolitics 4 (1994): 143-67.
    • (1995) Basic and Applied Social Psychology , vol.17 , pp. 305-325
    • Duck, J.M.1    Terry, D.J.2    Hogg, M.A.3
  • 11
    • 0030304251 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Opinions, perception, and social reality
    • ed. Glasser and Salmon
    • Numerous explanations have been offered to explain this effect. See Hans-Bernd Brosius and Dirk Engel, "The Causes of Third-Person Effects: Unrealistic Optimism, Impersonal Impact, or Generalized Negative Attitudes Towards Media Influence," International Journal of Public Opinion Research 8 (summer 1996): 142-62; Julie M. Duck, Michael A. Hogg, and Deborah J. Terry, "Me, Us and Them: Political Identification and the Third-Person Effect in the 1993 Australian Federal Election," European Journal of Social Psychology 25 (March/April 1995): 195-215; Julie M. Duck, Deborah J. Terry, and Michael A. Hogg, "The Perceived Influence of AIDS Advertising: Third-Person Effects in the Context of Positive Media Content," Basic and Applied Social Psychology 17 (1995): 305-325; Carroll J. Glynn, Ronald E. Ostman, and Daniel G. McDonald, "Opinions, Perception, and Social Reality," in Public Opinion and the Communication of Consent, ed. Glasser and Salmon, 249-77; Albert C. Gunther, "What We Think Others Think: Cause and Consequence in the Third Person Effect," Communication Research 18 (June 1991): 355-72; Diana C. Mutz, "The Political Effects of Perceptions of Mass Opinion," Research in Micropolitics 4 (1994): 143-67.
    • Public Opinion and the Communication of Consent , pp. 249-277
    • Glynn, C.J.1    Ostman, R.E.2    McDonald, D.G.3
  • 12
    • 84965558905 scopus 로고
    • What we think others think: Cause and consequence in the third person effect
    • June
    • Numerous explanations have been offered to explain this effect. See Hans-Bernd Brosius and Dirk Engel, "The Causes of Third-Person Effects: Unrealistic Optimism, Impersonal Impact, or Generalized Negative Attitudes Towards Media Influence," International Journal of Public Opinion Research 8 (summer 1996): 142-62; Julie M. Duck, Michael A. Hogg, and Deborah J. Terry, "Me, Us and Them: Political Identification and the Third-Person Effect in the 1993 Australian Federal Election," European Journal of Social Psychology 25 (March/April 1995): 195-215; Julie M. Duck, Deborah J. Terry, and Michael A. Hogg, "The Perceived Influence of AIDS Advertising: Third-Person Effects in the Context of Positive Media Content," Basic and Applied Social Psychology 17 (1995): 305-325; Carroll J. Glynn, Ronald E. Ostman, and Daniel G. McDonald, "Opinions, Perception, and Social Reality," in Public Opinion and the Communication of Consent, ed. Glasser and Salmon, 249-77; Albert C. Gunther, "What We Think Others Think: Cause and Consequence in the Third Person Effect," Communication Research 18 (June 1991): 355-72; Diana C. Mutz, "The Political Effects of Perceptions of Mass Opinion," Research in Micropolitics 4 (1994): 143-67.
    • (1991) Communication Research , vol.18 , pp. 355-372
    • Gunther, A.C.1
  • 13
    • 0030304251 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The political effects of perceptions of mass opinion
    • Numerous explanations have been offered to explain this effect. See Hans-Bernd Brosius and Dirk Engel, "The Causes of Third-Person Effects: Unrealistic Optimism, Impersonal Impact, or Generalized Negative Attitudes Towards Media Influence," International Journal of Public Opinion Research 8 (summer 1996): 142-62; Julie M. Duck, Michael A. Hogg, and Deborah J. Terry, "Me, Us and Them: Political Identification and the Third-Person Effect in the 1993 Australian Federal Election," European Journal of Social Psychology 25 (March/April 1995): 195-215; Julie M. Duck, Deborah J. Terry, and Michael A. Hogg, "The Perceived Influence of AIDS Advertising: Third-Person Effects in the Context of Positive Media Content," Basic and Applied Social Psychology 17 (1995): 305-325; Carroll J. Glynn, Ronald E. Ostman, and Daniel G. McDonald, "Opinions, Perception, and Social Reality," in Public Opinion and the Communication of Consent, ed. Glasser and Salmon, 249-77; Albert C. Gunther, "What We Think Others Think: Cause and Consequence in the Third Person Effect," Communication Research 18 (June 1991): 355-72; Diana C. Mutz, "The Political Effects of Perceptions of Mass Opinion," Research in Micropolitics 4 (1994): 143-67.
    • (1994) Research in Micropolitics , vol.4 , pp. 143-167
    • Mutz, D.C.1
  • 14
    • 85033074685 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Simpson's exwife and her friend were murdered 12 June 1994. Five days later, the exfootball star and film and television celebrity was discovered being driven down a Los Angeles freeway in a white Ford Bronco in what was widely regarded as an effort to avoid arrest. Subsequent police evidence, including blood samples in Simpson's apartment and truck, also pointed to Simpson's guilt. During the televised trial, Simpson's defense countered the prosecution's case by pointing to sloppy police work and mishandling of evidence. On 3 October, the jury delivered not guilty verdicts in both murder counts.
  • 15
    • 33751573913 scopus 로고
    • The influence of perceptions of media influence: Third-person effects and the public expression of opinions
    • spring
    • Diana C. Mutz, "The Influence of Perceptions of Media Influence: Third-Person Effects and the Public Expression of Opinions," International Journal of Public Opinion Research 1 (spring 1989): 3-23.
    • (1989) International Journal of Public Opinion Research , vol.1 , pp. 3-23
    • Mutz, D.C.1
  • 16
    • 0031515498 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Support for censorship of violent and misogynic rap lyrics: An analysis of the third-person effect
    • April
    • Douglas M. McLeod, William P. Eveland Jr., and Amy I. Nathanson, "Support for Censorship of Violent and Misogynic Rap Lyrics: An Analysis of the Third-Person Effect," Communication Research 24 (April 1997): 153-74.
    • (1997) Communication Research , vol.24 , pp. 153-174
    • McLeod, D.M.1    Eveland W.P., Jr.2    Nathanson, A.I.3
  • 17
    • 0002853701 scopus 로고
    • Discrepancy between perceived first-person and perceived third-person mass media effects
    • spring/summer
    • James T. Tiedge, Arthur Silverblatt, Michael J. Havice, and Richard Rosenfeld, "Discrepancy Between Perceived First-Person and Perceived Third-Person Mass Media Effects," Journalism Quarterly 68 (spring/summer 1991): 141-54.
    • (1991) Journalism Quarterly , vol.68 , pp. 141-154
    • Tiedge, J.T.1    Silverblatt, A.2    Havice, M.J.3    Rosenfeld, R.4
  • 18
    • 84937306863 scopus 로고
    • Direct and indirect effects of socioeconomic status on public affairs knowledge
    • summer
    • Douglas M. McLeod and Elizabeth M. Perse, "Direct and Indirect Effects of Socioeconomic Status on Public Affairs Knowledge," Journalism Quarterly 71 (summer 1994): 433-42; Michael B. Salwen and Paul D. Driscoll, "Feeling Informed? The 'Assurance Function' of the Mass Media," International Journal of Public Opinion Research 7 (fall 1995): 270-75; Serena Wade and Wilbur Schramm, "The Mass Media as Sources of Public Affairs, Science, and Health Knowledge," Public Opinion Quarterly 33 (summer 1969): 197-209.
    • (1994) Journalism Quarterly , vol.71 , pp. 433-442
    • McLeod, D.M.1    Perse, E.M.2
  • 19
    • 0007209453 scopus 로고
    • Feeling informed? The 'assurance function' of the mass media
    • fall
    • Douglas M. McLeod and Elizabeth M. Perse, "Direct and Indirect Effects of Socioeconomic Status on Public Affairs Knowledge," Journalism Quarterly 71 (summer 1994): 433-42; Michael B. Salwen and Paul D. Driscoll, "Feeling Informed? The 'Assurance Function' of the Mass Media," International Journal of Public Opinion Research 7 (fall 1995): 270-75; Serena Wade and Wilbur Schramm, "The Mass Media as Sources of Public Affairs, Science, and Health Knowledge," Public Opinion Quarterly 33 (summer 1969): 197-209.
    • (1995) International Journal of Public Opinion Research , vol.7 , pp. 270-275
    • Salwen, M.B.1    Driscoll, P.D.2
  • 20
    • 0038431448 scopus 로고
    • The mass media as sources of public affairs, science, and health knowledge
    • summer
    • Douglas M. McLeod and Elizabeth M. Perse, "Direct and Indirect Effects of Socioeconomic Status on Public Affairs Knowledge," Journalism Quarterly 71 (summer 1994): 433-42; Michael B. Salwen and Paul D. Driscoll, "Feeling Informed? The 'Assurance Function' of the Mass Media," International Journal of Public Opinion Research 7 (fall 1995): 270-75; Serena Wade and Wilbur Schramm, "The Mass Media as Sources of Public Affairs, Science, and Health Knowledge," Public Opinion Quarterly 33 (summer 1969): 197-209.
    • (1969) Public Opinion Quarterly , vol.33 , pp. 197-209
    • Wade, S.1    Schramm, W.2
  • 23
    • 84928507632 scopus 로고
    • Public opinion about public opinion
    • summer
    • Carroll J. Glynn and Ronald E. Ostman, "Public Opinion About Public Opinion," Journalism Quarterly 65 (summer 1988): 680-88; Albert C. Gunther, "Overrating the X-Rating: The Third-Person Perception and Support for Censorship of Pornography," Journal of Communication 45 (winter 1995): 27-38; Yu-Wei Hu and Yi-Chen Wu, "Testing a Theoretical Model on the Third-Person Effect: Perceived Impact of Election Polls" (paper presented at the annual meeting of AEJMC, Anaheim, CA, August 1996); Dominic L. Lasorsa, "Real and Perceived Effects of 'Amerika'," Journalism Quarterly 66 (summer 1989): 373-78, 529; Dianne Rucinski and Charles T. Salmon, "The 'Other' as the Vulnerable Voter: A Study of the Third-Person Effect in the 1988 U.S. Presidential Campaign," International Journal of Public Opinion Research 2 (winter 1990): 345-68; Tiedge, Silverblatt, Havice, and Rosenfeld, "Discrepancy Between Perceived First-Person and Perceived Third-Person Mass Media Effects."
    • (1988) Journalism Quarterly , vol.65 , pp. 680-688
    • Glynn, C.J.1    Ostman, R.E.2
  • 24
    • 84985135595 scopus 로고
    • Overrating the x-rating: The third-person perception and support for censorship of pornography
    • winter
    • Carroll J. Glynn and Ronald E. Ostman, "Public Opinion About Public Opinion," Journalism Quarterly 65 (summer 1988): 680-88; Albert C. Gunther, "Overrating the X-Rating: The Third-Person Perception and Support for Censorship of Pornography," Journal of Communication 45 (winter 1995): 27-38; Yu-Wei Hu and Yi-Chen Wu, "Testing a Theoretical Model on the Third-Person Effect: Perceived Impact of Election Polls" (paper presented at the annual meeting of AEJMC, Anaheim, CA, August 1996); Dominic L. Lasorsa, "Real and Perceived Effects of 'Amerika'," Journalism Quarterly 66 (summer 1989): 373-78, 529; Dianne Rucinski and Charles T. Salmon, "The 'Other' as the Vulnerable Voter: A Study of the Third-Person Effect in the 1988 U.S. Presidential Campaign," International Journal of Public Opinion Research 2 (winter 1990): 345-68; Tiedge, Silverblatt, Havice, and Rosenfeld, "Discrepancy Between Perceived First-Person and Perceived Third-Person Mass Media Effects."
    • (1995) Journal of Communication , vol.45 , pp. 27-38
    • Gunther, A.C.1
  • 25
    • 0039943244 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Testing a theoretical model on the third-person effect: Perceived impact of election polls
    • Anaheim, CA, August
    • Carroll J. Glynn and Ronald E. Ostman, "Public Opinion About Public Opinion," Journalism Quarterly 65 (summer 1988): 680-88; Albert C. Gunther, "Overrating the X-Rating: The Third-Person Perception and Support for Censorship of Pornography," Journal of Communication 45 (winter 1995): 27-38; Yu-Wei Hu and Yi-Chen Wu, "Testing a Theoretical Model on the Third-Person Effect: Perceived Impact of Election Polls" (paper presented at the annual meeting of AEJMC, Anaheim, CA, August 1996); Dominic L. Lasorsa, "Real and Perceived Effects of 'Amerika'," Journalism Quarterly 66 (summer 1989): 373-78, 529; Dianne Rucinski and Charles T. Salmon, "The 'Other' as the Vulnerable Voter: A Study of the Third-Person Effect in the 1988 U.S. Presidential Campaign," International Journal of Public Opinion Research 2 (winter 1990): 345-68; Tiedge, Silverblatt, Havice, and Rosenfeld, "Discrepancy Between Perceived First-Person and Perceived Third-Person Mass Media Effects."
    • (1996) Annual Meeting of AEJMC
    • Hu, Y.-W.1    Wu, Y.-C.2
  • 26
    • 0001741659 scopus 로고
    • Real and perceived effects of 'Amerika'
    • summer
    • Carroll J. Glynn and Ronald E. Ostman, "Public Opinion About Public Opinion," Journalism Quarterly 65 (summer 1988): 680-88; Albert C. Gunther, "Overrating the X-Rating: The Third-Person Perception and Support for Censorship of Pornography," Journal of Communication 45 (winter 1995): 27-38; Yu-Wei Hu and Yi-Chen Wu, "Testing a Theoretical Model on the Third-Person Effect: Perceived Impact of Election Polls" (paper presented at the annual meeting of AEJMC, Anaheim, CA, August 1996); Dominic L. Lasorsa, "Real and Perceived Effects of 'Amerika'," Journalism Quarterly 66 (summer 1989): 373-78, 529; Dianne Rucinski and Charles T. Salmon, "The 'Other' as the Vulnerable Voter: A Study of the Third-Person Effect in the 1988 U.S. Presidential Campaign," International Journal of Public Opinion Research 2 (winter 1990): 345-68; Tiedge, Silverblatt, Havice, and Rosenfeld, "Discrepancy Between Perceived First-Person and Perceived Third-Person Mass Media Effects."
    • (1989) Journalism Quarterly , vol.66 , pp. 373-378
    • Lasorsa, D.L.1
  • 27
    • 0000490653 scopus 로고
    • The 'other' as the vulnerable voter: A study of the third-person effect in the 1988 U.S. presidential campaign
    • winter
    • Carroll J. Glynn and Ronald E. Ostman, "Public Opinion About Public Opinion," Journalism Quarterly 65 (summer 1988): 680-88; Albert C. Gunther, "Overrating the X-Rating: The Third-Person Perception and Support for Censorship of Pornography," Journal of Communication 45 (winter 1995): 27-38; Yu-Wei Hu and Yi-Chen Wu, "Testing a Theoretical Model on the Third-Person Effect: Perceived Impact of Election Polls" (paper presented at the annual meeting of AEJMC, Anaheim, CA, August 1996); Dominic L. Lasorsa, "Real and Perceived Effects of 'Amerika'," Journalism Quarterly 66 (summer 1989): 373-78, 529; Dianne Rucinski and Charles T. Salmon, "The 'Other' as the Vulnerable Voter: A Study of the Third-Person Effect in the 1988 U.S. Presidential Campaign," International Journal of Public Opinion Research 2 (winter 1990): 345-68; Tiedge, Silverblatt, Havice, and Rosenfeld, "Discrepancy Between Perceived First-Person and Perceived Third-Person Mass Media Effects."
    • (1990) International Journal of Public Opinion Research , vol.2 , pp. 345-368
    • Rucinski, D.1    Salmon, C.T.2
  • 28
    • 85033083799 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Carroll J. Glynn and Ronald E. Ostman, "Public Opinion About Public Opinion," Journalism Quarterly 65 (summer 1988): 680-88; Albert C. Gunther, "Overrating the X-Rating: The Third-Person Perception and Support for Censorship of Pornography," Journal of Communication 45 (winter 1995): 27-38; Yu-Wei Hu and Yi-Chen Wu, "Testing a Theoretical Model on the Third-Person Effect: Perceived Impact of Election Polls" (paper presented at the annual meeting of AEJMC, Anaheim, CA, August 1996); Dominic L. Lasorsa, "Real and Perceived Effects of 'Amerika'," Journalism Quarterly 66 (summer 1989): 373-78, 529; Dianne Rucinski and Charles T. Salmon, "The 'Other' as the Vulnerable Voter: A Study of the Third-Person Effect in the 1988 U.S. Presidential Campaign," International Journal of Public Opinion Research 2 (winter 1990): 345-68; Tiedge, Silverblatt, Havice, and Rosenfeld, "Discrepancy Between Perceived First-Person and Perceived Third-Person Mass Media Effects."
    • Discrepancy Between Perceived First-Person and Perceived Third-Person Mass Media Effects
    • Tiedge1    Silverblatt2    Havice3    Rosenfeld4
  • 29
    • 0030158647 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The roles of question order, contrast, and knowledge in the third-person effect
    • summer
    • Vincent Price and David Tewksbury, "The Roles of Question Order, Contrast, and Knowledge in the Third-Person Effect," International Journal of Public Opinion Research 8 (summer 1996): 120-41.
    • (1996) International Journal of Public Opinion Research , vol.8 , pp. 120-141
    • Price, V.1    Tewksbury, D.2
  • 31
    • 0000118132 scopus 로고
    • Illusions of media power: The third-person effect
    • summer
    • L. Erwin Atwood, "Illusions of Media Power: The Third-Person Effect," Journalism Quarterly 71 (summer 1994): 269-81.
    • (1994) Journalism Quarterly , vol.71 , pp. 269-281
    • Atwood, L.E.1
  • 34
    • 85033089379 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • The terminology in the literature is inconsistent. The term perceptual bias or perceptual discrepancy refers to the absolute difference between beliefs about media effects on oneself and estimations of media effects on others, although the term is sometimes assumed to refer to the third-person direction of greater effects on perceived others than on oneself.
  • 38
    • 84986409103 scopus 로고
    • Measurement of effects of attention to news media
    • This may be because empirical findings indicate low to moderate associations between news media use and public affairs knowledge, as well as inconsistent findings across media. fall
    • This may be because empirical findings indicate low to moderate associations between news media use and public affairs knowledge, as well as inconsistent findings across media. Steven H. Chaffee and Joan Schleuder, "Measurement of Effects of Attention to News Media," Human Communication Research 13 (fall 1986): 76-107; Hugh M. Culbertson and Guido H. Stempel III, "How Media Use and Reliance Affect Knowledge Level," Communication Research 13 (October 1986): 579-602; Jack M. McLeod and D. McDonald, "Beyond Simple Exposure: Media Orientations and Their Impact on Political Processes," Communication Research 12 (January 1985): 3-34; John E. Newhagen, "Self-Efficacy, Media Use as Predictors of Current-Events Knowledge," Journalism Educator 49 (autumn 1994): 27-32.
    • (1986) Human Communication Research , vol.13 , pp. 76-107
    • Chaffee, S.H.1    Schleuder, J.2
  • 39
    • 84976933377 scopus 로고
    • How media use and reliance affect knowledge level
    • October
    • This may be because empirical findings indicate low to moderate associations between news media use and public affairs knowledge, as well as inconsistent findings across media. Steven H. Chaffee and Joan Schleuder, "Measurement of Effects of Attention to News Media," Human Communication Research 13 (fall 1986): 76-107; Hugh M. Culbertson and Guido H. Stempel III, "How Media Use and Reliance Affect Knowledge Level," Communication Research 13 (October 1986): 579-602; Jack M. McLeod and D. McDonald, "Beyond Simple Exposure: Media Orientations and Their Impact on Political Processes," Communication Research 12 (January 1985): 3-34; John E. Newhagen, "Self-Efficacy, Media Use as Predictors of Current-Events Knowledge," Journalism Educator 49 (autumn 1994): 27-32.
    • (1986) Communication Research , vol.13 , pp. 579-602
    • Culbertson, H.M.1    Stempel G.H. III2
  • 40
    • 84976985168 scopus 로고
    • Beyond simple exposure: Media orientations and their impact on political processes
    • January
    • This may be because empirical findings indicate low to moderate associations between news media use and public affairs knowledge, as well as inconsistent findings across media. Steven H. Chaffee and Joan Schleuder, "Measurement of Effects of Attention to News Media," Human Communication Research 13 (fall 1986): 76-107; Hugh M. Culbertson and Guido H. Stempel III, "How Media Use and Reliance Affect Knowledge Level," Communication Research 13 (October 1986): 579-602; Jack M. McLeod and D. McDonald, "Beyond Simple Exposure: Media Orientations and Their Impact on Political Processes," Communication Research 12 (January 1985): 3-34; John E. Newhagen, "Self-Efficacy, Media Use as Predictors of Current-Events Knowledge," Journalism Educator 49 (autumn 1994): 27-32.
    • (1985) Communication Research , vol.12 , pp. 3-34
    • McLeod, J.M.1    McDonald, D.2
  • 41
    • 0039223456 scopus 로고
    • Self-efficacy, media use as predictors of current-events knowledge
    • autumn
    • This may be because empirical findings indicate low to moderate associations between news media use and public affairs knowledge, as well as inconsistent findings across media. Steven H. Chaffee and Joan Schleuder, "Measurement of Effects of Attention to News Media," Human Communication Research 13 (fall 1986): 76-107; Hugh M. Culbertson and Guido H. Stempel III, "How Media Use and Reliance Affect Knowledge Level," Communication Research 13 (October 1986): 579-602; Jack M. McLeod and D. McDonald, "Beyond Simple Exposure: Media Orientations and Their Impact on Political Processes," Communication Research 12 (January 1985): 3-34; John E. Newhagen, "Self-Efficacy, Media Use as Predictors of Current-Events Knowledge," Journalism Educator 49 (autumn 1994): 27-32.
    • (1994) Journalism Educator , vol.49 , pp. 27-32
    • Newhagen, J.E.1
  • 42
    • 0039549049 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Censorship of political advertising: A third-person effect
    • Chicago, August
    • Dhavan V. Shah, Ronald J. Faber, Seounmi Hanyoun, and Hernando Rojas, "Censorship of Political Advertising: A Third-Person Effect" (paper presented at the annual meeting of AEJMC, Chicago, August 1997).
    • (1997) Annual Meeting of AEJMC
    • Shah, D.V.1    Faber, R.J.2    Hanyoun, S.3    Rojas, H.4
  • 43
    • 84985759085 scopus 로고
    • The public's view of the impact of the mass media: A test of the 'third person' effect
    • October/November
    • J. M. Innes and H. Zeitz, "The Public's View of the Impact of the Mass Media: A Test of the 'Third Person' Effect," European Journal of Social Psychology 18 (October/November 1988): 457-63.
    • (1988) European Journal of Social Psychology , vol.18 , pp. 457-463
    • Innes, J.M.1    Zeitz, H.2
  • 44
    • 84970746890 scopus 로고
    • Biased optimism and the third-person effect
    • spring
    • Albert C. Gunther and Paul Mundy, "Biased Optimism and the Third-Person Effect," Journalism Quarterly 70 (spring 1993): 58-67; Albert C. Gunther and Esther Thorson, "Perceived Persuasive Effects of Product Commercials and Public Service Announcements: Third Person Effects in New Domains," Communication Research 19 (October 1992): 574-596; Thomas Ashby Wills, "Downward Comparison Principles in Social Psychology," Psychological Bulletin 90 (September 1981): 245-71.
    • (1993) Journalism Quarterly , vol.70 , pp. 58-67
    • Gunther, A.C.1    Mundy, P.2
  • 45
    • 84965673612 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Perceived persuasive effects of product commercials and public service announcements: Third person effects in new domains
    • October
    • Albert C. Gunther and Paul Mundy, "Biased Optimism and the Third-Person Effect," Journalism Quarterly 70 (spring 1993): 58-67; Albert C. Gunther and Esther Thorson, "Perceived Persuasive Effects of Product Commercials and Public Service Announcements: Third Person Effects in New Domains," Communication Research 19 (October 1992): 574-596; Thomas Ashby Wills, "Downward Comparison Principles in Social Psychology," Psychological Bulletin 90 (September 1981): 245-71.
    • (1992) Communication Research , vol.19 , pp. 574-596
    • Gunther, A.C.1    Thorson, E.2
  • 46
    • 0000531398 scopus 로고
    • Downward comparison principles in social psychology
    • September
    • Albert C. Gunther and Paul Mundy, "Biased Optimism and the Third-Person Effect," Journalism Quarterly 70 (spring 1993): 58-67; Albert C. Gunther and Esther Thorson, "Perceived Persuasive Effects of Product Commercials and Public Service Announcements: Third Person Effects in New Domains," Communication Research 19 (October 1992): 574-596; Thomas Ashby Wills, "Downward Comparison Principles in Social Psychology," Psychological Bulletin 90 (September 1981): 245-71.
    • (1981) Psychological Bulletin , vol.90 , pp. 245-271
    • Wills, T.A.1
  • 48
    • 63749117144 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The perceived impact of the mass media: Reconsidering the third person effect
    • January-February
    • Julie M. Duck and Barbara-Ann Mullin, "The Perceived Impact of the Mass Media: Reconsidering the Third Person Effect," European Journal of Social Psychology 25 (January-February 1995): 77-93; Gunther and Thorson, "Perceived Persuasive Effects of Product Commercials and Public Service Announcements."
    • (1995) European Journal of Social Psychology , vol.25 , pp. 77-93
    • Duck, J.M.1    Mullin, B.-A.2
  • 51
    • 0001173474 scopus 로고
    • Third-person effects and the differential impact in negative political advertising
    • winter
    • Jeremy Cohen and Robert G. Davis, "Third-Person Effects and the Differential Impact in Negative Political Advertising," Journalism Quarterly 68 (winter 1991): 680-88. For related research see also Gunther and Mundy, "Biased Optimism and the Third-Person Effect"; Gunther and Thorson, "Perceived Persuasive Effects of Product Commercials and Public Service Announcements"; Ekaterina Ognianova, Robert Meeds, Esther Thorson, and James Coyle, "Political Adwatches and the Third-Person Effect" (paper presented at the annual meeting of AEJMC, Anaheim, CA, August 1996).
    • (1991) Journalism Quarterly , vol.68 , pp. 680-688
    • Cohen, J.1    Davis, R.G.2
  • 52
    • 0039817399 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Jeremy Cohen and Robert G. Davis, "Third-Person Effects and the Differential Impact in Negative Political Advertising," Journalism Quarterly 68 (winter 1991): 680-88. For related research see also Gunther and Mundy, "Biased Optimism and the Third-Person Effect"; Gunther and Thorson, "Perceived Persuasive Effects of Product Commercials and Public Service Announcements"; Ekaterina Ognianova, Robert Meeds, Esther Thorson, and James Coyle, "Political Adwatches and the Third-Person Effect" (paper presented at the annual meeting of AEJMC, Anaheim, CA, August 1996).
    • Biased Optimism and the Third-Person Effect
    • Gunther1    Mundy2
  • 53
    • 85033085536 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Jeremy Cohen and Robert G. Davis, "Third-Person Effects and the Differential Impact in Negative Political Advertising," Journalism Quarterly 68 (winter 1991): 680-88. For related research see also Gunther and Mundy, "Biased Optimism and the Third-Person Effect"; Gunther and Thorson, "Perceived Persuasive Effects of Product Commercials and Public Service Announcements"; Ekaterina Ognianova, Robert Meeds, Esther Thorson, and James Coyle, "Political Adwatches and the Third-Person Effect" (paper presented at the annual meeting of AEJMC, Anaheim, CA, August 1996).
    • Perceived Persuasive Effects of Product Commercials and Public Service Announcements
    • Gunther1    Thorson2
  • 54
    • 0041130083 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Political adwatches and the third-person effect
    • Anaheim, CA, August
    • Jeremy Cohen and Robert G. Davis, "Third-Person Effects and the Differential Impact in Negative Political Advertising," Journalism Quarterly 68 (winter 1991): 680-88. For related research see also Gunther and Mundy, "Biased Optimism and the Third-Person Effect"; Gunther and Thorson, "Perceived Persuasive Effects of Product Commercials and Public Service Announcements"; Ekaterina Ognianova, Robert Meeds, Esther Thorson, and James Coyle, "Political Adwatches and the Third-Person Effect" (paper presented at the annual meeting of AEJMC, Anaheim, CA, August 1996).
    • (1996) Annual Meeting of AEJMC
    • Ognianova, E.1    Meeds, R.2    Thorson, E.3    Coyle, J.4
  • 55
    • 0039225526 scopus 로고
    • The influence of O.J. Simpson's pre-trial publicity on public opinion and the public agenda
    • Albuquerque, NM, May
    • William J. Brown, Benson P. Fraser, and Mihai C. Bocarnea, "The Influence of O.J. Simpson's Pre-Trial Publicity on Public Opinion and the Public Agenda" (paper presented at the annual meeting of the International Communication Association, Albuquerque, NM, May 1995).
    • (1995) Annual Meeting of the International Communication Association
    • Brown, W.J.1    Fraser, B.P.2    Bocarnea, M.C.3
  • 56
    • 85033083444 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The hypotheses were stated in correlational terms. The research questions are stated in predictive terms. The relative importance of combinations of variables will be tested through regression analyses
    • The hypotheses were stated in correlational terms. The research questions are stated in predictive terms. The relative importance of combinations of variables will be tested through regression analyses.
  • 57
    • 85033090331 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • The sampling frame consisted of telephone directories from the forty-eight continental states and the District of Columbia. The sample was stratified proportionate to each state's population. The unit of analysis was the page, with pages randomly selected from the directories' residential sections. Adjustments were made in directory listings from highly populated areas because of the smaller type size with more listings on each page. After a page was selected, a column was randomly selected, followed by a randomly selected line down the column. The last digit of the number was increased by one so that respondents with unlisted numbers could be contacted. Since females are disproportionately likely to answer the telephone in surveys, about one-fourth of the callers requested to speak with the adult male in the household with the nearest birthday when a female answered the telephone.
  • 58
    • 85033085667 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • This was the ratio of completed calls to known valid contacts and outright refusals. The ratio of completed calls to all outgoing calls, including telephone machines that could not be reached after repeated attempts, invalid respondents, surveys that could not be completed, and the like, was about 36%
    • This was the ratio of completed calls to known valid contacts and outright refusals. The ratio of completed calls to all outgoing calls, including telephone machines that could not be reached after repeated attempts, invalid respondents, surveys that could not be completed, and the like, was about 36%.
  • 59
    • 85033083217 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Admittedly the use of single pairs of items measuring the third-person effect, unfortunately common in the literature, is problematic. As Price and Tewksbury observed, "all research to date bearing on the third-person effect has examined the phenomenon by posing to respondents a series of parallel questions about perceived media effects," "The Roles of Question Order, Contrast, and Knowledge in the Third-Person Effect," 124.
  • 60
    • 85033075740 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The questions involving media coverage of the Bronco chase and police mishandling of evidence were recoded so that for all questions, the higher the score the lower the estimate of media influence
    • The questions involving media coverage of the Bronco chase and police mishandling of evidence were recoded so that for all questions, the higher the score the lower the estimate of media influence.
  • 61
    • 84965673612 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • This is also referred to in the literature as the "reverse third-person effect."
    • This is also referred to in the literature as the "reverse third-person effect." Cohen and Davis, "Third-Person Effects and the Differential Impact in Negative Political Advertising"; Albert C. Gunther and Esther Thorson, "Perceived Persuasive Effects of Commercials and Public Service Announcements," Communication Research 19 (October 1992): 574-96.
    • Third-Person Effects and the Differential Impact in Negative Political Advertising
    • Cohen1    Davis2
  • 62
    • 84965673612 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Perceived persuasive effects of commercials and public service announcements
    • October
    • This is also referred to in the literature as the "reverse third-person effect." Cohen and Davis, "Third-Person Effects and the Differential Impact in Negative Political Advertising"; Albert C. Gunther and Esther Thorson, "Perceived Persuasive Effects of Commercials and Public Service Announcements," Communication Research 19 (October 1992): 574-96.
    • (1992) Communication Research , vol.19 , pp. 574-596
    • Gunther, A.C.1    Thorson, E.2
  • 63
    • 85033082861 scopus 로고
    • Descriptive statistics on the overall sample were generated and a series of stepwise multivariate linear regression models were run on the entire sample and the third-person effect "subsample." Data for each variable measure were examined for linearity and equality of variance using standardized residual scatterplots. Normal probability plots and detrended normal plots were generated to check for the normality assumption Chicago: SPSS, Inc.
    • Descriptive statistics on the overall sample were generated and a series of stepwise multivariate linear regression models were run on the entire sample and the third-person effect "subsample." Data for each variable measure were examined for linearity and equality of variance using standardized residual scatterplots. Normal probability plots and detrended normal plots were generated to check for the normality assumption. Maria J. Norusis, ed., SPSS Introductory Statistics Student Guide (Chicago: SPSS, Inc., 1990), 100.
    • (1990) SPSS Introductory Statistics Student Guide , pp. 100
    • Norusis, M.J.1
  • 64
    • 85033093917 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Too often researchers ask the race question as African American, White, Hispanic, or other. This categorization is problematic for distinguishing White Hispanics and Black Hispanics. Gainseville: University Press of Florida, See especially Chapter 2, "The Problems with Hispanic Research," After determining that Hispanic ethnicity was not a correlate of third-person effect on any of the three messages, White Hispanics were classified as Whites and Black Hispanics as Blacks
    • Too often researchers ask the race question as African American, White, Hispanic, or other. This categorization is problematic for distinguishing White Hispanics and Black Hispanics. Gonzalo Soruco, Cubans and the Mass Media in South Florida (Gainseville: University Press of Florida, 1996). See especially Chapter 2, "The Problems with Hispanic Research," 17-33. After determining that Hispanic ethnicity was not a correlate of third-person effect on any of the three messages, White Hispanics were classified as Whites and Black Hispanics as Blacks.
    • (1996) Cubans and the Mass Media in South Florida , pp. 17-33
    • Soruco, G.1
  • 65
    • 85033094103 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The distribution was: definitely not guilty (Whites 3.0%, African Americans 18.4%); probably not guilty (Whites 7.8%, African Americans 26.3%); don't really know (Whites 34.1%, African Americans 43.4%), probably guilty (Whites 34.7%, African Americans 6.6%); definitely guilty (Whites 20.4%, African Americans 5.3%)
    • The distribution was: definitely not guilty (Whites 3.0%, African Americans 18.4%); probably not guilty (Whites 7.8%, African Americans 26.3%); don't really know (Whites 34.1%, African Americans 43.4%), probably guilty (Whites 34.7%, African Americans 6.6%); definitely guilty (Whites 20.4%, African Americans 5.3%).
  • 66
    • 85033086051 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • This form of subgroup analysis was suggested by Gunther, "Overrating the X-Rating"
    • This form of subgroup analysis was suggested by Gunther, "Overrating the X-Rating."
  • 67
    • 85033096671 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • note
    • Because of the role perceptions of guilt were hypothesized to play, first-order partial correlations between the knowledge measures and the third-person perception measures were calculated controlling for perceptions of guilt. Only two notable changes occurred: (1) the relationship between current events knowledge and the Bronco measure became significant (r=.09, p=.02), and (2) the relationship between current events knowledge and the police measure slipped out of the significance range (r=.07, p=.06). On balance, support for H1 does not change. All other reported correlations of the knowledge items with third-person perception measures were also tested for the entire sample and the third-person effect subsamples. There were no notable changes in either the value or direction of the coefficients or their probability levels.
  • 68
    • 85033077876 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • Because of the nature of the Simpson trial, the race variable was dummy coded for these comparisons as either 0 (Whites, n=505) or 1 (African Americans, n=76). Those reporting other racial categories and Blacks were not included (n=19)
    • Because of the nature of the Simpson trial, the race variable was dummy coded for these comparisons as either 0 (Whites, n=505) or 1 (African Americans, n=76). Those reporting other racial categories and Blacks were not included (n=19).
  • 69
    • 0003566168 scopus 로고    scopus 로고
    • The stepwise procedure was chosen in an attempt to isolate a subset of available predictor variables yielding an optimal prediction equation with as few terms as possible. While forced entry procedures do have some advantages, there is no definite causal ordering among the variables or a clear rationale for ordered step-down significance tests of components
    • The stepwise procedure was chosen in an attempt to isolate a subset of available predictor variables yielding an optimal prediction equation with as few terms as possible. While forced entry procedures do have some advantages, there is no definite causal ordering among the variables or a clear rationale for ordered step-down significance tests of components; Norusis, ed., SPSS Introductory Statistics Student Guide, 273; Norman H. Nie, C. Hadlai Hull, Jean G. Jenkins, Karin Steinbrenner, and Dale H. Bent, SPSS Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (NY: McGraw-Hill, 1975), 344-45.
    • SPSS Introductory Statistics Student Guide , pp. 273
    • Norusis1
  • 70
    • 0003426070 scopus 로고
    • NY: McGraw-Hill
    • The stepwise procedure was chosen in an attempt to isolate a subset of available predictor variables yielding an optimal prediction equation with as few terms as possible. While forced entry procedures do have some advantages, there is no definite causal ordering among the variables or a clear rationale for ordered step-down significance tests of components; Norusis, ed., SPSS Introductory Statistics Student Guide, 273; Norman H. Nie, C. Hadlai Hull, Jean G. Jenkins, Karin Steinbrenner, and Dale H. Bent, SPSS Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (NY: McGraw-Hill, 1975), 344-45.
    • (1975) SPSS Statistical Package for the Social Sciences , pp. 344-345
    • Nie, N.H.1    Hull, C.H.2    Jenkins, J.G.3    Steinbrenner, K.4    Bent, D.H.5


* 이 정보는 Elsevier사의 SCOPUS DB에서 KISTI가 분석하여 추출한 것입니다.